Skip to main content
Log in

Cultural phenomena and the research enterprise: Toward a culturally anchored methodology

  • Published:
American Journal of Community Psychology

Abstract

Highlights the points at which culture intersects major phases of the research enterprise — problem formulation, population definition, concept and measurement development, research design, methodology, and data analysis — and influences and constrains what researchers deem worthy of investigation and how they interpret what they observe. These ethnocentric biases inhibit the development of a knowledge base for understanding diverse cultural communities. At each step of the research process, the need to carefully examine and expose the underlying cultural assumptions and to generate and develop alternative choices is emphasized. Guidelines are provided to encourage researchers to be aware of and deliberately make choices toward the development of a culturally anchored methodology that balances the demands for rigor and sensitivity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bachman, J. G., & O'Malley, P. M. (1984). Yes-saying, nay-saying, and going to extremes: Black-white differences in response styles.Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 491–509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauman, L. J., & Adair, E. G. (1992). The use of ethnographic interviewing to inform questionnaire construction.Health Education Quarterly, 19, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1972). An exploratory study of socialization effects on Black children: Some Black-White comparisons.Child Development, 43, 261–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, C. C., Anderson, L. S., Cooper, S., Hassol, L., Klein, D. C., & Rosenblum, G. (Eds.). (1966).Community psychology: A report of the Boston conference on the education of psychologists for community mental health. Boston: Boston University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J. W. (1989). Imposed etics-emics-derived etics: The operationalization of a compelling idea.International Journal of Psychology, 19, 335–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blalock, H. M. (1982).Conceptualization and measurement in the social sciences. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979).The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchannon, D. R. (1992). An uneasy alliance: Combining qualitative and quantitative research methods.Health Education Quarterly.19(1), 117–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chavis, D., Stucky, P. E., & Wandersman, A. (1983). Returning research to the community: A relationship between scientist and citizen.American Psychologist, 38, 424–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • D'Andrade, R. G. (1984). Cultural meaning systems. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (eds.),Cultural theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 88–122). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Vries, H., Weijts, W., Dijkstra, M., & Kok, G., (1992). The utilization of qualitative and quantitative data for health education program planning, implementation, and evaluation: A spiral approach.Health Education Quarterly, 19(1), 101–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairweather, G. W. (1967).Methods for experimental social innovation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, S. B. (1983).Ways with words: Language, life, and work communities and classrooms. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, K., & Monohan, J. (1977).Psychology and social change. Homewood, IL: Dorsey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hines, A. M. (1993). Linking qualitative and quantitative methods in cross-cultural survey research: Techniques from cognitive science.American Journal of Community Psychology, 21, 729–746.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, A., Scott, R. A. (1981). The study of minority groups in complex societies. In R. H. Munroe, R. L. Munroe, & B. B. Whiting (Eds.),Handbook of cross-cultural human development (pp. 113–152). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, D., & Dumont, K. (1993). Using focus groups to facilitate culturally anchored research.American Journal of Community Psychology, 21, 775–806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1989). Effects of culture and response format on extreme response style.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20(3), 296–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorgenson, D. J. (1989).Participant observation: A methodology for human studies, Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kameoka, V. A. (1985). Construct validation of psychological measures in cross-cultural research. Analysis of linear structural relationships. In R. Diaz Guerro (Ed.),Cross-cultural and national studies in social psychology (pp. 57–68). Amsterdam: Elsurer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, J. G. (1971). The quest for valid preventive interventions. In G. Rosenblum (Ed.),Issues in community psychology and preventive mental health, New York: Behavioral Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluckholn, C. (1949).Mirror for man. New York: Premier Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, D. H., & Shinn, M. (1992).Important interaction effects are hard to detect. Unpublished manuscript, Columbia University.

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1962).The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Laboratory of Human Cognition (1983). Culture and cognitive development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),Handbook of child psychology, 4th ed., Vol. 1 (pp. 295–356). New York. Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laboratory of Human Cognition (1983). Culture and cognitive development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),Handbook of child psychology, 4th ed., Vol. 1 (pp. 295–356). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linney, J. A., & Reppucci, N. D. (1982). Research design and methods in community psychology. In P. C. Kendall & J. N. Butcher (Eds.),Handbook of research methods in clinical psychology (pp. 535–566). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marin, G., & Marin, B. V. (1991).Research with Hispanic populations. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for emotion, cognition, and motivation.Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maruyama, M. (1983). Cross-cultural perspectives on social and community change. In E. Seidman (Ed.),Handbook of social intervention. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maruyama, M. (1980). Mindscapes and science theories.Current Anthropology, 21, 589–610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maton, K. I. (1993). A bridge between cultures: Linked ethnographic-empirical methodology for culture-anchored research. American Journal of Community Psychology, 21, 747–773.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects.Psychological Bulletin, 114, 376–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLoyd, V. C., & Randolph, S. (1985). Secular trends in the study of Afro-American children: A review of child development. In A. B. Smuts & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), History and research in child development.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(4–5 Serial No. 211).

  • O'Brien, K. O. (1993). Improving survey questionnaires through focus groups. In D. L. Morgan (Ed.),Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art (pp. 105–117). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapkin, B. D., & Luke, D. A. (1993). Cluster analysis in community research: Epistemology and practice.American Journal of Community Psychology, 21, 247–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, J. (1977).Community psychology: Values, research and action. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohner, R. P. (1984). Toward a conception of culture for cross-cultural psychology.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 111–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, W. (1971).Blaming the victim. New York: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, S. B. (1971).The culture of school and the problem of change. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schofield, J. W., & Anderson, K. (1987). Combining quantitative and qualitative components of research on ethnic identity and intergroup relations. in J. S. Phinney & M. J. Rotheram (Eds.),Children's ethnic socialization: Pluralism and development (pp. 252–273). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidman, E. (1978). Justice, values and social science: Unexamined premises. In R. J. Simon (Ed.),Research in law and sociology (pp. 175–200). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidman, E. (1983). Unexamined premises of social problem-solving. In E. Seidman (Ed.),Handbook of social intervention. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidman, E., & Rappaport, J. (1986).Redefining social problems. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serrano-Garcia, I. (1984). The illusion of empowerment: Community development within a colonial context. In J. Rappaport, C. Swift, & R. Hess (Eds.),Studies in empowerment: Steps toward understanding and action (pp. 173–200). New York: Haworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Susskind, E. C., & Klein, D. C. (Eds.). (1985).Community research: Methods, paradigms, and applications. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spradley, J. (1970).You owe yourself a drunk. Boston: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka-Matsumi, J., & Marsella, A. J. (1976). Cross-cultural variations in the phenomenological experience of depression: Word association.Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7, 379–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolan, P., Keys, C., Chertok, F., & Jason, L. (1990).Researching community psychology: Issues of theory and methods. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. (1978). Some universals of social behavior.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vega, W. A. (1992). Theoretical and pragmatic implications of cultural diversity for community research.American Journal of Community Psychology, 20, 375–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, M. C. (1971).Them children: A study in language and learning. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, B., Knodel, J., & Sittitrai, W. (1993). Focus groups and surveys as complementary research methods: A case example. In D. L. Morgan (Ed.),Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art (pp. 118–138). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The writing of this manuscript was supported in part by grants from the National Science Foundation (DBS-9154413) and the William T. Grant Foundation (92147692) awarded to the first author, and from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH43084) awarded to the second author.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hughes, D., Seidman, E. & Williams, N. Cultural phenomena and the research enterprise: Toward a culturally anchored methodology. Am J Commun Psychol 21, 687–703 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00942243

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00942243

Key words

Navigation