Skip to main content
Log in

A theory of developing competence with written mathematical symbols

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The paper presents a theory of how competence with written mathematical symbols develops. The theory proposes a succession of cognitive processes that cumulate to yield competence: (1) connecting individual symbols with referents; (2) developing symbol manipulation procedures; (3a) elaborating procedures for symbols; (3b) routinizing the procedures for manipulating symbols; and (4) using the symbols and rules as referents for building more abstract symbol systems. Each process must be engaged, and each must be engaged in sequence. Arguments in support of the theory are drawn from the history, philosophy and psychology of mathematics. The theory has implications for the sequencing and the content of instructional activities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • AlbersD. J. and G. A.Alexanderson (eds.).: 1985, Mathematical People: Profiles and Interviews, Contemporary Books, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • AllardiceB.: 1977, ‘The development of written representations for some mathematical concepts,’ Journal of Children's Mathematical Behavior 1(4), 135–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • BattistaM. T.: 1983, ‘A complete model for operations on integers,’ Arithmetic Teacher 30(9), 26–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • BriarsD. J. and J. G.Larkin: 1984, ‘An integrated model of skills in solving elementary word problems,’ Cognition and Instruction 1, 245–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • BrunerJ. S.; 1966, Toward a Theory of Instruction, Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • BrunerJ. S.: 1973, Beyond the Information Given Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • CarpenterT. P.: 1986, ‘Conceptual knowledge as a foundation for procedural knowledge: Implications from research on the initial learning of arithmetic,’ in J.Hiebert (ed.), Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, pp. 113–132, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. P., H. Bebout, and J. M. Moser: (in press), ‘The representation of basic addition and subtraction word problems,’ Journal for Research in Mathematics Education.

  • CarpenterT. P., J.Hiebert, and J. M.Moser: 1983, ‘The effect of instruction on children's solutions of addition and subtraction word problems,’ Educational Studies in Mathematics 14, 55–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • CarpenterT. P., W.Matthews, M. M.Lindquist, and E. A.Silver: 1984, ‘Achievement in mathematics: Results from the National Assessment,’ Elementary School Journal 84, 485–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • CarraherT. N. and A. D.Schliemann: 1987, ‘Manipulating equivalences in the market and in maths,’ in J. C.Bergeron, N.Hersovics, and C.Kieran (eds.), Proceedings of the Eleventh International Conference of the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Vol. 1, pp. 289–294, University of Montreal, Montreal.

    Google Scholar 

  • CaseR.: 1985, Intellectual Development: Birth to Adulthood, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • DavisR. B.: 1984, Learning Mathematics: The Cognitive Science Approach to Mathematics Education, Ablex, Norwood, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • DavisR. B.: 1986, ‘Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: A summary analysis,’ in J.Hiebert (ed.), Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, pp. 265–300, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCorteE. and L.Verschaffel: 1985 ‘Beginning first graders' initial representation of arithmetic word problems,’ Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 4, 3–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • DienesZ. P.: 1963, ‘On the learning of mathematics,’ Arithmetic Teacher, 10, 115–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • DoyleW.: 1988, “Work in mathematics classes: The context of students' thinking during instruction,’ Educational Psychologist 23, 167–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • GardnerH. and D.Wolf: 1983, ‘Waves and streams of symbolization: Notes on the development of symbolic capacities in young children,’ in D.Rogers and J. A.Sloboda (eds.), The Acquisition of Symbolic Skills, pp. 19–42, Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • GoldinG. A.: 1982, ‘Mathematical language and problem solving,’ Visible Language, 16, 221–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • GoldinG. A.: 1987 “Cognitive representational systems for mathematical problem solving,’ in C.Janvier (ed.), Problems of Representation in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, pp. 125–145, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • GoodmanN.: 1968, Languages of Art, Bobbs-Merrill, Indianapolis.

    Google Scholar 

  • GreenoJ. G.: 1983, “Conceptual entities,’ in D.Gentner and A. L.Stevens (eds.), Mental Models, pp. 227–252, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • HardyG. H.: 1941, A Mathematician's Apology, University Press, Cambridge, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, K.: (in press), ‘Ratio and proportion,’ in M. Behr and J. Hiebert (eds.), Research Agenda in Mathematics Education: Number Concepts and Operations in the Middle Grades, National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Reston, VA.

  • HiebertJ.: 1984, ‘Children's mathematics learning: The struggle to link form and understanding,’ Elementary School Journal 84, 497–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • HiebertJ. and P.Lefevre: 1986, ‘Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: An introductory analysis,’ in J.Hiebert (ed.), Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, pp. 1–27, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • HiebertJ. and D.Wearne: 1984, Children's Understanding of Decimal Numbers (Contract No. SPE-8218387), National Science Foundation, Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • HiebertJ. and D.Wearne: 1986, ‘Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, pp. 199–223, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • JanvierC. (ed.): 1987, Problems of representation in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • KaputJ. J.: 1987, ‘Towards a theory of symbol use in mathematics,’ in C.Javier (ed.), Problems of Representation in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, pp. 159–195, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • KennedyM. L.: 1977 ‘Young children's use of written symbolism to solve simple verbal addition and subtraction problems,’ Journal of Children's Mathematical Behavior 1(4), 122–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • KierenT. E.: 1988, ‘Personal knowledge of rational number: Its intuitive and formal development,’ in M.Behr and J.Hiebert (eds.), Research Agenda in Mathematics Education: Number Concepts and Operations in the Middle Grades, pp. 162–181, National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Reston, VA.

    Google Scholar 

  • KitcherP.: 1983, The Nature of Mathematical Knowledge, Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • KuhnD. and E.Phelps: 1982, ‘The development of problem-solving strateties,’ in H.Reese (ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 17, pp. 1–44, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • LakatosI.: 1976, Proofs and Refutations The Logic of Mathematical Discovery, in J.Worrall and E.Zahar (eds.), Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • LakatosI.: 1978, Mathematics, Science, and Epistemology, in J.Worrall and G.Currie (eds.), Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeshR., M.Landau, and E.Hamilton: 1983, ‘Conceptual models and applied mathematical problem-solving research,’ in R.Lesh and M.Landau (eds.), Acquisition of Mathematics Concepts and Processes, pp. 263–343, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • LindvallC. M. and C. G.Ibarra: 1980, ‘Incorrect procedures used by primary grade pupils in solving open addition and subtraction sentences,’ Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 11, 50–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • MasonJ. H.: 1987a, Representing representing: Notes following the conference, in C.Janvier (ed.), Problems of Representation in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, pp. 207–214, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • MasonJ. H.: 1987b, What do symbols represent?, in C.Janvier (ed.), Problems of Representation in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, pp. 73–81, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Assessment of Educational Progress: 1983 The Third National Mathematics Assessment: Results, Trends and Issues, Educational Commission of the States Denver, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Omanson, S. F., I. Peled, and L. B. Resnick: 1982, March, Instruction by Mapping: Its Effects on Understanding and Skill in Subtraction, Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York.

  • PoincareH.: 1921, The Foundations of Science (G. B. Halsted, trans.) Science Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • PolyaG.: 1962, ‘The teaching of mathematics and the biogenetic law,’ in J. J.Good (ed.), The Scientist Speculates, pp. 352–356, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • PressleyM.: 1986, ‘The relevance of the good strategy user model to the teaching of mathematics,’ Educational Psychologist 21, 139–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • ResnickL. B.: 1987, ‘Learning in school and out,’ Educational Researcher 16 (6), 13–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • ResnickL. B. and S. F.Omanson: 1987, ‘Learning to understand arithmetic’ in R.Glaser (ed.), Advances in Instructional Psychology, Vol. 3, pp. 45–91, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • RileyM. S., J. G.Greeno, and J. I.Heller: 1983, ‘Development of children's problem-solving ability in arithmetic,’, in H. P.Ginsburg (ed.), The Development of Mathematical Thinking, pp. 153–196, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • RoboldA. I.: 1983, ‘Grid arrays for multiplication,’ Arithmetic Teacher 30(5), 14–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • SawadaD.: 1985, ‘Mathematical symbols: Insight through invention,’ Arithmetic Teacher 32(6), 20–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • SchoenfeldA. H.: 1986. Mathematical Problem Solving, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • SchoolsCouncil: 1969, Mathematics in Primary Schools (3rd ed.), Her Majesty's Stationery Office, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • ShulmanL. S.: 1974, ‘The psychology of school subjects: A premature obituary?’ Journal of Research in Science Teaching 11, 319–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • SinclairA. and H.Sinclair: 1984, ‘Preschool children's interpretation of written numbers,’ Human Learning 3, 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • SinclairH. and A.Sinclair: 1986, ‘Children's mastery of written numerals and the construction of basic number concepts,’ in J.Hiebert (ed.), Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, pp. 59–74, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • SkempR. R.: 1982, ‘Communicating mathematics: Surface structures and deep structures,’ Visible Language 16, 281–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skemp, R. R. (ed.): 1982b, ‘Understanding the symbolism of mathematics [special issue],’ Visible Language 16(3).

  • SteffeL. P., E.vonGlasersfeld, J.Richards, and P.Cobb: 1983, Children's Counting Types: Philosophy, Theory, and Application, Preager, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanEngenH.: 1949, ‘An analysis of meaning in arithmetic,’ Elementary School Journal 49, 321–329; 395–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanLehnK.: 1986, ‘Arithmetic procedures are induced from examples,’ in J.Hiebert (ed.), Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, pp. 133–179, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • vonGlasersfeldE.: 1987, ‘Preliminaries to any theory of representation,’ in C.Janvier (ed.), Problems of Representation in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, pp. 215–225, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wearne, D. and J. Hiebert: (in press), ‘A cognitive approach to meaningful mathematics instruction: Testing a local theory,’ Journal for Research in Mathematics Education.

  • WhiteheadA. N.: 1911, An Introduction to Mathematics, Oxford University, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • WoodrowD.: 1982, ‘Mathematical symbolism,’ Visible Language 16, 289–302.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The writing of this paper was supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (MDR 8651552). However, the opinions are those of the author rather than the Foundation.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hiebert, J. A theory of developing competence with written mathematical symbols. Educ Stud Math 19, 333–355 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00312451

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00312451

Keywords

Navigation