Skip to main content
Log in

Children's algorithms as schemes

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Arithmetical knowledge is viewed as simply the coordinated schemes of actions and operations the child has constructed at a particular point in time. A fundamental distinction is drawn between operative and figurative schemes in the context of additive schemes. Numerical extension, an operative scheme, is distinguished from intuitive extension, a figurative scheme. The basis of the distinction resides in (1) the anticipatory nature of the schemes, (2) the adaptability of the schemes, and (3) the nature of the concepts which serve as a basis for what triggers the schemes. This fundamental distinction is crucial in the mathematical instruction of the child for the child necessarily has to construct his or her own mathematical reality even if that reality eventually becomes compatible with the reality of the social group in which the child has to operate.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BoothL. R.: 1981, ‘Child methods in secondary school mathematics’, Educational Studies in Mathematics 12, 29–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, W. A.: 1935, ‘Psychological consideration in the learning and teaching of arithmetic’, in W. D. Reeve (ed.), Teaching of Arithmetic. The Tenth Yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University.

  • CaramuelJ.: 1680/1977, ‘Meditatio prooemialis, Mathesis biceps’, in A.Parea, P.Soriano, and P.Terzi (eds.), L'arithmetica binaria e le altre aritmetiche di Giovanni Caramuel, Vescovo di Vigevano, Vigevano, Italy: Academia Tiberina.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easley, J. A., Jr.: 1975, ‘Thoughts on individualized instruction’, in H. Bauersfeld, M. Otte and H. C. Steiner (eds.), Schriftenreihe des IDM, Institut fur Didaktic der Mathematik. Universitat Bielefeld.

  • ErlwangerS. H.: 1973, ‘Benny's concept of rules and answers in IPI mathematics’, Journal of Children's Mathematical Behavior 5, 34–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • FreudenthalH.: 1981, ‘Major problems of mathematics education’, Educational Studies in Mathematics 12, 133–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • GelmanR. and GallistelC. R.: 1978, ‘The child's understanding of number’, Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, L.: 1976, ‘Child-generated computational algorithms: An exploratory investigation of cognitive development and mathematics instruction with Grade II Children’, unpublished manuscript.

  • InhelderB. and PiagetJ.: 1969, The Early Growth of Logic in the Child: Classification and Seriation, New York: The Norton Library, W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • JamesG. and JamesR. C.: 1968, Mathematics Dictionary, New Jersey: Van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • MassimoPiatelli-Palmarini.: 1980, Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKnightC. and DavisR. B.: 1980, ‘The influence of semantic content on algorithmic behavior’, The Journal of Mathematical Behavior 3, 39–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • OsborneA. R.: 1976, ‘Conditions for algorithmic imagination’, in M.Suydam and A. R.Osborne (eds.), Algorithmic Learning, ERIC/SMEAC Columbus, Ohio.

    Google Scholar 

  • PiagetJ.: 1970, Genetic Epistemology, New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J.: 1980, ‘The psychogenesis of knowledge and its epistemological significance’, in Massimo Piatelli-Palmarini (1980).

  • ResnickL. B.: 1982, ‘Syntax and semantics in learning to subtract’, in T.Carpenter, J.Moser, and T.Romberg (eds.), Addition and Subtraction: A Cognitive Perspective, Hilsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • RiedesalC. A.: 1973, Guiding Discovery in Elementary School Mathematics, New York: Appleton Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • SematH.: 1959, Introduction to Atomic and Nuclear Physics, New York: Rinehart.

    Google Scholar 

  • SkempR.: 1978, ‘Relational understanding and instrumental understanding’, Arithmetic Teacher 26, 9–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • SteffeL. P., ThompsonP., and RichardsJ.: 1982, ‘Children's counting in arithmetic problem solving’, in T.Carpenter, J.Moser, and T.Romberg (eds.), Addition and Subtraction: A Cognitive Perspective, Hilsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • SteffeL. P., vonGlaserfeldE., RichardsJ., and CobbP.: 1983, ‘Children's counting types: philosophy, theory, and application, New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P.: 1982, ‘A theoretical framework for understanding young children's concepts of whole number numeration’, Doctoral Dissertation, Department of Mathematics Education, University of Georgia.

  • vonGlasersfeldE.: 1980, ‘The concept of equilibration in a constructivist theory of knowledge’, in F.Benseler, P. M.Hejl, and W. K.Kock (eds.), Autopoiesis, Communication, and Society, New York: Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • vonGlasersfeldE.: 1981, ‘An attentional model for the conceptual construction of units and number’, Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 12, 83–94.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Additional information

The comments and criticisms of Paul Cobb, Ernst von Glaserfeld, Lauren Resnick, and an anonymous reviewer on an earlier version of this paper are gratefully acknowledged.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Steffe, L.P. Children's algorithms as schemes. Educ Stud Math 14, 109–125 (1983). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00303681

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00303681

Keywords

Navigation