Skip to main content
Log in

Musical pluralism and the science of music

  • Original paper in Philosophy of Science
  • Published:
European Journal for Philosophy of Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The scientific investigation of music requires contributions from a diverse array of disciplines (e.g. anthropology, musicology, neuroscience, psychology, music theory, music therapy, sociology, computer science, evolutionary biology, archaeology, acoustics and philosophy). Given the diverse methodologies, interests and research targets of the disciplines involved, we argue that there is a plurality of legitimate research questions about music, necessitating a focus on integration. In light of this we recommend a pluralistic conception of music—that there is no unitary definition divorced from some discipline, research question or context. This has important implications for how the scientific study of music ought to proceed: we show that some definitions are complementary, that is, they reflect different research interests and ought to be retained and, where possible, integrated, while others are antagonistic, they represent real empirical disagreement about music’s nature and how to account for it. We illustrate this in discussion of two related issues: questions about the evolutionary function (if any) of music, and questions of the innateness (or otherwise) of music. These debates have been, in light of pluralism, misconceived. We suggest that, in both cases, scientists ought to proceed by constructing integrated models which take into account the dynamic interaction between different aspects of music.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. An anonymous referee points out that such operational, instrumental definitions often require a stipulated, general account of ‘music’ to get off the ground, otherwise the relationship between some of the concepts we discuss, for instance the distinction between ‘music’ and ‘musicality’, is decidedly ambiguous. This might be right, the relationship between the various concepts required to make music scientifically tractable can be very complex. We take this to be grist for our mill: on our view, different research agendas require a number of different, but importantly related concepts. Considering how these concepts interact is an important part of integrating such perspectives.

  2. Note that our use of the term ‘scale’ is not meant to imply literal measurement.

  3. Musical features associated with Western music-theoretic concepts, however, are arranged temporally—meter, tonality and so on have specific meanings relative to different eras in music history. A chronology of Western music-theoretic concepts might fall under the broad study of History of music on Fig. 1. Of course, Western music theory is not the only music theory! One of the great weaknesses of much musical research is that it assumes that a Western musical experience is universal (or very nearly) and thus fails to take into account the nature of music/musicality and its social correlates in a variety of non-Western contexts. Some music researchers are acutely aware of this, and keen to point out the problem, and we are deeply sympathetic. Thanks to an anonymous referee for pressing us on this issue.

  4. See Craver (2005, 2007) for discussions of both different conceptions of ‘levels’ and integration in cognitive neuroscience.

  5. Grahn (2012) also discusses the benefits and limitations of employing neuroscientific methods in the study of musical rhythm in general (see also Grahn 2009).

  6. Another example of integration across levels of organization: Temperley (2014) asks which probability model best predicts available data about musical intervals? He distinguishes the Markov model (Rohrmeier et al. 2015; Conklin and Whitten 1995; Pearce and Wiggins 2004, 2006) from the Gaussian model (Von Hippel 2000; Marr 1982; Shi et al. 2010); the Markov model defines a musical interval’s probability ‘by its count in a corpus, conditioned on previous intervals’, while the Gaussian model defines it as ‘a simple function of the size of the interval to the previous note and the distance to the mean pitch of the melody’ (Temperley 2014, p. 96). These models are tested against three data sets: sequential data from large samples of folksong, chorale, classical, and rock melodies, experimental data from the empirical study of melodic expectation, and distribution data capturing the melodic intervals comprising the Essen Folksong Collection (cf. Schaffrath 1995). Again, we here see the explanatory force carried by the modelling, and the data from actual musical form playing a supporting role; Temperley argues that the Markov model is to be preferred for its lower cross-entropy than that of the Gaussian model (see Temperley 2014, pp. 90–92).

  7. See Davies (2012a) and McKeown-Green (2014) for counterexamples to Kania’s analysis.

  8. For some schematic examples of this tension, note that music that does not meet Kania’s ‘intentional’ requirement might still realize the role of music in some socio-cultural context and thus should be categorised as music by the ethnomusicologist, given their research agenda, but perhaps not by the psychologist, given theirs. Similarly, music that does not meet a (Western) ‘basic musical features’ requirement might still count as music for the ethnomusicologist and psychologist, but perhaps not for the (Western) music theorist or aesthetician. In short, there are multiple, non-equivalent concepts of music that are legitimized by their role in some research agenda.

  9. Consider the interdisciplinary dissonance which comes to the fore in debates over musical versus linguistic syntax (see e.g. Patel 2012a, b versus London 2012; also Slevc et al. 2009 on ‘making psycholinguistics musical’), notions of musical ‘meaning’ (e.g. Koelsch 2011; Cross 2008; Davies 1994), and so on. Thanks to an anonymous referee for pressing us on this.

  10. Note that Cross does call for a (mere) terminological revision; he argues that cognitive science should be more sympathetic to accommodating music in non-Western cultures to reach a more comprehensive understanding of human musical faculties. We concur (also, see Stevens 2012); theorists should be wary of reducing ‘music’ to ‘Western music’.

  11. For example, studies have reported interesting findings regarding the cultural evolution of non-human animal song (Noad et al. 2000; Payne and Payne 1993; Payne et al. 1988; Hoeschele et al. 2015; Merchant et al. 2015).

  12. To be sure, ‘theory of mind’ approaches are compatible with other models and hypotheses too, including the co-evolutionary approach that we endorse in the discussion that follows. The uses and implications of theory of mind research is another example of the pluralistic research strategy that we endorse in our paper.

  13. The idea that the biological and cultural aspects of music are correlated and interact is reasonably uncontroversial (cf. Hambrick and Tucker-Drob 2014), however this has not been adequately reflected in most theories of music evolution (though see Tomlinson 2015).

  14. Similarly, the mutation of gene MYH16, thought to be responsible for the weakening of our ancestors’ more robust jaw muscles (Wrangham 2009), might also be reasonably thought to be an adaptation to novel cooking environments (that is, if Wrangham’s hypothesis about the deep history of cooking is right).

  15. We are not necessarily committing to scientific or theory-holism. Rather, we mean a model which incorporates both evidence-streams and complementary definitions from different disciplines. Worries about testing holistic models are not pressing in such cases. In fact, extra streams of evidence can improve our capacity to test such models.

  16. We do not deny that such music/musicality distinctions may have important roles in, for instance, anthropological investigation of cross-cultural universals, and so on.

  17. One example of this debate targets beat perception. Some (e.g. Winkler et al. 2009) favour an innate model based on the early development of this perception in infants. However, others (e.g. Grahn 2012) point out that this is too quick: much learning occurs in pre-natal stages. For instance, empirical studies demonstrate that ‘infants exposed to particular pieces of music before birth show distinct preferences for those same pieces after birth’ (Sloboda 2005, p. 267; see also Hepper 1991 and Parncutt 2009 for more on prenatal and infant musical conditioning).

  18. An anonymous referee highlights a disanalogy between the acquisition of chess skills and culturally-appropriate music appreciation: the former is effortful while the latter is a much simpler task.

  19. More precisely, entrainment is the ‘coming together’ or synchronicity of multiple interacting, though independent, rhythmic oscillators or processes.

References

  • Abrahamsen, A. A. (1987). Bridging boundaries versus breaking boundaries—psycholinguistics in perspective. Synthese, 72(3), 355–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariew, A. (2003). Ernst Mayr’s ‘ultimate/proximate’ distinction reconsidered and reconstructed. Biology and Philosophy, 18(4), 553–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrow, J. D. (2005). The artful universe expanded. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, J. (1994). The proximate/ultimate distinction in the multiple careers of Ernst Mayr. Biology and Philosophy, 9(3), 333–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bello, J. P., Rowe, R., Guedes, C., & Toussaint, G. (2015). Five perspectives on musical rhythm. Journal of New Music Research, 44(1), 1–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bender, A., Hutchins, E., & Medin, D. (2010). Anthropology in cognitive science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(3), 374–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blacking, J. (1973). How musical is man? Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brigandt, I. (2010). Beyond reduction and pluralism: toward an epistemology of explanatory integration in biology. Erkenntnis, 73, 295–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. (2000). Evolutionary models of music: From sexual selection to group selection. In F. Tonneau & N. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in ecology 13: Behavior, evolution and culture (pp. 231–281). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S., Savage, P. E., Ko, A. M., Stoneking, M., Ko, Y., Loo, J., & Trejaut, J. A. (2013). Correlations in the population structure of music, genes and language. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20132072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calcott, B. (2013). Why the proximate-ultimate distinction is misleading, and why it matters for understanding the evolution of cooperation. In K. Sterelny, R. Joyce, B. Calcott, & B. Fraser (Eds.), Cooperation and its evolution (pp. 249–264). Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conklin, D., & Whitten, I. H. (1995). Multiple viewpoint systems for music prediction. Journal of New Music Research, 24, 51–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, P., Rouse, A., Wilson, M. & Reichmuth, C. (2013). A Californian sea lion (Zalophus californianus) can keep the beat: motor entrainment to rhythmic auditory stimuli in a non vocal mimic. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(4), 412–427.

  • Craver, C. (2005). Beyond reduction: mechanisms, multifield integration and the unity of neuroscience. Studies in the History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 36, 373–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craver, C. (2007). Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, I. (2007). Music and cognitive evolution. In L. Barrett & R. I. M. Dunbar (Eds.), Oxford University Press handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 649–667). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross, I. (2008). Musicality and the human capacity for culture. Musicae Scientiae, 12(1 suppl), 147–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, I. (2012a). Cognitive science and the cultural nature of music. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 668–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, I. (2012b). Music and biocultural evolution. In M. Clayton, T. Herbert, & R. Middleton (Eds.), The cultural study of music: A critical introduction (2nd ed., pp. 15–27). London: Routlegde.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross, I. (2012c). Music as a social and cognitive process. In P. Rebuschat, M. Rohrmeier, J. A. Hawkins, & I. Cross (Eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems (pp. 313–328). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Currie, A. (2013). Convergence as evidence. British Journal for Philosophy of Science, 64(4), 763–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darden, L., & Maull, N. (1977). Interfield theories. Philosophy of Science, 44, 43–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, S. (1994). Musical meaning and expression. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, S. (2012a). On defining music. The Monist, 95(4), 535–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, S. (2012b). The artful species: Aesthetics, art & evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake, E. (1982). Aesthetic experience and human evolution. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 41(2), 145–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake, E. (2008). If music is the food of love, what about survival and reproductive success? Musicae Scientiae, 12(1 suppl), 169–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size, and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eerola, T. (2012). Modeling listeners’ emotional response to music. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 607–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ereshefsky, M. (1998). Species pluralism and anti-realism. Philosophy of Science, 65(1), 103–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ereshefsky, M. (2010). Species. In: The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2010 Edition), E. N. Zalta (ed.), URL = <http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2010/entries/species/>.

  • Esparza, T. M., Bello, J. P., & Humphrey, E. J. (2015). From genre classification to rhythmic similarity: computational and musicological insights. Journal of New Music Research, 44(1), 39–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, M. W., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1976). Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission. Theoretical Population Biology, 9(2), 238–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, M. W., & Laland, K. N. (1996). Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 11, 453–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2005). The evolution of music in comparative perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2009). Biology of music: another one bites the dust. Current Biology, 19(10), 403–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2015). Four principles of bio-musicology. Philosophical Transactions B: Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0091.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1974). Special sciences: or the disunity of science as a working hypothesis. Synthese, 28, 97–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerry, D. W., Faux, A. L., & Trainor, L. J. (2010). Effects of Kindermusik training on infants’ rhythmic enculturation. Developmental Science, 13(3), 545–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godt, I. (2005). Music: a practical definition. Musical Times, 146, 83–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grahn, J. A. (2009). Neuroscientific investigations of musical rhythm: recent advances and future challenges. Contemporary Music Review, 28(3), 251–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grahn, J. A. (2012). Neural mechanisms of rhythm perception: current findings and future perspectives. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 585–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: a review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hambrick, D. Z., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2014). The genetics of music accomplishment: evidence for gene-environment correlation and interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(1), 112–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, A. (2007). Aesthetics and music. Michigan: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannon, E. E., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11(11), 466–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005a). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood. Psychological Science, 16(1), 48–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005b). Tuning in to rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(35), 12639–12643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hattori, Y., Tomonaga, M. & Matsuzawa, T. (2013). Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Scientific Reports. doi:10.1038/srep.01566.

  • Hepper, P. G. (1991). An examination of fetal learning before and after birth. Irish Journal of Psychology, 12(2), 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D. & Verpooten, J. (2015). The evolutionary significance of the arts: expoloring the by-product hypothesis in the context of ritual, precursors, and cultural evolution. Biological Theory, 10, 73–85.

  • Hoeschele, M., Merchant, H., Kikuchi, Y., Hattori, Y., & ten Cate, C. (2015). Searching for the origins of musicality across species. Philosophical Transactions B: Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0094.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzapfel, A. (2015). Relation between surface rhythm and rhythmic modes in Turkish makam music. Journal of New Music Research, 44(1), 25–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Honing, H., & Ploeger, A. (2012). Cognition and the evolution of music. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 513–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Honing, H., Merchant, H., Háden, G. P., Prado, L., & Bartolo, R. (2012). Rhesus monkeys (macaca mulatta) detect rhythmic groups in music, but not the beat. PLoS One, 7(12), e51369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huron, D. (1999). Ernest Bloch lectures. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kania, A. (2011). Definition. In T. Gracyk & A. Kania (Eds.), Routledge companion to philosophy and music (pp. 3–13). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Killin, A. (2013). The arts and human nature: evolutionary aesthetics and the evolutionary status of art behaviours. Biology and Philosophy, 28(4), 703–718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, P. (1984). Species. Philosophy of Science, 51(2), 308–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koelsch, S. (2011). Towards a neural basis of processing musical semantics. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(2), 89–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W., & Uller, T. (2013). More on how and why: cause and effect in biology revisited. Biology and Philosophy, 28(5), 719–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerud, K. D., Almonte, F. V., Kim, J. C., & Large, E. W. (2014). Mode-locking neurodynamics predict auditory brainstem responses to musical intervals. Hearing Research, 308, 41–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, J. (1990). Music, art, and metaphysics. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone, S. R., & Thompson, W. F. (2009). The emergence of music from the theory of mind. Musicae Scientiae, 13(2 suppl), 83–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • London, J. (2012). Schemas, not syntax: A reply to Patel. In P. Rebuschat, M. Rohrmeier, J. A. Hawkins, & I. Cross (Eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems (pp. 242–247). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loui, P. (2012). Learning and liking of melody and harmony: further studies in artificial grammar learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 554–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Love, A. C. (2006). Explaining evolutionary innovations and novelties: criteria of explanatory adequacy and epistemological prerequisites. Philosophy of Science, 75(5), 874–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F. (2012). Musicality: instinct or acquired skill? Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 498–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134, 1501–1506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAuley, J. D. (2010). Tempo and rhythm. In M. R. Jones, R. R. Fay, & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Music perception: Springer handbook of auditory research (pp. 165–199). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, J. (2008). The evolution of music. Nature, 453, 287–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKeown-Green, J. (2014). What is music? Is there a definitive answer? Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 72(4), 393–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. (2014). The aesthetic niche. British Journal of Aesthetics, 54, 471–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merchant, H., Grahn, J., Trainor, L., Rohrmeier, M., & Fitch, W. T. (2015). Finding the beat: a neural perspective across humans and non-human primates. Philosophical Transactions B: Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0093.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merker, B. (2012). The vocal learning constellation: Imitation, ritual culture, encephalization. In N. Bannan (Ed.), Music, language, and human evolution (pp. 215–260). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Merker, B., Morley, I., & Zuidema, W. (2015). Five fundamental constraints on theories of the origins of music. Philosophical Transactions B: Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0095.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merriam, A. P. (1963). Purposes of ethnomusicology, an anthropological view. Ethnomusicology, 7(3), 206–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merriam, A. P. (1964). The anthropology of music. Chicago: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. F. (2000). Evolution of human music through sexual selection. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 329–360). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. F. (2001). Aesthetic fitness: how sexual selection shaped artistic virtuosity as a fitness indicator and aesthetic preference as mate choice criteria. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 2(1), 20–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. (2005). The singing Neanderthals. Great Britain: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, I. T., Bentley, G. E., Wotus, C., & Wingfield, J. C. (2006). Photoperiod-independent changes in immunoreactive brain gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) in a free-living, tropical bird. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution, 68(1), 37–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morley, I. (2013). The prehistory of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nettl, B. (2005). The study of ethnomusicology: thirty-one issues and concepts (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuhaus, C. (2013). Processing musical form: behavioural and neurocognitive approaches. Musicae Scientiae, 17(1), 109–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noad, M. J., Cato, D. H., Bryden, M. M., Jenner, M., & Jenner, K. C. S. (2000). Cultural revolution in whale songs. Nature, 408, 537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, P., & Putnam, H. (1958). The unity of science as a working hypothesis. In G. Maxwell, H. Feigl, & M. Scriven (Eds.), Concepts, theories, and the mind-body problem (pp. 3–36). Minneapolis: Minnesota University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (2009). The emotional antecedents to the evolution of music and language. Musicae Scientiae, 13(2 suppl), 229–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parncutt, R. (2009). Prenatal and infant conditioning, the mother schema, and the origins of music and religion. Musicae Scientiae, 13(2 suppl), 119–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, A. D. (2006). Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music Perception, 24(1), 99–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, A. D. (2010). Music, biological evolution, and the brain. In M. Bailar, C. Field, & C. J. Henry (Eds.), Emerging disciplines (pp. 91–144). Houston: Rice University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel, A. D. (2012a). Language, music, and the brain: A resource-sharing framework. In P. Rebuschat, M. Rohrmeier, J. A. Hawkins, & I. Cross (Eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems (pp. 204–223). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel, A. D. (2012b). Advancing the comparative study of linguistic and musical syntactic processing. In P. Rebuschat, M. Rohrmeier, J. A. Hawkins, & I. Cross (Eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems (pp. 248–253). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, R. B., & Payne, L. L. (1993). Song copying and cultural transmission in indigo buntings. Animal Behaviour, 46(6), 1045–1065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, R. B., Payne, L. L., & Doehlert, S. M. (1988). Biological and cultural success of song memes in indigo buntings. Ecology, 69(1), 104–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, M. T., & Wiggins, G. A. (2004). Improved methods for statistical modelling of monophonic music. Journal of New Music Research, 33, 367–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, M. T., & Wiggins, G. A. (2006). Expectation in melody: the influence of context and learning. Music Perception, 23, 377–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, M. T., & Wiggins, G. A. (2012). Auditory expectation: the information dynamics of music perception and cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 625–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potochnik, A. (2010). Explanatory independence and epistemic interdependence: a case study of the optimality approach. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 61, 213–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rebuschat, P., Rohrmeier, M., Hawkins, J. A., & Cross, I. (Eds.). (2012). Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., Zuidema, W., Wiggins, G. A., & Scharff, C. (2015). Principles of structure building in music, language and animal song. Philosophical Transactions B: Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0097.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothenberg, D. (2008). Whale music: anatomy of an interspecies duet. Leonardo Music Journal, 18, 47–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rzeszutek, T., Savage, P. E., & Brown, S. (2012). The structure of cross-cultural musical diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1733), 1606–1612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaal, N. K., Banissy, M. J., & Lange, K. (2015). The rhythm span task: comparing memory capacity for musical rhythms in musicians and non-musicians. Journal of New Music Research, 44(1), 3–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schachner, A. (2013). The origins of human and avian auditory-motor entrainment. Nova Acta Leopoldina, 111, 243–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaffrath, H. (1995). In D. Huron (Ed.), The Essen folksong collection. Stanford: Center for Computer-Assisted Research in the Humanities.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, E. G., & Trainor, L. J. (1996). Sensory consonance and the perceptual similarity of complex-tone harmonic intervals: tests of adult and infant listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100(5), 3321–3328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1996a). Children’s discrimination of melodic intervals. Developmental Psychology, 32(6), 1039–1050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1996b). Natural intervals in music: a perspective from infant listeners. Psychological Science, 7(5), 272–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1999). Culture-general and culture-specific factors in the discrimination of melodies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(2), 107–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sephus, N. H., Lanterman, A. D., & Anderson, D. V. (2015). Modulation spectral features: in pursuit of invariant representations of music with application to unsupervised source identification. Journal of New Music Research, 44(1), 58–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sethares, W. A., & Toussaint, G. (2015). Expressive timbre and timing in rhythmic performance: analysis of Steve Reich’s Clapping Music. Journal of New Music Research, 44(1), 11–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, L., Griffiths, L., Feldman, N., & Sanborn, A. (2010). Exemplar models as a mechanism for performing Bayesian inference. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17, 443–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slevc, L. R., Rosenberg, J. C., & Patel, A. D. (2009). Making psycholinguistics musical: self-paced reading time evidence for shared processing of linguistic and musical syntax. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 374–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloboda, J. A. (2005). Exploring the musical mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soley, G., & Hannon, E. E. (2010). Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: a cross-cultural comparison. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 286–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stalinski, S. M., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2012). Music cognition: a developmental perspective. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 485–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, C. J. (2012). Music perception and cognition: a review of recent cross-cultural research. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 653–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tëmkin, I., & Eldgredge, N. (2007). Phylogenetics and material cultural evolution. Current Anthropology, 48(1), 146–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temperley, D. (2014). Probabilistic models of melodic interval. Music Perception, 32(1), 85–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tesoriero, M., & Rickard, N. S. (2012). Music-enhanced recall: an effect of mood congruence, emotion arousal or emotion function? Musicae Scientiae, 16(3), 340–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20(4), 410–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson, G. (2015). A million years of music. New York: Zone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trainor, L. J. (2005). Are there critical periods for musical development? Developmental Psychobiology, 46(3), 262–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trainor, L. J. (2015). The origins of music in auditory scene analysis and the roles of evolution and culture in musical creation. Philosophical Transactions B: Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0089.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trainor, L. J., & Corrigall, K. A. (2010). Music acquisition and effects of musical experience. Music Perception, 36, 89–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S. E. (2003). The developmental origins of musicality. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 669–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S. E., & Trainor, L. J. (1998). Singing to infants: lullabies and play songs. Advances in Infancy Research, 12, 43–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S. E., Becker, J., & Morley, I. (2015). Cross-cultural perspectives on music and musicality. Philosophical Transactions B: Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0096.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Hippel, P. (2000). Redefining pitch proximity: tessitura and mobility as constraints on melodic intervals. Music Perception, 17, 315–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., & Brown, S. (Eds.). (2000). The origins of music. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt, W. (1994). The ontology of complex systems: levels of organization, perspectives and causal thickets. Canadian Journal of Philosophy (supplementary volume), 20, 207–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkler, I., Háden, G. P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., & Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(7), 1479–1489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. (2009). Catching fire: How cooking made us human. London: Profile.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R., & Conklin-Brittain, N. (2003). Cooking as a biological trait. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 136(1), 35–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, A. (1999). Rethinking unity as a ‘working hypothesis’ for philosophy: how archaeologists exploit the disunities of science. Perspectives on Science, 7(3), 293–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anton Killin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Currie, A., Killin, A. Musical pluralism and the science of music. Euro Jnl Phil Sci 6, 9–30 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13194-015-0123-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13194-015-0123-z

Keywords

Navigation