Advertisement

Morphology

, Volume 27, Issue 4, pp 643–684 | Cite as

Blocking and paradigm gaps

  • Itamar Kastner
  • Vera Zu
Article

Abstract

Gaps in morphological paradigms are often explained in terms of blocking: generating one form is blocked by the existence of a paraphrase. Another way of thinking about paradigm gaps dissociates their existence from competition between forms. Unlike in competition-based approaches, systematic gaps can be seen as true gaps; the system might not generate a certain form, but this form is not considered in comparison to others. Adopting this latter approach, we argue that inflectional paradigms are neither morphosyntactic primitives nor the result of competition. This claim is supported by data from two unrelated languages. For Hebrew, we demonstrate that a passive gap is not the result of competition with analytic paraphrases. For Latin, we show that a cyclic, syntax-based approach is superior to a theory that generates nonactive verbs in the lexicon and has them compete against each other. Systematic paradigm gaps are thus argued to result from syntactic structure building, without competition regulating independent morphological constructions.

Keywords

Competition Hebrew Latin Paradigm gaps Passive Voice 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Bronwyn Bjorkman, Dave Embick, Richie Kayne, Alec Marantz and Neil Myler for comments on previous versions of this article, and to Stephanie Harves, Tal Linzen, Roumi Pancheva, Jim Wood, the NYU Morphology Research Group and the audience at WCCFL 32 for discussion (in particular Rajesh Bhatt, Sabine Iatridou and Alfredo García Pardo). We are also grateful for the highly constructive comments of the reviewers and editor Adam Albright. This research was supported in part by DFG award AL 554/8-1 to Artemis Alexiadou (I.K.).

References

  1. Adger, D. (2003). Core syntax: a minimalist approach. London: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  2. Adger, D., Béjar, S., & Harbour, D. (2003). Directionality in allomorphy: a reply to Carstairs-McCarthy. Transactions of the Philological Society, 101(1), 109–115. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Albright, A. (2003). A quantitative study of Spanish paradigm gaps. In G. Garding & M. Tsujimura (Eds.), Proceedings of WCCFL (Vol. 22, pp. 1–14). Somerville: Cascadilla Press. Google Scholar
  4. Albright, A. (2011). Paradigms. In M. van Oostendorp, C. J. Ewen, E. Hume, & K. Rice (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to phonology (pp. 1972–2001). Oxford: Blackwell Sci. Google Scholar
  5. Alexiadou, A., & Doron, E. (2012). The syntactic construction of two non-active voices: passive and middle. Journal of Linguistics, 48, 1–34. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Alexiadou, A., Gehrke, B., & Schäfer, F. (2014). The argument structure of adjectival participles revisited. Lingua, 149, 118–138. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Arad, M. (2005). Roots and patterns: Hebrew morpho-syntax. Berlin: Springer. Google Scholar
  8. Aronoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  9. Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself: stems and inflectional classes. Cambridge: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  10. Arregi, K., & Nevins, A. (2014). A monoradical approach to some cases of disuppletion. Theoretical Linguistics, 40(3/4), 311–330. Google Scholar
  11. Bachrach, A., & Nevins, A. (Eds.) (2008). Inflectional identity. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  12. Baerman, M. (2007). Morphological typology of deponency. In M. Baerman, G. Corbett, D. Brown, & A. Hippisley (Eds.), Deponency and morphological mismatches (pp. 1–19). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Baerman, M., Corbett, G., & Brown, D. (Eds.) (2010). Defective paradigms: missing forms and what they tell us. Proceedings of the British Academy: Vol. 163. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  14. Bailyn, J. F., & Nevins, A. (2008). Russian genitive plurals are impostors. In A. Bachrach & A. Nevins (Eds.), Inflectional identity (pp. 237–270). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  15. Baker, M., Johnson, K., & Roberts, I. (1989). Passive arguments raised. Linguistic Inquiry, 20(2), 219–251. Google Scholar
  16. Bat-El, O. (1994). Stem modification and cluster transfer in Modern Hebrew. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 12, 571–596, doi: 10.1007/BF00992928. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Bat-El, O. (2002). Semitic verb structure within a universal perspective. In J. Shimron (Ed.), Language processing and acquisition in languages of semitic, root-based, morphology (pp. 29–59). Amsterdam: Benjamins. Google Scholar
  18. Bat-El, O. (2008). Morphologically conditioned v–ø alternation in Hebrew: distinction among nouns, adjectives & participles, and verbs. In S. Armon-Lotem, G. Danon, & S. Rothstein (Eds.), Current issues in generative Hebrew linguistics. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Google Scholar
  19. Bermúdez-Otero, R. (2013). The Spanish lexicon stores stems with theme vowels, not roots with inflectional class features. Probus, 25, 3–103. Google Scholar
  20. Bermúdez-Otero, R. (2016). We do not need structuralist morphemes, but we do need constituent structure. In D. Siddiqi & H. Harley (Eds.), Morphological metatheory (pp. 385–428). Amsterdam: Benjamins. Google Scholar
  21. Bjorkman, B. (2011). BE-ing default: the morphosyntax of auxiliaries. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Google Scholar
  22. Bjorkman, B. (2014). Verbal inflection and overflow auxiliaries. Ms., University of Toronto. Google Scholar
  23. Bobaljik, J. D. (2000). The ins and outs of contextual allomorphy. In K. Grohmann & C. Struijke (Eds.), University of Maryland working papers in linguistics (Vol. 10, pp. 35–71). Google Scholar
  24. Bonami, O. (2015). Periphrasis as collocation. Morphology, 25, 63–110. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Boneh, N. (2013). Tense: Modern Hebrew. In G. Khan (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Hebrew language and linguistics. Leiden: Brill. Google Scholar
  26. Borer, H. (2013). Structuring sense, vol. 3: taking form. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Borer, H., & Wexler, K. (1987). The maturation of syntax. In T. Roeper & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter setting, studies in theoretical psycholinguistics. Berlin: Springer. Google Scholar
  28. Božič, J. (2017). Non-local allomorphy in a strictly local system. Ms., McGill University. lingbuzz/003469. Google Scholar
  29. Bresnan, J. (2001). Explaining morphosyntactic competition. In M. Baltin & C. Collins (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary syntactic theory (pp. 1–44). Oxford: Blackwell Sci. Google Scholar
  30. Bruening, B. (2013). By phrases in passives and nominals. Syntax, 16, 1–41. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Bruening, B. (2014). Word formation is syntactic: adjectival passives in English. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 32(2), 363–422. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Burzio, L. (1986). Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Reidel. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Calabrese, A. (2015). Irregular morphology and athematic verbs in Italo-Romance. Isogloss, 70, 69–102, doi: 10.5565/rev/isogloss.17. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (2001). Grammatically conditioned allomorphy, paradigmatic structure, and the Ancestry Constraint. Transactions of the Philological Society, 99(2), 223–245. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton. Google Scholar
  36. Doron, E. (2000). Habeynoni hasavil [the passive participle]. Balshanut Ivrit [Hebrew Linguistics], 47, 39–62. Google Scholar
  37. Doron, E. (2003). Agency and voice: the semantics of the Semitic templates. Natural Language Semantics, 11, 1–67. doi: 10.1023/A:1023021423453. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Doron, E. (2013). Participle: modern Hebrew. In G. Khan (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Hebrew language and linguistics. Leiden: Brill. Google Scholar
  39. Doron, E. (2014). The interaction of adjectival passive and voice. In A. Alexiadou, H. Borer, & F. Schäfer (Eds.), The syntax of roots and the roots of syntax (pp. 164–191). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Dunbar, E., & Wellwood, A. (2016). Addressing the ‘two interface’ problem: comparatives and superlatives. Glossa, 1(1), 5, doi: 10.5334/gjgl.9. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Embick, D. (2000). Features, syntax and categories in the Latin perfect. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(2), 185–230. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Embick, D. (2004). On the structure of resultative participles in English. Linguistic Inquiry, 35(3), 355–392. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Embick, D. (2007). Blocking effects and analytic/synthetic alternations. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 25, 1–37. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Embick, D. (2010). Localism versus globalism in morphology and phonology. Linguistic inquiry monographs (Vol. 60). Cambridge: MIT Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Embick, D., & Marantz, A. (2008). Architecture and blocking. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(1), 1–53. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Embick, D., & Noyer, R. (2001). Movement operations after syntax. Linguistic Inquiry, 32(4), 555–595. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Fanselow, G., & Féry, C. (2002). Ineffability in grammar. In G. Fanselow & C. Féry (Eds.), Resolving conflicts in grammars: optimality theory in syntax, morphology, and phonology, Linguistische Berichte (Vol. 11, pp. 265–307). Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag. Google Scholar
  48. Folli, R., & Harley, H. (2008). Teleology and animacy in external arguments. Lingua, 118, 190–202. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Frampton, J. (2001). The amn’t gap, ineffability, and anomalous aren’t: against morphosyntactic competition. In Proceedings of CLS (Vol. 37). Google Scholar
  50. Freese, J. W. (1843). The Latin governess: a manual of instruction in the elements of Latin. London: Mills and Son. Google Scholar
  51. Gildersleeve, B., & Lodge, G. (1903). Latin grammar (3rd ed.). London: Macmillan. Google Scholar
  52. Giorgi, A., & Pianesi, F. (1991). Toward a syntax of temporal representations. Probus, 3, 187–213. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Goldsmith, J. A. (1976). Autosegmental phonology. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge: MIT. Google Scholar
  54. Gorman, K., & Yang, C. (to appear). When nobody wins. In F. Rainer, F. Gardani, H. C. Luschützky, & W. U. Dressler (Eds.), Competition in inflection and word formation. Dordrecht: Springer. Google Scholar
  55. Gouskova, M., & Ahn, S. (2016). Sublexical phonotactics and english comparatives. Ms., New York University. Google Scholar
  56. Halle, M. (1973). Prolegomena to a theory of word formation. Linguistic Inquiry, 4(1), 3–16. Google Scholar
  57. Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20: essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger (pp. 111–176). Cambridge: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  58. Harbour, D. (2003). Some outstanding problems of Yimas. Transactions of the Philological Society, 101(1), 125–136. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Harley, H. (2014a). On the identity of roots. Theoretical Linguistics, 40(3/4), 225–276. Google Scholar
  60. Harley, H. (2014b). Reply to commentaries “On the identity of roots”. Theoretical Linguistics, 40(3/4), 447–474. Google Scholar
  61. Haugen, J. D., & Siddiqi, D. (2013). Roots and the derivation. Linguistic Inquiry, 44(3), 493–517. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Hazout, I. (1995). Action nominalizations and the lexicalist hypothesis. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 13, 355–404. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Higgins, E. T. (1977). The varying presuppositional nature of comparatives. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 6(3), 203–222. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Hippisley, A. (2007). Declarative deponency: a network morphology account of morphological mismatches. In M. Baerman, G. Corbett, D. Brown, & A. Hippisley (Eds.), Deponency and morphological mismatches (pp. 145–173). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  65. Horvath, J., & Siloni, T. (2008). Active lexicon: adjectival and verbal passives. In S. Armon-Lotem, G. Danon, & S. Rothstein (Eds.), Current issues in generative Hebrew linguistics (pp. 105–134). Amsterdam: Benjamins. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Horvath, J., & Siloni, T. (2009). Hebrew idioms: the organization of the lexical component. Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 1, 283–310. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Kallulli, D. (2013). (Non-)canonical passives and reflexives: deponents and their like. In A. Alexiadou & F. Schäfer (Eds.), Non-canonical passives, linguistik aktuell/linguistics today (Vol. 205, pp. 337–358). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Google Scholar
  68. Kalvana, A., & Lokmane, I. (2010). Defective paradigms of reflexive nouns and participles in Latvian. In M. Baerman, G. Corbett, & D. Brown (Eds.), Proceedings of the British Academy: Vol. 163. Defective paradigms: missing forms and what they tell us (pp. 53–68). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  69. Kastner, I. (2016). Form and meaning in the Hebrew verb. Ph.D. thesis, New York University, New York, NY. lingbuzz/003028. Google Scholar
  70. Kastner, I. (2017). Templatic morphology as an emergent property: roots and functional heads in Hebrew. Ms., Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin. lingbuzz/003103. Google Scholar
  71. Kastner, I. (to appear). Reflexive verbs in Hebrew: deep unaccusativity meets lexical semantics. Glossa. Google Scholar
  72. Kennedy, C. (2007). Vagueness and grammar: the semantics of relative and absolute gradable adjectives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30(1), 1–48. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Kiparsky, P. (2005). Blocking and periphrasis in inflectional paradigms. In Yearbook of Morphology, (Vol. 204, pp. 113–135). Google Scholar
  74. Kiparsky, P. (2010). Dvandvas, blocking, and the associative: the bumpy ride from phrase to word. Language, 86(2), 302–331. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Kramer, R. (2014). Clitic doubling or object agreement: the view from Amharic. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 32, 593–634. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Kratzer, A. (2000). Building statives. In L. J. Conathan, J. Good, D. Kavitskaya, A. B. Wulf, & A. C. Yu (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-sixth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, (pp. 385–399). University of California, Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley. Google Scholar
  77. Laks, L. (2011). Morpho-phonological and morpho-thematic relations in Hebrew and Arabic verb formation. Ph.D. thesis, Tel Aviv University, Tel-Aviv. Google Scholar
  78. Laks, L. (2014). Morpho-thematic mismatches in Hebrew: what happens when morphological and thematic criteria collide? Lingua, 138, 107–127. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Legendre, G. (2009). The neutralization approach to ineffability in syntax. In C. Rice & S. Blaho (Eds.), Modeling ungrammaticality in optimality theory. London: Equinox. Google Scholar
  80. Legendre, G., Smolensky, P., & Wilson, C. (1998). When is less more? Faithfulness and minimal links in wh-chains. In P. Barbosa, D. Fox, P. Hagstrom, M. McGinnis, & D. Pesetsky (Eds.), Is the best good enough? Optimality and competition in syntax (pp. 249–289). Cambridge: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  81. Levin, B., & Rappaport, M. (1986). The formation of adjectival passives. Linguistic Inquiry, 17(4), 623–661. Google Scholar
  82. Lieb, H. H. (2005). Notions of paradigm in grammar. In D. A. Cruse, F. Hundsnurscher, M. Job , & P. Lutzeier (Eds.), Lexikologie/Lexicology: ein internationales Handbuch zur Natur und Struktur von Wörtern und Wortschätzen/An international handbook on the nature and structure of words and vocabularies, Handbücher zur Sprach- und Kommunikationswissenschaft (Vol. 2, pp. 1613–1646). Berlin: de Gruyter. Google Scholar
  83. Löwenadler, J. (2010). Restrictions on productivity: defectiveness in Swedish adjective paradigms. Morphology, 20, 71–107. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Marantz, A. (2013). Locality domains for contextual allomorphy across the interfaces. In A. Marantz & O. Matushansky (Eds.), Distributed morphology today: morphemes for Morris Halle (pp. 95–115). Cambridge: MIT Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Matushansky, O. (2006). Head movement in linguistic theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 37(1), 69–109. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Matushansky, O. (2013). More or better: on the derivation of synthetic comparatives and superlatives in English. In A. Marantz & O. Matushansky (Eds.), Distributed morphology today: morphemes for Morris Halle (pp. 59–78). Cambridge: MIT Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. McCarthy, J. J. (1981). A prosodic theory of nonconcatenative morphology. Linguistic Inquiry, 12, 373–418. Google Scholar
  88. McCarthy, J. J., & Pater, J. (2016). Harmonic grammar and harmonic serialism. Sheffield: Equinox. Google Scholar
  89. McCarthy, J. J., & Prince, A. S. (1990). Prosodic morphology and templatic morphology. In M. Eid & J. J. McCarthy (Eds.), Perspectives on Arabic linguistics (Vol. 2, pp. 1–54). Amsterdam: Benjamins. Google Scholar
  90. McIntyre, A. (2013). Adjectival passives and adjectival participles in English. In A. Alexiadou & F. Schäfer (Eds.), Non-canonical passives (pp. 21–42). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Meltzer-Asscher, A. (2011). Adjectival passives in Hebrew: evidence for parallelism between the adjectival and verbal systems. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 29(3), 815–855. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Merchant, J. (2015). How much context is enough? Two cases of span-conditioned stem allomorphy. Linguistic Inquiry, 46(2), 273–303. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Miller, D. G. (2010). The mediopassive: Latin to Romance. In Language change and linguistic theory (Vol. 2). Clarendon: Oxford University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Moreland, F. L., & Fleischer, R. M. (1977). Latin: an intensive course. Berkeley: University of California Press. Google Scholar
  95. Müller, G. (2011). Syncretism without underspecification in optimality theory: the role of leading forms. Word Structure, 4(1), 53–103. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Müller, G. (2013). Approaches to deponency. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7(6), 351–369. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Nevins, A. (2015). The exponent list and the encyclopedic list: structurally parallel, mutually mute. In Roots IV. New York: New York University. Google Scholar
  98. Oltra Massuet, I. (1999). On the notion of theme vowel: a new approach to Catalan verbal morphology. Master’s thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA. Google Scholar
  99. Pertsova, K. (2016). Transderivational relations and paradigm gaps in Russian verbs. Glossa, 1(1), 13 doi: 10.5334/gjgl.59. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Pesetsky, D. (1982). Paths and categories. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA. Google Scholar
  101. Platzack, C., & Rosengren, I. (1998). On the subject of imperatives: a minimalist account of the imperative clause. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics, 1(3), 177–224. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (1993/2004). Optimality theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar. Malden: Blackwell. Revision of 1993, technical report, Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science. ROA 537. Google Scholar
  103. Reinhart, T., & Siloni, T. (2005). The lexicon-syntax parameter: reflexivization and other arity operations. Linguistic Inquiry, 36(3), 389–436. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Rice, C. (2005). Optimal gaps in optimal paradigms. Catalan Journal of Linguistics, 4, 155–170. Google Scholar
  105. Sadler, L., & Spencer, A. (2001). Syntax as an exponent of morphological features. In Yearbook of Morphology (Vol. 2000, pp. 71–96). Google Scholar
  106. Schäfer, F. (2008). The syntax of (anti-)causatives. Amsterdam: Benjamins. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Sichel, I. (2009). New evidence for the structural realization of the implicit external argument in nominalizations. Linguistic Inquiry, 40(4), 712–723. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Spathas, G., Alexiadou, A., & Schäfer, F. (2015). Middle voice and reflexive interpretations: afto-prefixation in Greek. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 33, 1293–1350. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Spencer, A. (2003). Periphrastic paradigms in Bulgarian. In U. Junghanns & L. Szucsich (Eds.), Syntactic structures and morphological information, interface explorations (Vol. 7, pp. 249–282). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Google Scholar
  110. Stump, G. T. (2001). Inflectional morphology: a paradigm structure approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Stump, G. T. (2006). Heteroclisis and paradigm linkage. Language, 82, 279–322. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Svenonius, P. (2004). Slavic prefixes inside and outside VP. Nordlyd, 32(2), 205–253. Google Scholar
  113. Trommer, J. (2001). Distributed optimality. PhD thesis, University of Potsdam, Potsdam. Google Scholar
  114. Tucker, M. A. (2015). Your roots are showing: on root-sensitive allomorphy in Arabic. In Roots IV. New York: New York University. Google Scholar
  115. Ussishkin, A. (2005). A fixed prosodic theory of nonconcatenative templatic morphology. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 23, 169–218. doi: 10.1007/s11049-003-7790-8. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Wasow, T. (1977). Transformations and the lexicon. In P. Culicover, T. Wasow, & A. Akmajian (Eds.), Formal syntax. New York: Academic Press. Google Scholar
  117. Wood, J. (2015). Icelandic morphosyntax and argument structure. Studies in natural language and linguistic theory: Vol. 90. Berlin: Springer. Google Scholar
  118. Wunderlich, D. (2001). How gaps and substitutions can become optimal: the pronominal affix paradigms of Yimas. Transactions of the Philological Society, 99(2), 315–366. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Yang, C. (2016). The price of linguistic productivity. Cambridge: MIT Press. Google Scholar
  120. Zanuttini, R., Pak, M., & Portner, P. (2012). A syntactic analysis of interpretive restrictions on imperative, promissive, and exhortative subjects. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 30(4), 1231–1274. CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Humboldt-Universität zu BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.New York UniversityNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations