Sex Roles

, Volume 62, Issue 7–8, pp 532–544 | Cite as

How Nice of Us and How Dumb of Me: The Effect of Exposure to Benevolent Sexism on Women’s Task and Relational Self-Descriptions

  • Manuela Barreto
  • Naomi Ellemers
  • Laura Piebinga
  • Miguel Moya
Original Article

Abstract

This research demonstrates how women assimilate to benevolent sexism by emphasizing their relational qualities and de-emphasizing their task-related characteristics when exposed to benevolent sexism. Studies 1 (N = 62) and 2 (N = 100) show, with slightly different paradigms and measures, that compared to exposure to hostile sexism, exposure to benevolent sexism increases the extent to which female Dutch college students define themselves in relational terms and decreases the extent to which they emphasize their task-related characteristics. Study 3 (N = 79) demonstrates that benevolent sexism has more pernicious effects when it is expressed by someone with whom women expect to collaborate than when no collaboration is expected with the source of sexism. The implications of these results are discussed.

Keywords

Benevolent sexism Women’s self-descriptions Task-related and relational self-descriptions Stereotype confirmation 

References

  1. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  2. Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2005). The burden of benevolent sexism: how it contributes to the maintenance of gender inequalities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 633–642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barreto, M., Ryan, M., & Schmitt, M. (2009). Introduction: Is the glass ceiling still relevant in the 21st century? In M. Barreto, M. Ryan & M. Schmitt (Eds.), The glass ceiling in the 21st century: Understanding barriers to gender equality. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper, M., & Bouvrette, A. (2003). Contingencies of self-worth in college students: theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 894–908.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Dardenne, B., Dumont, M., & Bollier, T. (2007). Insidious dangers of benevolent sexism: consequences for women’s performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 764–779.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1993). Are people prejudiced against women? Some answers from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes, and judgments of competence. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology, 5, 1–35.Google Scholar
  7. Eccles, J. S., Barber, B., & Jozefowicz, D. (1999). Linking gender to educational, occupational, and recreational choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choices. In W. B. Swann Jr., J. H. Langlois & L. Albino Gilbert (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society: The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence. Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  8. Eurobarometer (2008). Discrimination in the European Union (Country fiche: The Netherlands). Retrieved at http://ec.europa.eu/public_opinion/archives/eb_special_en.htm
  9. Fiske, S. T., Xu, J., Cuddy, A. C., & Glick, P. (1999). (Dis)respecting versus (Dis)liking: status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 473–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 491–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Jackman, M. R. (1994). The velvet glove: Paternalism and conflict in gender, class, and race relations. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  14. Jennings, J., Geis, F. L., & Brown, V. (1980). Influence of television commercials on women’s self-confidence and independent judgement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 203–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jost, J. T., & Kay, A. C. (2005). Exposure to benevolent sexism and complementary gender stereotypes: consequences for specific and diffuse forms of system justification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 498–509.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Killianski, S. E., & Rudman, L. (1998). Wanting it both ways: do women approve of benevolent sexism? Sex Roles, 39, 333–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Konrad, A., Ritchie, J. E., Jr., Lieb, P., & Corrigall, E. (2000). Sex differences and similarities in job attribute preferences: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 593–641.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Kray, L. J., Thompson, L., & Galinsky, A. (2001). Battle of the sexes: gender stereotype confirmation and reactance in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 942–958.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Merens, A., & Hermans, B. (2008). Emancipatiemonitor 2008. CBS/SCP.Google Scholar
  20. Moya, M., Glick, P., Expósito, F., de Lemus, S., & Hart, J. (2007). It’s for your own good: benevolent sexism and women’s reactions to protectively justified restrictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1421–1434.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Powell, G. N. (1999). Reflections on the glass ceiling: Recent trends and future prospects. In G. N. Powell (Ed.), Handbook of Gender and Work (pp. 325–346). Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  22. Rudman, L. A., & Heppes, J. B. (2003). Implicit romantic fantasies and women’s interest in personal power: a glass slipper effect? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1357–1370.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Sinclair, S., & Lun, J. (2006). Significant other representations activate stereotypic self-views among women. Self and Identity, 5, 196–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Sinclair, S., Huntsinger, J., Skorinko, J., & Hardin, C. (2005). Social tuning of the self: consequences of the self-evaluations of stereotype targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 160–175.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Vescio, T. K., Gervais, S. J., Snyder, M., & Hoover, A. (2005). Power and the creation of patronizing environments: the stereotype-based behaviours or the powerful and their effects on female performance in masculine domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 658–672.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Von Baeyer, C. L., Sherk, D. L., & Zanna, M. P. (1981). Impression management in the job interview: when the female applicant meets the male (chauvinist) interviewer. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 45–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. World Economic Forum. The global gender gap report 2007. Retrieved at www.weforum.org
  29. Zanna, M. P., & Pack, S. J. (1975). On the self-fulfilling nature of apparent sex differences in behaviour. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 583–591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Manuela Barreto
    • 1
  • Naomi Ellemers
    • 2
  • Laura Piebinga
    • 2
  • Miguel Moya
    • 3
  1. 1.Centre for Social Research and Intervention (CIS), Av das Forças Armadas-Ed ISCTELisbonPortugal
  2. 2.Leiden Institute for Psychological ResearchLeiden UniversityLeidenthe Netherlands
  3. 3.University of GranadaGranadaSpain

Personalised recommendations