Skip to main content
Log in

Strategy effects on word searching in Japanese letter fluency tests: evidence from the NIRS findings

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Strategy effects on word searching in the Japanese letter fluency test were investigated using the Near-infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS). Participants were given a Japanese letter fluency test and they were classified into two types of strategy users, based on analysis of their recorded verbal responses. One group, AIUEO-order strategy users, employed sequential word searching based upon the imagery of the AIUEO-table which they mastered when young, and the other group, semantic strategy users, searched for the next word mainly based on semantic relations. The blood-flow volumes (OxyHb) measured of the AIUEO-order strategy users were greater than for the semantic strategy users in the frontal parts of the brain, and the blood-flow was more active in the right than in the left part of the brain. These different activation patterns suggest that the NIRS might be useful new brain imaging technique for examination of psychological process in verbal behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldo, J. V., Schwartz, S., Wilkins, D., & Dronkers, N. F. (2006). Role of frontal versus temporal cortex in verbal fluency as revealed by voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 896–900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbeart, M. (1995). Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recording. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 434–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cai, Q., Lavidor, M., Brysbeart, M., Pauligan, Y., & Nazir, T. A. (2008). Cerebral lateralization of frontal lobe language processes and lateralization of the posterior visual word processing system. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences, 20, 372–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charlot, V., Tzourio, N., Zilbovicious, M., & Mazoyer, B. M. (1992). Different mental imagery abilities result in different regional cerebral blood flow activation patterns during cognitive tasks. Neuropsychologia, 30, 565–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Akhavein, R. (1995). Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J. (1984). The neural basis of mental imagery: A componential analysis. Cognition, 18, 245–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J. (1995). The neural basis of mental imagery. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 963–975). Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fossati, P., Harvey, P. O., Le Bastard, G., Ergis, A. M., Jouvent, R., & Allilaire, J. F. (2003). Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency in depression. Psychiatry Research, 117, 17–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, J. A., Binder, J. R., Springer, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S. F., Rao, S. M., et al. (1999). Language processing is strongly left lateralized in both sexes: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 122, 199–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gourpvitch, M. L., Kirkby, B. S., Goldberg, T. E., Weinberger, D. R., Gold, J. M., Esposito, G., et al. (2000). A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology, 14, 353–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatta, T. (1978). Visual field differences in a mental transformation task. Neuropsychologia, 16, 637–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatta, T., Koike, M., & Langman, P. (1994). Laterality of mental imagery generation and operation: Tests with brain-damaged patients and normal adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 577–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, M. J., Ehlis, A.-C., & Fallgatter, A. J. (2003). Frontal activation during a verbal-fluency task as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Brain Research Bulletin, 61, 51–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirshorn, E. A., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2006). Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: Neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2547–2557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoshi, Y., Oda, I., Wada, Y., Ito, Y., Yamashita, Y., Oda, M., et al. (2000). Visuospatial imagery is a fruitful strategy for the digit span backward task: A study with near-infrared optical tomography. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 339–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoshi, Y., & Tamura, M. (1997). Near-infrared optical detection of sequential brain activation in the prefrontal cortex during mental tasks. NeuroImage, 5, 292–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, E., & Hatta, T. (2002). Development of the verbal fluency test for Japanese. Studies in Informatics and Sciences, 15, 81–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2003). Semantic processing of Arabic, Kanji, and Kana numbers: Evidence from interference in physical and numerical size judgments. Memory and Cognition, 31, 360–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32, 662–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jobsis, F. F. (1977). Noninvasive, infrared monitoring of cerebral and myocardial oxygen sufficiency and circulatory parameters. Science, 198, 1264–1274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanari, A., & Hatta, T. (2007). Mechanisms of word generation in the Japanese letter fluency test: Does Japanese employ a specific strategy? Journal of Human Environmental Studies, 5, 7–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1996). Image and brain (pp. 315–317). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, C., Dehaene, S., Pappata, S., Jobert, A., Bottelaneder, M., Roumenov, D., et al. (2007). Dynamics of prefrontal and cingulate activity during a reward-based logical deduction task. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 749–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. D. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Hodges, J. R., & Wise, R. J. (1996). Generating ‘tiger’ as an animal name or a word beginning with T: Differences in brain activation. Proceedings of Biological Sciences/The Royal Society, 263, 989–995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., Hyder, F., Blamire, A. M., & Shulman, R. G. (1997). FMRI of the prefrontal cortex during overt verbal fluency. Neuroreport, 8, 561–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, M., & Dehaene, S. (2004). From number neurons to mental arithmetic: The cognitive neuroscience of number senses. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 865–875). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Deus, J., Kulisevsky, J., et al. (1996). Frontal lobe activation during word generation studied by functional MRI. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 93, 403–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raskin, S. A., Sliwinski, M., & Borod, J. C. (1992). Clustering strategies on tasks of verbal fluency in Parkinson’s disease. Nuropsychologia, 30, 95–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rende, B., Ramsberger, G., & Miyake, A. (2002). Commonalities and differences in the working memory components underlying letter and category fluency tasks: A dual-task investigation. Neuropsychology, 16, 309–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robert, P. H., Lafont, V., Medecin, I., Berthet, L., Thauby, S., Baudu, C., et al. (1998). Clustering and switching strategies in verbal fluency tasks: Comparison between schizophrenics and healthy adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 539–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., & Levin, H. S. (1997). The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and Language, 57, 394–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saito, K., & Watanabe, S. (2006). Spatial memory activation of the parietal cortex measured with near-infrared spectroscopic imaging in the finger-maze of the Morris water maze analogue for humans. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 17, 227–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlosser, R., Hutchinson, M., Joseffer, S., Rusinek, H., Saarimaki, A., Stevenson, J., et al. (1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity in a verbal fluency task. Journal of Neurology and Neurosurgical Psychiatry, 64, 492–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroeter, M. L., Zysset, S., Wahl, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development – An event-related fNIRS study. Neuroimage, 23, 1317–1325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S., Baldo, J., Graves, R. E., & Brugger, P. (2003). Pervasive influence of semantics in letter and category fluency: A multidimensional approach. Brain and Language, 87, 400–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. A. (1998). Compendium of neuropsychological tests (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troyer, A. K. (2000). Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 370–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11, 138–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villringer, A., & Chance, B. (1997). Non-invasive optical spectroscopy and imaging of human brain function. Trends in Cognitive Neurosciences, 20, 435–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, E. M., Hofer, A., Golaszewski, S., Siedentopf, C., Brinkhoff, C., Kremser, C., et al. (2004). Brain activation patterns during a verbal fluency test – A functional MRI study in healthy volunteers and patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 70, 287–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Takeshi Hatta.

Appendix

Appendix

Japanese syllabary table

ん (nn)

わ (wa)

ら (ra)

や (ya)

ま (ma)

は (ha)

な (na)

た (ta)

さ (sa)

か (ka)

あ (a)

  

り (r i)

 

み (mi)

ひ (hi)

に (ni)

ち (t i)

し (si)

き (ki)

い (i)

  

る (ru)

ゆ (yu)

む (mu)

ふ (hu)

ぬ (nu)

つ (tu)

す (su)

く (ku)

う (u)

  

れ (re)

 

め (me)

へ (he)

ね (ne)

て (t e)

せ (se)

け (ke)

え (e)

 

を (wo)

ろ (ro)

よ (yo)

も (mo)

ほ (ho)

の (no)

と (to)

そ (so)

こ (ko)

お (o)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hatta, T., Kanari, A., Mase, M. et al. Strategy effects on word searching in Japanese letter fluency tests: evidence from the NIRS findings. Read Writ 22, 1041–1052 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9143-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9143-9

Keywords

Navigation