Skip to main content
Log in

Benford’s Law and Number Selection in Fixed-Odds Numbers Game

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In fixed-odds numbers games, the prizes and the odds of winning are known at the time of placement of the wager. Both players and operators are subject to the vagaries of luck in such games. Most game operators limit their liability exposure by imposing a sales limit on the bets received for each bet type, at the risk of losing the rejected bets to the underground operators. This raises a question—how should the game operator set the appropriate sales limit? We argue that the choice of the sales limit is intimately related to the ways players select numbers to bet on in the games. There are ample empirical evidences suggesting that players do not choose all numbers with equal probability, but have a tendency to bet on (small) numbers that are closely related to events around them (e.g., birth dates, addresses, etc.). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first paper to quantify this phenomenon and examine its relation to the classical Benford’s law. We use this connection to develop a choice model, and propose a method to set the appropriate sales limit in these games.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Notes

  1. The BBC news article on this can be accessed at http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/uk/6174648.stm.

  2. In fact, China Mobile’s Jiangxi branch held an auction to sell a “lucky” phone number recently, and one such number-with six consecutive eights—was sold for RMB 44,000!

  3. Interestingly, our data suggests that players in Pennsylvania have an aversion to the digit 2, but favor digits 7 and 8.

  4. Note that this simplifying assumption may not hold in general, as some players may pad the numbers with trailing zeros, and some may simply duplicate the numbers to reach a 3-digit number. Halpern and Devereaux (1989) mentioned that triplets like 111 or 888 are very popular in the Pick-3 game in Pennsylvania. Unfortunately, it does not appear possible to incorporate such features into the model, without sacrificing the simplicity and tractability of the calibration model.

References

  • Benford, F. (1938). The law of anomalous numbers. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 78, 551–572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chernoff, H. (1999). How to beat the Massachusetts number game: An application of some basic ideas in probability and statistics. The Mathematical Intelligencer, 3, 166–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haigh, J. (1997). The statistics of national lottery. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series A , 160(2), 187–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, A. R., & Devereaux, S. D. (1989). Lucky numbers: Choice strategies in the Pennsylvania daily number game. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27(2), 167–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardoon, K., Baboushkin, H., Gupta, R., Powell, G. J., & Derevensky, J. L. (1997). Underlying cognitions in the selection of lottery tickets. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, Toronto, Ontario, June.

  • Henze, N. (1997). A statistical and probabilistic analysis of popular lottery tickets. Statistica Neerlandica, 51(2), 155–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, J., Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. L. (1998). Children’s cognitive perceptions of 6/ 49 lottery tickets. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14, 227–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, T. (1995a). Base-invariance implies benford’s law. Proceedings of the American Mathematical Society, 123, 887–895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, T. P. (1995b). A statistical derivation of the significant-digit law. Statistical Science, 10, 354–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, T. P. (1998). The first digit phenomenon. The American Scientist, 10(4), 354–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladouceur, R., Dube, D., Giroux, I., Legendre, N., & Gaudet, C. (1996). Cognitive biases and playing behavior on American roulette and the 6/49 lottery. Unpublished manuscript. Québec: Universite Laval.

  • Ladouceur, R., & Walker, M. (1996). A cognitive perspective on gambling. In P. M. Salkovskis (Ed.), Trends in cognitive and behavioral therapies (pp. 89–120). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lafaille, J. M., & Simonis, G. (2005). Dissected re-assembled: An analysis of gaming.

  • Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ley, E. (1996). On the peculiar distribution of the U.S. stock indexes’ digits. American Statistician, 50, 311–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., & Brugger, P. (2007). Exploring number space by random digit generation. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 655–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, S. (1881). Note on the frequency of use of the different digits in natural numbers. American Journal of Mathematician, 4, 39–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigrini, M. J. (1996). A taxpayer compliance application of benford’s law. Journal of the American Taxation Association, 18, 72–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raimi, R. (1976). The first digit problem. The American Mathematical Monthly, 83, 887–895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatte, P. (1988). On mantissa distributions in computing and benford’s law. Journal of Information Processing and Cybernetics, 24, 443–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. (1999). An analysis of the distribution of combinations chosen by UK national lottery players. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 17(3), 243–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, H., & Cover, T. M. (1989). Maximum entropy and the lottery. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 84, 980–985.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teo, C. P., & Leong, S. M. (2002). Managing risk in a four digit number game. SIAM Review 44(4), 601–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, J. K. (1989). Unusual patterns in reported earnings. The Accounting Review, 64, 773–787.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tijms, H. (2007). Understanding probability: Chance rules in everyday lives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Varian, H. R. (1972). Benford’s law. The American Statistician, 26, 65–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitney, R. E. (1972). Initial digits for the sequence of primes. American Mathematical Monthly, 79, 150–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziemba, W. T., Brumelle, S. L., Gautier, A., & Schwartz, S. L. (1986). Dr. Z’s 6/49 Lotto Guidebook. Vancouvrr and Los Angeles: Dr. Z. Investments. Inc.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chung-Piaw Teo.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chou, M.C., Kong, Q., Teo, CP. et al. Benford’s Law and Number Selection in Fixed-Odds Numbers Game. J Gambl Stud 25, 503–521 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-009-9145-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-009-9145-9

Keywords

Navigation