Introduction

Sex differences in life history, behavior, and physiology are nearly ubiquitous in nature (e.g., Lee 2006; Fairbairn et al. 2007; Restif and Amos 2010; Maklakov and Lummaa 2013; Adler and Bonduriansky 2014). Building on life history theory (Stearns 1992), the pace-of-life syndrome (POLS) framework predicts that life history and consistent behavioral differences (“personalities,” sensu Dall et al. 2004, that differ among individuals creating a behavioral syndrome, sensu Sih et al. 2004) covary together with physiology along a fast-slow continuum across individuals, populations, and species as a result of trade-offs between reproduction and survival or future reproductive rate (Ricklefs and Wikelski 2002; Réale et al. 2010). This is because differences in fitness expectations can result in systematic differences in risk-taking behavior, including traits like aggressiveness, boldness, and exploration, that facilitate the trade-off between current and future reproduction: individuals with high future expectations, and thus much to lose should display risk-adverse behaviors while those with low expectations should invest into current reproduction through risk-prone behavior (Wolf et al. 2007; Réale et al. 2010). Although sex differences have not received much focus in the POLS framework, theories on life history variation, sexual selection, and sexual conflicts suggest that sex differences in the optimal life history strategy also commonly lead to predictable variation along the axis of pace-of-life (Wedell et al. 2006; Maklakov and Lummaa 2013; Adler and Bonduriansky 2014), and these can covary with behavioral differences (Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes). As a consequence, sex differences may even generate POLS at the population level (Fig. S1).

Sex-specific optima for reproductive investment and life history scheduling are a result of a difference in the potential rate of reproduction between the sexes that ultimately stem from anisogamy (Bateman 1948; Maynard Smith 1982; Wedell et al. 2006; Lehtonen et al. 2016). The sex with the higher potential reproductive rate (typically males) (Andersson 1994), and/or a lower expected potential for future reproduction, is predicted to trade off self-maintenance against reproduction—including engaging in more aggressive and risky behaviors, while the slower-reproducing sex (typically females) with a higher potential for future reproduction is thought to experience stronger selection on self-maintenance. Sex differences in the reproductive costs, behavior, and physiology can result in differences in mortality risk and aging between males and females, which themselves provide feedback to sex-specific selection on life histories (Vinogradov 1998; Bonduriansky et al. 2008; Adler and Bonduriansky 2014). Sexual dimorphism in pace-of-life has been clearly demonstrated (e.g., Bonduriansky et al. 2008; Adler and Bonduriansky 2014), and there is also some evidence of systematic sex differences in POLS, whereby males were shown to be the faster sex across traits (e.g., Lovlie et al. 2014; Berger et al. 2014a, b, 2016). It is important to recognize, however, that the ways in which natural and sexual selection operate on each sex depends on the reproductive roles and mating system, breeding strategy, and environment, which influence whether selection is concordant or antagonistic between the sexes. Whether, and in which POLS traits, sex differences evolve is therefore predicted to differ across species, and potentially between populations. We discuss these tenets in our accompanying paper by Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes (see also Tarka et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes).

Most studies on POLS have thus far focused on phenotypic correlations between traits, and without taking into account the potential for sex-specific POLS. However, expanding the approach to consider the genetic basis of POLS in each sex is necessary for answering several important evolutionary questions. The concept of POLS—i.e., covariation between behavioral syndromes and life history trade-offs—itself begs the question of how such a covariation is generated and whether it can evolve. The answer requires knowledge of the genetic architecture of POLS in each sex, in addition to understanding how selection operates on the sexes. Knowledge of the underlying genes also helps to understand how variation in natural populations is mechanistically created, and whether the same genes impact POLS within and across species. Identifying candidate genes can also reveal new traits not considered previously that may be important for POLS evolution through shared mechanistic basis.

Here, we bring together central insights from quantitative and molecular genetics and genomics to discuss how sexual dimorphism can evolve in POLS, owing to the sex-specific genetic architecture that arises from sex differences in gene expression and sex chromosome linkage. We present examples of important neuroendocrine pathways underlying animal personalities and highlight some of their pleiotropic effects on life history and physiological traits, with the aim to identify candidate genes and molecular pathways for future studies to test for the genetic integration underlying POLS. We similarly review the key mechanisms underlying life history trade-offs and discuss the roles of sex steroids in vertebrates and juvenile hormone in insects as regulators of sex differences in POLS. We also discuss the importance of considering morphology as an integral part of POLS due to many mechanistic links between POLS phenotypes and, e.g., body size or color variation. Although not exhaustive, our review aims at covering a broad range of potential mechanisms, to spark ideas and create a foundation for future research directions investigating the role of sex in the evolution of POLS.

Genetic architecture of sex differences

Evolution of multitrait dimorphism in the face of a shared genome

How males and females can evolve differences in the POLS traits is an important theoretical and empirical question. Evolution of sexual dimorphism is complicated by the fact that males and females of all species share the same genes, apart from those residing in the heteromorphic sex chromosomes, such as Y and W. Most homologous traits in the sexes will therefore have a shared genetic basis. Sexually antagonistic selection over optimal expression of a shared locus creates intra-locus sexual conflict (Parker 1979; Rice and Chippindale 2001; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Bonduriansky and Chenoweth 2009; Connallon and Clark 2014). This conflict can be resolved or at least mitigated by the evolution of sexual dimorphism, moving one or both sexes closer to their sex-specific adaptive optima (Lande 1980; Fairbairn et al. 2007; Bonduriansky and Chenoweth 2009; Connallon and Clark 2014; Connallon and Clark 2010). The amount of sex-specific genetic variance and the strength of the genetic covariance between the sexes will influence how readily sexual dimorphism can evolve (Lande 1980). Many homologous (and even non-homologous) traits show a strong positive genetic correlation between males and females suggesting that genetic constraints preventing phenotypic sexual divergence in the face of sexually antagonistic selection are common (Cox and Calsbeek 2009; Poissant et al. 2009).

Before turning to the genomic and molecular underpinnings of how the sexes may break the genetic correlation, we will discuss the complexity that necessarily accompanies evolution of POLS—genetic covariances between traits. Sexual dimorphism in POLS traits evolves largely as a result of sex-specific selection on single traits and/or on their correlations (Wyman et al. 2013; see Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes for sources of sex-specific selection on POLS). Trait correlations can be created and altered in strength and directionality by correlational selection (e.g., Cheverud 1984; Kingsolver and Wiernasz 1987; Brodie 1989, 1992; Lynch and Walsh 1998; Sinervo et al. 2000; Svensson et al. 2001). Genetic trait correlations can be formed by pleiotropy or physical linkage, but adaptive combinations of alleles that would otherwise be broken down by recombination can be maintained by strong correlational selection even in unlinked loci. Frequency-dependent correlational selection can act to maintain variation in multitrait phenotypes (Sinervo and Svensson 2002) such as POLS. If (correlational) selection on POLS traits is sex-specific, this can result in sex differences in within-sex covariances between the traits—in strength and orientation—(Fig. S1), potentially even without a change in the trait means between the sexes. It may even be possible that POLS exists only in one sex, or that completely different traits form the syndrome in each sex due to fundamentally different trade-offs (Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes). One important consequence of sex differences in the means along the axes of life history, behavioral, and physiological traits is that they can form a positive covariation between these traits across the sexes: sex may be the main cause for POLS at the population level (Fig. S1, see also Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes).

Recognizing the two different levels at which variation in POLS traits can occur—within and between the sexes—is important because they may or may not align (Fig. S1). Consistent trait covariances between and within the sexes could arise due to the sexes evolving along the same trajectory as the trait covariances within sexes (for an analogous mechanism proposed at population level see Sokal 1978; Scheiner and Istock 1991). This is perhaps the most likely scenario given that sex-specific covariances can be constrained on multiple levels; by intra-locus conflict, just like evolution of mean differences, but also by physiological, developmental, and genetic constraints that govern trade-offs underlying the patterns of trait covariances, which may be harder to break by selection on one sex alone. Indeed, phenotypic traits are not varying as separate units, but are integrated in trait networks through genetic, developmental, physiological, and functional interactions (Arnold 1992; Armbruster et al. 2014), forming the conceptual basis for POLS theory (Ricklefs and Wikelski 2002; Réale et al. 2010). Although substantially more intricate, a multivariate view more closely reflects the true biological complexity of the genetic architecture and evolution of phenotypes (Walsh and Blows 2009), and also the evolutionary dynamics of multivariate sexual dimorphism (Lande 1980; Wyman et al. 2013). Therefore, evolution of multitrait phenotypes depends not only on the amount of additive genetic variance but also on the strength and directionality of additive genetic covariances between traits (together called the genetic (co)variance matrix or the G-matrix (e.g., Lynch and Walsh 1998) and the strength and directionality of multivariate selection acting on the G-matrix (Lande and Arnold 1983). The direction of selection will matter, because there might not be equal amount of additive genetic variance in all directions of the multivariate character space, restricting the directions in which traits and trait combinations can respond to selection, i.e., evolve (Schluter 1996; Hansen and Houle 2008; for visualization see Fig. 1 in Teplitsky et al. 2014).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Examples of candidate genes and molecular pathways (highlighted with different letters and colors) that influence multiple traits associated with POLS, with evidence for sex specificity in gene action and/or function. See Tables 1 and 2 for species, description of effects, and references

Formally, the G-matrix can be broken down into sex-specific G-matrices (Gmale and Gfemale), each consisting of genetic variances and covariances of traits within each sex, and the cross-sex genetic trait covariances (called the B-matrix) (Lande 1980; Reeve and Fairbairn 1996; Gosden et al. 2012; Wyman et al. 2013). Both the sex-specific G-matrices and the B-matrix will together influence the speed and direction of the evolutionary response to multivariate selection in each sex (Lande 1980; Gosden et al. 2012; Wyman et al. 2013). Strong, positive cross-sex genetic covariances between traits (i.e., in the B-matrix) will mainly constrain evolution of mean differences between sexes and strong, positive trait covariances within sexes (i.e., the Gmale and Gfemale) will constrain changes in the trait means within sexes. Recent studies exemplify these tenets in terms of the evolution POLS in a seed beetle Callosobruchus maculatus, where sexually antagonistic selection operates on an integrated phenotype (metabolic rate, lifespan, behavioral activity and body weight) favoring a “fast” life history strategy in males and a “slow” in females. Artificial male-limited selection on lifespan revealed an intra-locus sexual conflict due to genetic correlations between the traits within and between the sexes. In addition to these constraints, evolutionary response in activity levels in the direction of male-specific selection (i.e., for higher levels) was limited by lack of sufficient additive genetic variance with consequences on sex differences in POLS trait covariance (Berg and Maklakov 2012; Berger et al. 2014a, b). The cross-sex covariances can even cause evolutionary response in the opposite direction to that of selection: In great reed warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus), for example, wing length is under sexually antagonistic selection and despite higher fitness in females with shorter wings, female wing length is expected to increase due to a correlated response to selection in males (Tarka et al. 2014).

Given these constraints, how can sexual dimorphism be so commonplace and how do we expect it to evolve in POLS? There are a number of mechanisms that may facilitate the process. Tightly integrated traits, as predicted by the POLS framework, can more readily respond to selection as a unit, so that when selection acts on one trait, a correlated response is obtained in coadapted traits (Futuyma 2010). However, this alone does not circumvent the putative constraint from strong genetic covariances between the sexes (Poissant et al. 2009; Gosden et al. 2012). It should be noted that constraints are not usually absolute, and as long as the genetic correlation deviates from unity and there is sufficient genetic variation for the trait, sexual dimorphism will evolve, albeit slowly. Another way to circumvent constrains from the genetic correlation between sexes is through sex differences in the amount of additive genetic variance (e.g., found for aggression and exploration in crickets: Han and Dingemanse 2017). Even when under the same strength and direction of selection, the sex with more additive genetic variance will evolve faster than the sex with less additive genetic variance (even if the genetic correlation is 1), leading to evolution of dimorphism (Lande 1980; Cheverud 1984; Wyman and Rowe 2014). This suggests that in addition to sources of sex-specific selection, mechanisms that lower the cross-sex covariance and create differences in the additive genetic variances are important for the evolution of sexual dimorphism in POLS.

Whether the genetic architecture will pose constraints on evolution also depends on how stable it is over time. The stability of the G-matrix in time and space is an ongoing debate, with some studies supporting conserved matrices between populations and environments (e.g., Delahaie et al. 2017) and temporal stability (e.g., Garant et al. 2008), while others show rapid fluctuations over time (e.g., Björklund et al. 2013). Variation in the strength of genetic covariances enables faster independent evolution of traits within or between sexes during the times when covariances are lowered. The within-sex genetic (co)variance matrix (Gmale and Gfemale) is considered to be more stable over time than the cross-sex covariance matrix (B), which suggests that there is more constraint to change POLS trait covariances within the sexes than it is between them (Barker et al. 2010; Gosden and Chenoweth 2014). Low stability in the B-matrix should open up periods of time with possibilities of independent evolution of the sexes, generating differences in means and covariance patterns between sexes. Artificial selection experiments have shown that both differences in means (Alicchio and Palenzona 1971; Bird and Schaffer 1972) and cross-sex genetic correlations can be quite readily changed (Delph et al. 2011). Nevertheless, several studies show that the cross-sex genetic correlation constrains evolution within (Arnqvist and Tuda 2010), and across species (Schluter 1996; Poissant et al. 2009), suggesting that at least some stability in the genetic architecture across sexes persists over evolutionary time scales. Further investigation is therefore clearly needed on the stability of G and B-matrices.

These insights suggest that sex differences in POLS may evolve even in the absence of sex-specific selection, but become especially likely in its presence affecting both trait means and covariances. However, we are currently lacking studies of multivariate G- and B-matrix architecture of POLS traits in both sexes, which are essential in predicting the evolutionary trajectories of POLS. Experimental evolution or artificial selection studies can be useful for quantifying to what extent genetic architectures are constraining contemporary evolution of POLS in the sexes, while studying the G- and B-matrices in a comparative context would help to understand how species vary in the genetic integration of the different POLS traits and to what extent this is sex-dependent and predictable based on the mating system or other factors that affect sex-specific selection regimes (Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes). G- and B-matrices can be estimated using animal models with pedigreed wild populations or by laboratory breeding designs, including using artificial selection or experimental evolution lines. It is important to note, however, that reliable estimation of covariance matrices requires a lot of data because of the typically large sampling variance, which makes this line of work labor-intensive and subsequently costly, and perhaps most suitable for species with relatively shorter generation times and possibility for laboratory breeding.

Genomic location for genes with sex-specific effects

Where in the genome can we predict to find loci encoding sexually dimorphic POLS phenotypes? This question matters because the genomic location determines in part the genetic architecture of variation in POLS in each sex. Sex chromosomes are subject to different dynamics of sex-specific selection because, unlike autosomes, they are unevenly inherited between males and females. Because they are the only regions of the genome that differ between the sexes, much research has been devoted to understanding what part do sex chromosomes play in accumulating loci with sexually antagonistic effects. Indeed, under some conditions, sex chromosomes can become hot spots for sexually antagonistic genes (Rice 1984; Albert and Otto 2005; Connallon and Clark 2010; Fry 2010). Importantly, this theory could be expanded to sexual dimorphism, which may evolve when modifiers to the antagonistic loci arise in their vicinity that allow sex-specific expression of the antagonistic loci, and hence the development of sexual dimorphism in the phenotype (Rice 1984; but see Connallon and Clark 2010).

The sex chromosomes X and Z  may also contribute disproportionately to the within-sex genetic variation in a sexually dimorphic trait in the heterogametic sex (Reinhold and Engqvist 2013). This idea is based on the logic that genetic variance in the homogametic sex can be reduced by heterozygosis, whereas in the heterogametic sex the allelic effects are immediately exposed (Reinhold and Engqvist 2013). In support of these predictions, Reinhold and Engqvist (2013) found that genetic variance in body size is greater in males of X/Y species and in females of Z/W species. The heterogametic sex could therefore generally show greater amounts of additive genetic variance in POLS traits associated with the X/Z chromosomes than the homogametic sex. How this contributes to the total amount of additive genetic variance will also depend on dosage compensation and the contribution of autosomal alleles (Wyman and Rowe 2014).

Sex differences in recombination rate (i.e., heterochiasmy) is also expected to influence the genomic distribution of loci under sex-specific selection (Connallon and Clark 2010; Wyman and Wyman 2013). Heterochiasmy is very common across taxa and can facilitate coinheritance of beneficial alleles across loci, such as those influencing correlations between POLS traits, in a single linkage group in one sex while allowing their reshuffling in the other (Lenormand 2003). The genomic locations that show heterochiasmy differ among species, and while they also occur in autosomes, the chromosome with sex-determining locus is the most famous such location (Ritz et al. 2017). Linkage with this locus is a safe zone for loci with sexually antagonistic effects because of the reduced or absent recombination (Wright et al. 2017). Y-chromosome (which contains the sex-determining locus in Drosophila) for example influences the expression of many autosomal and X-linked male-biased genes with roles in male reproduction and metabolism, despite having only few genes (Lemos et al. 2008). Y-linked expression modifiers could therefore indirectly regulate other POLS traits that depend on metabolic pathways in males (see also below).

Taken together, there are many circumstances where sex chromosomes are expected to play a disproportionate role in harboring loci for sexually dimorphic traits, and empirical evidence provides support for this (Mank 2009; Dean and Mank 2014; Mank et al. 2014; but see, e.g., Husby et al. 2013), even for behavior and brain sexual differentiation (De Vries et al. 2002; Arnold et al. 2004; Gatewood et al. 2006). However, the exact conditions under which sexual dimorphism evolves via sex-linked loci are still debated (Rice 1984; Connallon and Clark 2010; Fry 2010) and autosomes could also be important (Fry 2010). Future studies that investigate the chromosomal linkage patterns of POLS traits in both sexes, combined with a detailed understanding of how trait variation influences fitness in each sex, will help to elucidate where in the genome POLS variation is encoded. Narrowing down the candidate genes will further allow identifying the molecular mechanisms of sexual dimorphism, such as sex-specific regulation of gene expression, tissue-specificity of such patterns, and putative pleiotropic effects on multiple POLS traits.

Sex differences in gene expression underlie sexually dimorphic phenotypes

At the molecular level, phenotypic differences between the sexes should arise through differential expression of the shared genes, evolved in response to sex-specific selection to resolve the intra-locus sexual conflict (Ellegren and Parsch 2007; Mank 2017). The evidence for these straightforward predictions is accumulating but remains anecdotal (Hollis et al. 2014; Immonen et al. 2014; Harrison et al. 2015; Cheng and Kirkpatrick 2016; Veltsos et al. 2017). Expression differences occur in a large proportion of animal genomes and can be influenced for example by gene duplication followed by evolution of sex-specific gene expression of the new paralog (Connallon and Clark 2011), as well as genomic imprinting (Iwasa and Pomiankowski 2001). Genes with a higher expression in one sex are generally thought to play a more important functional role and experience stronger selection in that sex (e.g., Ellegren and Parsch 2007), and particularly male-biased genes show elevated rates of both gene expression and protein sequence divergence between species (Ellegren and Parsch 2007; Grath and Parsch 2016). However, the patterns of sex-biased expression can be specific to each tissue (Grath and Parsch 2016), developmental (Perry et al. 2014) and reproductive status (Immonen et al. 2017), and therefore, the sex-bias status of the gene may not always be a reliable indicator of its fitness consequences (see also Mank 2017).

In principle, sex-biased expression results from ontogenic sexual differentiation by sex determination cascades. In genetic sex determination, which is the most common mechanism in metazoans, the signal that induces sex differentiation via triggering the development of the gonads is carried by sex chromosomes (Saccone et al. 2002; Smith and Sinclair 2004; Jazin and Cahill 2010). Subsequent sex differences in many somatic traits results from sex-specific alternative splicing that regulates sex-biased expression in a tissue- and cell-specific way (Hartmann et al. 2011).

Although the initial sex differentiation arises from sex-linked genes independent of hormonal control (Arnold 2009), in vertebrates, sex steroids are regarded as the master regulators of most sex differences (Zauner et al. 2003; Ketterson et al. 2005; Mank et al. 2007), including in POLS (see below). In line with this, a recent study in the brown anoles lizard (Anolis sagrei) showed how testosterone regulates changes in sex-biased expression in genes involved in growth and metabolism associated with the development of sexual size dimorphism (Cox et al. 2017). Testosterone-mediated changes in sex-biased expression are also likely behind male-limited polymorphism in life history strategies in the wild turkey (Meleagris gallopavo), in which sexual dimorphism in the gene expression correlates with dimorphism in behavior and plumage (Pointer et al. 2013).

There has been much progress in identifying sex hormone receptor molecules and their sex-dependent expression patterns (Reinius et al. 2008; Wu and Shah 2011). Estrogen signaling is primarily conveyed by the estrogen receptors alpha and beta (ERα and ERβ) that bind to specific DNA sequences—estrogen-responsive elements (EREs)—resulting in the transcriptional activation of genes in sex-specific ways (Jazin and Cahill 2010). ERα and ERβ are particularly interesting for animal “personalities” through their regulatory effect on the oxytocin pathway (see Table 1, Fig. 1). Also androgens exert their function by binding to androgen receptors, which in turn cause the expression of target genes by binding to different sets of androgen-responsive elements (Jazin and Cahill 2010). Overall less is known about the downstream targets of sex hormone receptors and how specific variation in behaviors are regulated in the sexes. A study in mice (Mus musculus) suggests that the different features of male sexual behavior, male aggression, maternal behavior, and female sexual behavior are controlled by many sex-specifically regulated genes (including Brs3, Cckar, Irs4, Sytl4) with highly specific effects on components of behaviors, rather than few genes with broad pleiotropic effects on whole behaviors (Xu et al. 2012). Such architecture may allow selection to fine tune behaviors in the sexes more easily without disruption of complete behaviors.

Table 1 Candidate molecules influencing behavioral syndromes; their putative pleiotropic effects on life history and physiological traits and evidence for sex differences in activity or function. The superscript numbers connect a focal trait with an example of a species and reference where the effect has been observed (separately for each molecule/pathway)

Mechanisms for POLS

Most traits have a highly polygenic basis, and empirical evidence suggests that this may also be true for trait correlations (Saltz et al. 2017). Both linkage disequilibrium (LD), due to correlational selection or physical linkage, and pleiotropy can underlie genetic correlations forming POLS (these are not mutually exclusive; Saltz et al. 2017). LD solely due to correlational selection on unlinked loci is considered evolutionarily less stable due to recombination; however, LD formed by large physical rearrangements such as inversions are a powerful (although relatively rare) way of forming trait correlations, because they can capture multiple genes in a single non-recombining locus. Gene pleiotropy is however likely to be most relevant for POLS because of trade-offs that are at the heart of pace-of-life variation and covariation with behavioral syndromes (Réale et al. 2010). Trade-offs reflect functional relationships between traits that compete over finite resources, and therefore, any variant that influences investment in one trait must necessarily have pleiotropic consequences on other traits dependent on the same resource (Houle 1991). It is important to note, however, that there are many, mutually non-exclusive ways how to define pleiotropy (Paaby and Rockman 2013). For the purpose of this section (summarized in Fig. 1 and Tables 1 and2), we combine evidence of both “molecular gene pleiotropy” (i.e., evidence of functional pleiotropy from, e.g., gene knockout and pharmacological studies) and “developmental pleiotropy” (evidence of mutations influencing the phenotype-genotype map and ontogenic relationships of traits). Here, our aim is to present some of the most interesting candidate molecules involved in neural and physiological pathways that can and should be studied further to test if genetic variation and/or covariation between POLS traits maps to any of the genes involved that could explain for example sex-specific fitness variation or antagonistic pleiotropy between current and future reproduction or early and late life.

Table 2 Examples of candidate molecules and pathways influencing life history, morphological, and physiological traits involved in variation in the pace-of-life, their pleiotropic effects on other traits and evidence of sex differences in activity and/or function. The superscript numbers connect a focal trait with an example of a species and reference where the effect has been observed (separately for each molecule/pathway)

Below, we summarize the candidate pathways for (I) behavioral syndromes and (II) life history trade-offs that have shared effects on multiple POLS traits, and discuss evidence for sex specificity in these effects (Fig. 1, Tables 1 and 2). We specifically discuss the roles of nutrient sensing pathways (“Nutrient sensing pathways”) and glucocorticosteroids (“Glucocorticoids”) in mediating life history trade-offs, as well as metabolic genes (“Metabolic genes”), with a focus on the evolutionary consequences of their inheritance patterns, and male seminal fluid proteins as mediators of life history trade-offs and behavior in females (“Male seminal fluid proteins affect female behavior and life-history trade-offs”). We also discuss the importance of sex steroids and juvenile hormone in mediating sex-specific effects in POLS (III), and lastly, we argue why body size should be considered as an integral part of POLS (IV).

Mechanisms behind behavioral syndromes

In the POLS framework systematic differences in risk-taking behavior, including traits like aggressiveness, boldness, and exploration are expected to coevolve with the trade-off between current and future reproduction (Wolf et al. 2007; Réale et al. 2010). This implies that the neuroendocrine control of behaviors may be coupled with life history variation either indirectly (e.g., through increased mortality due to fighting), or directly, through shared functions, e.g., in metabolism, immunity, and/or reproductive capacity (Fig. 1). How such a network of effects may have evolved is beyond the scope of this article, but correlational selection to position the focal genes under a shared regulatory pathway (see, e.g., the melanocortin system below) has likely played a role. Gene pleiotropy is often discussed in the context of genetic constraints for future evolution; however, selection for alternative life history strategies could have favored such architecture as the most parsimonious way for phenotypic integration.

A growing body of work demonstrates that behavioral syndromes have a genetic basis (van Oers et al. 2005; Dochtermann 2011; Dochtermann et al. 2015; Han and Dingemanse 2017) and studies on humans and rodent models have identified several molecular pathways likely contributing to the genetic correlations among systematic behaviors. Whether the same behavioral pathways influence life history, variation has rarely been studied, but provides a promising future avenue of research. Sex differences have been found in most cases where these pathways have been studied; however, there is a striking bias in studies in favor of using only males as subjects or not testing sex-specific effects when both sexes are included (e.g., Dumais and Veenema 2016).

Perhaps the most promising candidate pathway for POLS in vertebrates is the melanocortin system, which regulates several behavioral traits (aggressiveness, activity and sexual behavior), physiology (metabolism, stress response via HPA axis, immunity and reproductive function) and also morphology (body mass via regulation of feeding rate and adipose tissue, melanin coloration) (Fig. 1, Table 1) (Ducrest et al. 2008; Mundy 2005; Roulin and Ducrest 2013). The melanocortin system is most famous for its effects on melanin pigmentation, which is often a sexual signal and under sex-specific selection (e.g., Saino et al. 2013). Although color variation is not part of the POLS concept, the melanin polymorphism correlates remarkably well with POLS: darker individuals across taxa are often more aggressive (e.g., Mafli et al. 2011), have higher metabolic rate, physical and sexual activity levels (Ducrest et al. 2008), and show increased exploratory behavior (Mateos-Gonzalez and Senar 2012), compatible with a faster pace-of-life syndrome (but see, e.g., Kittilsen et al. 2009 that shows dark individuals have higher resistance to stressors). Sex specificity in the effects has rarely been studied, but melanocortins affect sex hormone production, through which its effects could be sex-specifically modulated. In kestrels (Falco tinnunculus), there is a sex-specific genetic correlation between melanic coloration, immunity, and body mass (Kim et al. 2013) implying sex differences in the melanocortin pathway. Melanin variation is also differently associated with life history strategies in the sexes (Emaresi et al. 2014; Meunier et al. 2010). Melanocortin system consists of a gene POMC that encodes for multiple melanocortin hormones that bind to different receptors with downstream effects on a great variety of traits (Table 1) (Cone 2005). One of the receptors, MC4R, has been implicated to affect sexual (pair-bonding), aggressive, and exploratory behaviors differently in the sexes in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster), while its effects on corticosterone levels are sexually monomorphic (Barrett et al. 2014). In laboratory mice, the receptors MC3R and MC4R affect energy expenditure and foraging differently in the sexes, possibly due to female-biased expression of agouti-related protein (AgRP), which is an antagonist for both of these receptors (Lensing et al. 2016). Because of the staggering array of effects relevant to POLS (Cone 2005; Ducrest et al. 2008) that link both behavioral variation and physiological traits important for life history trade-offs, the melanocortin system is an interesting candidate pathway even in species that lack melanin polymorphism.

Animal “personality” variation is regulated by neurotransmitters dopamine and serotonin systems, which affect various behaviors related to risk-taking and activity with some evidence for sex-specific effects (Table 1, Fig. 1). The gene-encoding Dopamine receptor 4 (DRD4) is polymorphic in many species, and several studies on birds (Fidler et al. 2007; Korsten et al. 2010; Garamszegi et al. 2014; Mueller et al. 2014; Riyahi et al. 2015; van Dongen et al. 2015; Holtmannn et al. 2016; but see Mueller et al. 2013; Edwards et al. 2015; Rollins et al. 2015), mice (Holmes et al. 2003), and rhesus macaques (Coyne et al. 2015) have indicated an association with exploration and boldness (Table 1) and in humans and mice even with lifespan, especially in females (Grady et al. 2013). Interestingly, both DRD4 and serotonin transporter (SERT) have pleiotropic effects also on sexual behaviors (Table 1). In humans and rats (Rattus norvegicus), DRD4 affects sexual desire, arousal, and function (Ben Zion et al. 2006; Melis et al. 2006). A recent study in dunnocks (Prunella modularis) shows how a number of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in both DRD4 and SERT are sex-specifically associated with mating behavior (whether birds breed in monogamous pairs or in promiscuous groups), as well as risk taking (Holtmannn et al. 2016). Dunnocks have an unusually complicated mating system, combining social monogamy, polyandry, and polygyny, and the SNP effects have likely arisen in response to sex-specific selection on different reproductive strategies (Holtmann et al. 2016). However, fitness consequences of the alleles on each sex still need to be quantified. A gene monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) is another candidate gene in the dopaminergic and serotonin systems and associated with variation in aggression human and mouse males (Shih and Chen 1999; Raine 2008), as well as nitric oxidase synthase 1 (NOS1) which has pleiotropic effects also on sexual function (see Table 1).

Parental care is an important component of POLS framework, in which caring for offspring is predicted to trade off with investment into fast reproduction and pace-of-life (Réale et al. 2010; Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes). The neurobiology of parental care is sex-specific and involves neuropeptides, of which vasopressin (VP) and oxytocin (OXT) are the most potent ones (Bosch and Neumann 2012; Bales and Saltzman 2016; Dumais and Veenema 2016; Table 1). Both VP and OXT together with their receptors also affect sexual behaviors such as pair bonding and preference for monogamous pairing as well as mate guarding, social bonding and recognition, aggression, stress, and anxiety (Table 1, Fig. 1). VP-encoding gene is implicated in a recent study to underlie sex-specific differences in parental care between two closely related species of mice, oldfield mice (Peromyscus polionotus) and deer mice (P. maniculatus), that differ markedly in their mating system and level of offspring care (Bendesky et al. 2017). The species have diverged in 12 genomic regions associated with differences in parental behavior, most of which have sex-specific effects. This suggests that the paternal and maternal care differences between the species are governed by different mechanisms (Bendesky et al. 2017), which aligns well with the literature on neuronal regulatory pathways (Bosch and Neumann 2012; Bales and Saltzman 2016). Both VP and OXT are thus important candidate pathways that may govern not only within-species variation but also differences between species in behavioral syndromes associated with sex-specific divergence in mating systems and parental behavior.

Mechanisms underlying pace-of-life

The mechanisms underlying evolutionary costs of reproduction form the basis of pace-of-life variation. Current reproduction may trade off with future reproductive effort or longevity, because resources are diverted from somatic maintenance, or because of the damage it inflicts. Studies on model organisms suggest that reproductive trade-offs arise from the links between resource acquisition, metabolism, reproduction, and lifespan. All of these components are commonly sexually dimorphic and thought to evolve due to sex-specific selection on resource allocation and sexual conflicts (e.g., effects of seminal fluid proteins, see below) (Adler and Bonduriansky 2014; Brooks and Garratt 2017). In line with roles of resource acquisition and allocation into longevity, dietary restriction (limiting calories, macronutrients or certain amino acids) extends lifespan and delays senescence in a range of organisms from yeasts to humans (Masoro 2005; Fontana et al. 2010; Le Couteur et al. 2016; Brooks and Garratt 2017). This effect is commonly sex-specific (Magwere et al. 2004; Tower 2006; Baar et al. 2016; Brooks and Garratt 2017), reflecting differences in dietary resource optimization between females and males (Maklakov et al. 2008; Brooks and Garratt 2017). Importantly, what matters for trade-offs is not simply variation in resource levels such as calories, but the balance of available nutrients: Reproductive rate and lifespan can be maximized by different diet components and these effects can be sex-specific (Brooks and Garratt 2017).

Nutrient sensing pathways

The genetic architecture governing the trade-off between current and future reproduction is poorly understood; however, the reproduction-longevity trade-offs has been investigated intensively, and frequently found to show sex differences (e.g., seed beetles: Fox et al. 2004; Bilde et al. 2009; Immonen et al. 2016a, Drosophila: Vieira et al. 2000; Burger and Promislow 2004). The most important pathways mediating reproductive costs, in terms of longevity, include those controlling growth and energy metabolism via nutrient sensing pathways, including insulin/insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF1), the mechanistic target of rapamycin (mTOR), and sirtuins (Fontana et al. 2010; Brooks and Garratt 2017). IGF1 and growth hormone (GH) influence mammalian growth, and plasma levels of IGF1 correlate with the pace-of-life across species (Swanson and Dantzer 2014). Reducing the function of these growth-promoting signals leads to a greater elongation of lifespan in females than in males, as well as when mTOR activity is decreased, although the patterns also differ between species (Brooks and Garratt 2017).

Insulin-like growth factors (and homologs across species, e.g., CHICO in Drosophila, Clancy et al. 2001) and mTOR have important pleiotropic effects on other traits through which they not only regulate the reproduction-longevity trade-offs but also integration with sexually dimorphic growth rate and development time, as well as secondary sexual signaling and attractiveness via cuticular hydrocarbon signaling (Tatar et al. 2003; Emlen et al. 2012). While these genes have not been studied in the POLS context, they may also directly influence behaviors, possibly through involvement in neuroendocrine control. For example, rapamycin, which blocks some of the mTOR functions, is implicated in (pathological) behaviors of mice (Cleary et al. 2008; Halloran et al. 2012), and neuroendocrine receptors of insulin-producing cells in Drosophila brain affect aggressive and courtship behaviors (Luo et al. 2014). An association of insulin signaling with juvenile hormone secretion has also been implicated as a mechanism behind behavioral variation in Drosophila (Belgacem and Martin 2006), thus linking the expression of behavior, life history, and physiology.

Glucocorticoids

Glucocorticoids (GC) are an important candidate mediator of POLS because they orchestrate energy allocation and storage and allow individuals to respond to the environment adaptively to maximize fitness through their influence on investment into reproduction, immunity, and resource acquisition (Sapolsky et al. 2000; Bonier et al. 2009). Glucocorticoids are responsible for quick mobilization of energy reserves and suppression of non-essential metabolic processes. Therefore, high GC levels can be an adaptive response to acute stressors but detrimental over time (Sapolsky 2005). GCs have also been linked to “coping styles” under stress (proactive and reactive) that potentially link life history, physiology, and behavioral traits in predictable ways (Øverli et al. 2006; Carere et al. 2010; Koolhaas et al. 2010; Silva et al. 2010; Tudorache et al. 2013). A proactive stress-coping involves using aggression to counteract the stressful stimulus, while reactive coping style is characterized by immobility and avoidance of aggression, associated with differences in hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Table 2) responsiveness and brain monoamine neurochemistry (Øverli et al. 2007). The sexes commonly differ in their behavioral and physiological response to stress (e.g., Bale and Epperson 2015) as well as in their immune response, because glucocorticoids interact with sex hormones (Handa et al. 1994; Silva 1999; Lighthall et al. 2009; Bourke et al. 2012). Although the patterns are not yet clear, glucocorticoid baseline or stress-induced levels tend to be higher in females (Kudielka and Kirschbaum 2005; Hämäläinen et al. 2015). It is plausible that sex-specific effects of the environment may be a result of sex-specific responsiveness to stressors (Killen et al. 2013).

Metabolic genes

The POLS framework hypothesizes a link between energy metabolism, behavioral, and life history traits due to the simple reason that limited amounts of energy are needed to fuel all competing processes of the body. Individuals and species vary in how much energy is available to them and how efficiently it may be converted to different processes. Because of this, a correlation between metabolic rate and pace-of-life is expected (Careau et al. 2008, 2010; Biro and Stamps, 2010; Careau and Garland 2012; Glazier 2014). Also, females and males often differ in their metabolic requirements (e.g., Rogowitz and Chappell 2000), and the mechanisms that govern the energy balance should therefore evolve under sex-specific selection. In line with this (mass-specific), metabolic rate commonly differs between the sexes (e.g., Kolluru et al. 2004; Krasnov et al. 2004; Berger et al. 2014a, b, 2016; Kurbalija Novicic et al. 2014; Rønning et al. 2014; but see Krams et al. 2017), and a recent study on a seed beetle (Acanthoscelides obtectus) shows how divergence in the pace-of-life is associated with metabolic rate in a sex-specific way (Arnqvist et al. 2017).

The relationship between metabolic rate and other POLS traits suggests involvement of metabolic genes with major pleiotropic effects that may differ in the sexes (Stamps 2007; Careau et al. 2008; Biro and Stamps 2010). Although the genetic architecture of metabolic rate is not yet fully understood, it involves epistatic interactions between gene products from both mitochondrial (mtDNA) and nuclear genomes (Arnqvist et al. 2010; Immonen et al. 2016b) that together form the three main energy pathways generating ATP (glycolysis, tricarboxylic acid cycle and oxidative phosphorylation). This kind of mitonuclear epistasis not only influences life history traits but importantly also their covariance with systematic behaviors in a seed beetle C. maculatus, consistent with POLS (Lovlie et al. 2014). In A. obtectus beetles, different mtDNA haplotypes are associated with “slow” and “fast” pace-of-life and sex-specific mitochondrial bioenergetics (Dordevic et al. 2017). Indeed, the effects of mtDNA variation and mitonuclear epistasis are often sex-specific (Dobler et al. 2014; Immonen et al. 2016a). This is predicted because of the maternal inheritance of mitochondria, which allows mtDNA to respond to selection only through females, and therefore the potential for accumulation of male-harming mtDNA mutations (i.e., “mother’s curse,” Gemmell et al. 2004; Innocenti et al. 2011). As a result, males can show more phenotypic variation arising from mtDNA variation than females, as demonstrated for aging and fertility in D. melanogaster (Camus et al. 2012). Selection on males to restore and improve male-specific mitochondrial function can operate via nuclear genes (e.g., Gallach and Betran 2011), which should lead to a genetic architecture of metabolism that involves partly different genes and regulatory patterns in males and females. In a bank vole M. glareolus, mtDNA haplotype variation influences the correlation between metabolic rate and proactive behavior depending on the nuclear genetic background, but only in males (Sichova et al. 2014). In accordance with theory, these patterns suggest that mitonuclear cross talk has the power to generate sex-specific patterns in POLS trait correlations that may also be sub-optimal in males due to the sex difference in the efficacy of selection. More work is however needed to understand the mechanistic relationship between the metabolic pathways and “personality” behaviors (see also Krams et al. 2017).

Male seminal fluid proteins affect female behavior and life history trade-offs

An interesting but less often considered mechanism affecting exclusively female reproductive investment and trade-offs with lifespan includes molecular interactions that occur upon mating with males. Due to the different evolutionary interests of the sexes over reproductive rates (Parker 1979; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005), males can be selected to manipulate female reproductive effort towards current reproductive event via transferring a cocktail of seminal fluid proteins and peptides (SFPs) during mating (Poiani 2006). One such molecule, the sex peptide (SP) of D. melanogaster, influences female fecundity, re-mating rate, feeding, activity patterns, and aggression, as well as immunity activation (Avila et al. 2011; Sirot et al. 2015; Bath et al. 2016). SP improves male fitness (Fricke et al. 2010), at the expense of female lifespan (Chapman et al. 1995). Such effects can arise at least partly through trade-offs: males induce fecundity in females, who pay an extra latent cost of reproduction through accelerated aging. However, SFPs reduce lifespan of even sterilized females in D. melanogaster (Barnes et al. 2008; see Maures et al. 2014 for similar findings in Caenorhabditis elegans). The mechanisms behind these effects in female Drosophila are not yet known, but hormonal control via juvenile hormone may play a role (Moshitzky et al. 1996; Yamamoto et al. 2013). The fact that molecular interactions can have such profound consequences on key life history and behavioral traits in females implies that many traits in female POLS may be subject to both direct genetic effects of the females and indirect genetic effects that depend on male genotype (Immonen et al. 2016a). What consequences such a complex genetic control has for the evolution of POLS is an important question for elucidating the evolutionary trajectories in each sex.

Sex steroids and juvenile hormone are master regulators of sex-specific POLS

As discussed earlier, the sex hormones estrogen and testosterone are essential for sexually dimorphic behaviors in vertebrates. Estrogen is responsible for generating the repertoire of sexual and territorial behaviors in both sexes, while testosterone modifies the intensity of these behaviors (Ogawa et al. 2000; Kudwa and Rissman 2003; Kudwa et al. 2006; Raskin et al. 2009; Wu et al. 2009; Juntti et al. 2010; Wu and Shah 2011). Sex hormones are important mediators of sexual dimorphism in systematic behaviors, such as those forming animal “personality,” because they influence not only the development and modification of sex-specific behaviors in adults but also behaviors such as aggression and anxiety (Book et al. 2001). The relative roles for testosterone and estrogen in aggression differ in females and males (Hau 2007), which can allow decoupling the effects in the sexes (Cain et al. 2016). Similar sex specificity underlies anxiety: Studies in rodents discovered that a particular isoform of the estrogen receptor (ERβ) decreases anxiety in females (Imwalle et al. 2005; Lund et al. 2005) while increased levels of circulating testosterone decrease anxiety in both sexes (Celec et al. 2015) via androgen receptors (Zuloaga et al. 2008). Indeed, both estrogen receptors α and β regulate oxytocin levels, through which they influence territorial aggression, anxiety, parental behaviors, and mate preference (Table 1). Genetic variation in sex hormone receptors such as ERα and ERβ is therefore one putative way to achieve sex-specific functional variation. In the white-throated sparrow (Zonotrichia albicollis), plumage coloration and sexually dimorphic polymorphism in reproductive strategies is determined by alternative alleles at an inversion-based “supergene” including ERα (Tuttle et al. 2016). The behavioral differences in territorial aggression and parental care between males of the different morphs are mediated by expression differences in ERα, while behavioral differences in female sparrows have a different neural mechanism than in males (Horton et al. 2014). ERα causes similar behavioral differences also in prairie voles: Males showing more promiscuity and less parental care express ERα at a higher level in the same brain region as the white-morph sparrows (Cushing et al. 2008).

Testosterone has received particular interest in evolutionary biology because of its role in mediating trade-offs between reproductive allocation and self-preservation via physiological effects such as lower investment in immune defense (Muehlenbein and Bribiescas 2005) and between mating and parental effort (McGlothlin et al. 2007). For example, increased aggressiveness may benefit a male in terms of acquisition and defense of resources or mates, but also influence parasite loads and immunocompetence (Zuk and Stoehr 2002; Muehlenbein and Bribiescas 2005; Hayward 2013) as well as the expression of secondary sexual characters (Roberts et al. 2004). Testosterone has also a negative/reducing effect on longevity, either directly or through its effects on behavior, while estrogen levels extend lifespan (Brooks and Garratt 2017). Testosterone also plays an important role in limiting expression of alternative male phenotypes (i.e., polymorphism in physiological, behavioral, morphological and life history differences) to males (Hau 2007). The ruff (Philomachus pugnax) is a good example of such a polymorphic reproductive strategy: Males exhibit three alternative mating tactics where they differ in aggression, hormone levels, mating behaviors, and morphology (Lank et al. 1995, 2013; Kupper et al. 2016). Females can also express morph differences if their testosterone levels are artificially increased (Lank et al. 1999).

Although sex steroids play a pivotal part in controlling sex-biased expression, thereby decreasing the genetic correlation between the sexes (Cox et al. 2017), they can also be a source of sexual conflict (Mokkonen et al. 2011; Mills et al. 2012). Sex-specific and sexually antagonistic selection may help to explain some of the surprising differences observed between species (Hau and Goymann 2015) and offer one mechanism for how commonly observed variation in testosterone levels among individuals (Williams 2008) can be maintained (Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes). To improve our understanding of how sex hormones, and particularly testosterone effects on behavior, and life history trade-offs evolve, we need to start to factor in their fitness impacts on each sex and to estimate the genetic architecture of the trait correlations within and across sexes. We suspect that multitrait phenotypes involving sex steroids may commonly have sex-specific genetic architecture, as implicated in masked boobies (Sula dactylatra) (Fargallo et al. 2014).

Juvenile hormone has been raised as a candidate testosterone-analogue responsible for mediating reproductive and immune investment in invertebrates (Rantala et al. 2003) and has potential to influence polymorphisms and polyphenisms (Nijhout and Wheeler 1982). It has effects on multiple life history components including, for instance, development (Schal et al. 1997), metamorphosis (Flatt et al. 2005), sexual maturation (Schal et al. 1997), (sexual) behavior (Strambi et al. 1997; Flatt et al. 2005), reproduction, diapause, stress resistance, aging (Flatt et al. 2005), and gene expression (reviewed in Jones 1995; Wyatt and Davey 1996). Juvenile hormone also plays a role in sex-specific signaling by mediating sexual horn dimorphism in horned beetles (Shelby et al. 2007) and possibly in sexual size dimorphism (Stillwell et al. 2010). The hormone is broadly involved in parental care, with levels during parental care differing in the sexes (Trumbo 2002; Panaitof et al. 2004). Although early evidence suggested that male juvenile hormone titres may be higher than those of females (Gilbert and Schneiderman 1961), very few studies have measured juvenile hormone levels or its effects in both sexes (Stillwell et al. 2010). Thus, broader sex differences in its activity remain to be resolved.

Sexual dimorphism in body size and integration with POLS

Sexual size dimorphism is widespread and may be directly associated with sex differences in life history, behavioral, and physiological traits through differing resource acquisition-allocation patterns, sex-specific effects of the environment, and sexual selection (Shine 1989; Magurran and Garcia 2000; Isaac 2005; Stillwell et al. 2010). Body size has traditionally been considered a part of the fast-slow continuum, where small adult body size was thought to covary with a fast life history and large body size with a slow one (Reynolds 2003). This categorization is not, however, straightforward and is further complicated by taxon-specific sexual size dimorphism (Abouheif and Fairbairn 1997; Fairbairn 1997).

Nevertheless, body size is an important variable that can influence suites of traits simultaneously, and subsequently sex-specific fitness through these associations (see, e.g., Berg and Maklakov 2012; Berger et al. 2014a, b; Gaillard et al. 2016). For example, body size scales with the size of the digestive tract, basal metabolic rate, and to some extent feeding behavior (Perez-Barberia et al. 2008). When comparing species, allometric scaling of morphology plays an important role in evolutionary trajectories (Gould 1966), and therefore, it is likely that body size also influences the evolution of POLS. Sex-specific integration of body size and POLS may be expected for example when size dimorphism is related to sex differences in resource requirements, predation, or pathogen infection risk (Peters 1983), which can cause sex-specific selection on foraging activity, boldness, or behavioral activity and immunity, respectively. Body size may therefore be a central player underlying sex-specific genetic correlations among POLS traits, for example through shared molecular mechanisms influencing growth and variation in pace-of-life. For example VGLL3 and insulin/IGF1 signaling pathway both affect body size and development time with sex-specific effects, with the latter also important for longevity trade-offs (see above) (Table 2, Fig. 1). Melanocortin system is another interesting pathway, affecting not only body size via adipose tissue regulation, immunity, and stress but also systematic variation in aggressive and sexual behaviors (Table 1, Fig. 1).

The degree of integration (i.e., strength of covariation, Klingenberg 2008) with body size will affect the potential for independent evolution of the specific POLS traits, with strong integration constraining independent evolution of the traits, thus maintaining the syndrome. However, the level of integration between body size and different POLS trait categories differs, being greater with physiology, such as basal metabolic rate (Konarzewski and Książek 2012), and lower with life history traits, such as developmental time (e.g., Blanckenhorn et al. 2007). The integration of body size and POLS traits also varies between species, and particularly so in those with high sexual dimorphism (Fairbairn and Preziosi 1994; Fairbairn 1997). If the variation in integration with different POLS traits varies also sex-specifically, there is opportunity for independent trait evolution and sex-specific covariances between POLS traits. Given that body size is a common target of sex-specific selection (Fairbairn et al. 2007), more work should be devoted in understanding coevolution of body size and POLS in the sexes coupled with identifying the molecular underpinnings of the genetic integration.

Conclusions and future prospects

The POLS framework stems from the observation that animals vary consistently in their behavior, which could both arise from, as well as be the source of, variation in life history traits along the pace-of-life continuum, integrated through physiology. Although behavioral differences between the sexes are highly prevalent (e.g., Schuett et al. 2010) and their study has been central to behavioral ecology from the start of this field, studies examining the evolutionary causes and mechanisms underlying sex differences in POLS have only now started to appear. On the other hand, sex differences have been a focus of much research in life history and physiological contexts, and particularly in animals that can be reared in the laboratory. Our aim in this article was to bridge some of this gap by bringing together key concepts and insights from the fields of behavior, life history, and reproductive genetics, to inspire future work to look more closely into how genetic covariances underlying POLS are generated in each sex.

In particular, we emphasize the need for (quantitative) genetic studies on covariation between life history trade-offs between current and future reproduction and behavioral syndromes in each sex, which are currently rare. This entails studying correlations between traits in more than two POLS categories at a time (e.g., immunity and behavior), which is currently the dominating approach. Also, given that the POLS composition is expected to vary between the sexes and species depending, e.g., on the mating system and environment (Hämäläinen et al. 2018, topical collection on Pace-of-life syndromes), care should be taken to study the most relevant trait combinations for a given study system. Comparative studies across species with different mating systems would be particularly useful in testing the consequence of variation in sex-specific selection on the genetic architecture (i.e., G- and B-matrices) of POLS in the sexes. Experimental evolution and artificial selection studies can be used to study these at the micro-evolutionary scale.

Molecular genetics and pharmacological studies suggest that shared pathways underlie many POLS traits, opening opportunities to study genetic variation in these pathways and the relative roles of gene pleiotropy and linkage in forming genetic correlations. Although pleiotropy is commonly seen as a constraint of independent trait evolution, POLS framework can offer a fruitful context in which to study whether selection may have favored genetic coupling of traits under shared pathways. Studies of sex-specific genetic and hormonal effects on POLS traits have started to emerge and suggest that sex specificity is common in their effects on both behavioral and life history traits. What we need to understand next is what consequences such effects have on trait correlations and what are their fitness impacts in each sex, and thus consequences on the evolutionary dynamics of POLS. Failure to incorporate the putative sex specificity and the integrated nature of traits in the studies of POLS can risk overlooking important mechanisms that can only be found in one sex.

Studying POLS in the context of sex differences forces us also to consider how primary and secondary sexual characteristics covary with POLS traits. For example, sex-specific selection on melanin pigmentation—due to its role as a sexual signal or in immunity—may affect POLS due to the pleiotropic effects of melanocortins. Sexually dimorphic body size and its role in both reproductive trade-offs and behavioral syndromes is another example. Although predictions for such traits are not straightforward in the POLS framework (e.g., Réale et al. 2010), incorporating sexual traits would improve our understanding of how selection operates on integrated phenotypes as a whole.

It is interesting to note that observing sex differences at the molecular level may not translate into sex differences at the phenotypic level: Some sex differences in gene action may have evolved to enable the sexes to display similar phenotypes, such as behaviors (De Vries 2004). For example, evolution of monogamy and convergent reproductive roles from a polygamous ancestral state may require further sexually dimorphic or sex-limited changes because of the pre-existing sex differences. This would suggest that—depending on the evolutionary history—the genetic architecture of POLS may be sex-specific even when the phenotype is monomorphic in the sexes. Comparative studies between species that differ in reproductive roles, and thus in selection for sexual dimorphism, may be particularly useful for illuminating the extent to which variation in the sexual dimorphism in POLS is reflected on dimorphism in the underlying genetic architecture.

In this article, we have mainly focused on discussing how sex differences within species arise and what consequences these may have for POLS at a population level. Given that differences in reproductive trade-offs between the sexes as well as species can create a similar gradient of POLS, one important question is whether there is a conserved genetic basis underlying this gradient for both individuals of the different sexes as well as species. Many of the genes and pathways discussed here are largely conserved (e.g., nutrient sensing pathways, melanocortins, DRD4, SERT; Tables 1 and 2) opening up this opportunity, and comparative work would help to test whether “fast” and “slow” strategies indeed share a genetic basis across sexes and species.

Currently, there is a strong bias in the molecular studies in favor of model organisms. Several molecular and quantitative genetic tools are now, however, also available for studying personality and life history variation in non-model organisms, allowing answering questions such as what genetic variants are segregating in natural populations and how the genetic effects may interact with the natural environment. The study on New Zealand population of dunnocks is an example of utilizing a human candidate gene approach to unravel the genetic basis of personality and mating behavior variation in the wild (Holtmann et al. 2016). Sequencing candidate genes is an inexpensive alternative to looking for new ones with quantitative trait loci (QTL) mapping or using a genome wide association study (GWAS) technique that utilizes genome-wide information of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). However, with the candidate gene approach, care needs to be taken to properly account for population structure by using several neutral genetic markers. Discovering novel candidate genes also in non-model organisms is now made possible by the rapid rise of affordable sequencing technologies, such as RNA-seq and RAD-seq, that can also be used with species without previous genetic tools (da Fonseca et al. 2016). RNA-seq can also be used to study the role of sex-biased gene expression in regulating sexual dimorphism in POLS. Functional studies of genes to elucidate putative sex-specific effects are essential but have only recently started to emerge (e.g., Dumais and Veenema 2016). These are now made available also in non-model organisms with CRISPR/Cas9 technique (Singh et al. 2015).

We hope that this review will encourage more endeavors of studying the mechanistic basis of POLS but also spark general interest in appreciating what makes males and females unique and how far-reaching consequences these differences may have on the evolution of POLS.