Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 68, Issue 8, pp 1345–1355 | Cite as

Social and ecological correlates of long-distance pant hoot calls in male chimpanzees

Original Paper

Abstract

Long-distance calls have a variety of functions in different animal species. However, where multiple functions are proposed for a single long-distance call type, little is known about their relative importance. Chimpanzees are one species where several functions have been proposed for their long-distance call, the pant hoot. In this study, we investigated the effect of social factors, including the rank of the caller, party size, fission–fusion rates, and the presence of estrus females as well as ecological factors including the type of food consumed and travel time, on male chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) pant hooting, in order to identify the key correlates of this behavior. The wild male chimpanzees of the Kanyawara community, Uganda, produced more pant hoots on days when there were frequent changes in the male, but not female, composition of the focal’s party. This factor accounted for the largest amount of variation in pant hoot production, and we found that males were more likely to repeat a call prior to rather than after fusion with other males, suggesting that the calls facilitate fusion. Pant hoots therefore seem to play a pivotal role in regulating grouping dynamics in chimpanzees. Our study also shows that pant hooting was positively correlated with the rank of the caller, the presence of parous females in estrus, and the consumption of high-quality food, suggesting that pant hoots signal social status or social bonds when between-male competition is high. This study supports the view that pant hoots fulfill a complex social function.

Keywords

Chimpanzee Pant hooting Pan troglodytes Long-distance call Fission–fusion 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the directors of the Kibale Chimpanzee Project, Richard Wrangham and Martin Muller, for allowing them to conduct this research on the Kanyawara chimpanzees as well as for commenting on a previous version of the paper. The KCP field manager Emily Otali and KCP field assistants Francis Mugurusi, Solomon Musana, James Kyomuhendo, Wilberforce Tweheyo, Sunday John, and Christopher Irumba, who were extremely helpful during the fieldwork are also thanked. The authors also thank Stuart Semple and Geoffrey Hall for helpful comments on a previous version of the draft and Simon Townsend for statistical advice. Comments and suggestions were provided by the editor David Watts, and two anonymous reviewers substantially improved the paper. PF was funded by a BBSRC studentship, a Leakey Foundation General Grant, and an American Society of Primatologists General Small Grant.

Ethical standards

Permission to conduct the study was granted by the Ugandan Wildlife Authority and the Ugandan National Council for Science and Technology. The study complied with the current laws of Uganda.

References

  1. Akaike H (1974) New look at statistical-model identification. IEEE Trans Autom Control 19:716–723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2013) Female putty-nosed monkeys use experimentally altered contextual information to disambiguate the cause of male alarm calls. PLoS ONE 8:e65660PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Aureli F, Schaffner CM (2007) Aggression and conflict management at fusion in spider monkeys. Biol Lett 3:147–149PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Aureli F, Schaffner CM, Boesch C, Bearder SK, Call J et al (2008) Fission–fusion dynamics. New research frameworks. Curr Anthropol 49:627–654CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Basabose AK (2002) Diet composition of chimpanzees inhabiting the montane forest of Kahuzi, Democratic Republic of Congo. Am J Primatol 58:1–21PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burnham KP, Anderson DR (2004) Multimodel inference: understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociol Methods Res 33:261–304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bygott JD (1979) Agonistic behaviour, dominance, and social structure in wild chimpanzees of the Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg DA, McCown ER (eds) The great apes. Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, pp 405–427Google Scholar
  8. Chapman CA, Wrangham RW (1993) Range use of the forest chimpanzees of Kibale: implications of the understanding of chimpanzee social organization. Am J Primatol 31:263–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chapman CA, Wrangham RW, Chapman LJ (1995) Ecological constraints on group size: an analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 36:59–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clark AP (1991) The socioecology of wild chimpanzee vocal behavior in the Kibale forest. Dissertation, University of Michigan, UgandaGoogle Scholar
  11. Clark AP (1993) Rank differences in the production of vocalisations by wild chimpanzees as a function of social context. Am J Primatol 31:159–179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Clark AP, Wrangham RW (1993) Acoustic analysis of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: do Kibale Forest chimpanzees have an acoustically distinct food arrival pant hoot? Am J Primatol 31:99–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Clark AP, Wrangham RW (1994) Chimpanzee arrival pant-hoots: do they signify food or status? Int J Primatol 15:185–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD (1979) The roaring of red deer and the evolution of honest advertisement. Behaviour 69:145–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Conklin-Brittain NL, Wrangham RW, Hunt KD (1998) Dietary response of chimpanzees and cercopithecines to seasonal variation in fruit abundance. II. Macronutrients. Int J Primatol 19:971–998CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. de Vries H (1993) Matman: a program for the analysis of sociometric matrices and behavioural transition matrices. Behaviour 125:157–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. de Vries H (1995) An improved test of linearity in dominance hierarchies containing unknown or tied relationships. Anim Behav 50:1375–1389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Delgado RA (2006) Sexual selection in the loud calls of male primates: signal content and function. Int J Primatol 27:5–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Duffy KG, Wrangham RW, Silk JB (2007) Male chimpanzees exchange political support for mating opportunities. Curr Biol 17:R586–R587PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Emery Thompson M, Wrangham RW (2008) Diet and reproductive function in wild female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Kibale National Park, Uganda. Am J Phys Anthropol 135:171–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Emery Thompson M, Muller MN, Wrangham RW, Lwanga JS, Potts KB (2009) Urinary C-peptide tracks seasonal and individual variation in energy balance in wild chimpanzees. Horm Behav 55:299–305PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fedurek P, Slocombe KE (2013) The social function of food-associated calls in male chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 75:726–739PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fedurek P, Machanda Z, Schel AM, Slocombe KE (2013) Pant hoot chorusing and social bonds in male chimpanzees. Anim Behav 86:189–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2002) Acoustic features of male baboon loud calls: influences of context, age, and individuality. J Acoust Soc Am 111:1465–1474PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fischer J, Kitchen DM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2004) Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56:140–148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fuller JL (2013) The vocal repertoire of adult male blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stulmanni): a quantitative analysis of acoustic structure. Am J Primatol 76:203–216PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Furmankiewicz J, Ruczynski I, Urban R, Jones G (2001) Social calls provide tree-dwelling bats with information about the location of conspecifics at roosts. Ethology 117:480–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Geissmann T (1999) Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: II. Testing the pair-bonding hypothesis during a partner exchange. Behaviour 136:1005–1039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gilby IC, Wrangham RW (2008) Association patterns among wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) reflect sex differences in cooperation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1831–1842CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  31. Gottlander K (1987) Variation in the song rate of the male pied flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca: causes and consequences. Anim Behav 35:1037–1043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hall ML (2004) A review of hypotheses for the functions of avian duetting. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:415–430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Harris TR (2006) Within- and among-male variation in roaring by black and white colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza): what does it reveal about function? Behaviour 143:197–218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Isabirye-Basuta G (1987) Feeding ecology of chimpanzees in the Kibale Forest. In: Heltne PG, Marquardt LA (eds) Understanding chimpanzees. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 166–127Google Scholar
  35. Kitchen DM, Seyfarth RM, Fischer J, Cheney DL (2003) Loud calls as indicators of dominance in male baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53:374–384Google Scholar
  36. Lammers MO, Schotten M, Au WWL (2006) The spatial context of free-ranging Hawaiian spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) producing acoustic signals. J Acoust Soc Am 119:1244–1250PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Leighty KA, Soltis J, Wesolek CM, Savage A (2008) Rumble vocalizations mediate interpartner distance in African elephants, Loxodonta africana. Anim Behav 76:1601–1608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Marler P, Hobbett L (1975) Individuality in a long-range vocalization of wild chimpanzee. Z Tierpsychol 38:97–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McComb KE (1991) Female choice for high roaring rates in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Anim Behav 41:79–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. McComb K, Reby D, Baker L, Moss C, Sayialel S (2003) Longdistance communication of acoustic cues to social identity in African elephants. Anim Behav 65:317–329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mitani JC, Brandt KL (1994) Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology 96:233–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Mitani JC, Gros-Louis J (1998) Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: tests of three hypotheses. Behaviour 135:1041–1064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Mitani JC, Nishida T (1993) Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Anim Behav 45:735–746CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Mitani JC, GrosLouis J, Macedonia JM (1996) Selection for acoustic individuality within the vocal repertoire of wild chimpanzees. Int J Primatol 17:569–583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Muller MN (2002) Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) Diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 112–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Muller MN, Wrangham RW (2004) Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ' challenge hypothesis'. Anim Behav 67:113–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Muller MN, Thompson ME, Wrangham RW (2006) Male chimpanzees prefer mating with old females. Curr Biol 16:2234–2238PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Neumann C, Assahad G, Hammerschmidt K, Perwitasari-Farajallah D, Engelhardt A (2010) Loud calls in male crested macaques, Macaca nigra: a signal of dominance in a tolerant species. Anim Behav 79:187–193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Nishida T (1983) Alpha status and agonistic alliance in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Primates 24:318–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Nishida T, Kano T, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nakamura M (1999) Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropol Sci 107:141–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Notman H, Rendall D (2005) Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Anim Behav 70:177–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Reynolds R (2005) The chimpanzees of Budongo forest: ecology, behaviour and conservation. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Reynolds V, Reynolds R (1965) Chimpanzees of the Budongo forest. In: De Vore I (ed) Primate behavior: field studies of monkeys and apes. Rinehart & Winston, New York, pp 368–424Google Scholar
  54. Schel A, Tranquilli S, Zuberbühler K (2009) The alarm call system of two species of black-and-white colobus monkeys (Colobus polykomos and Colobus guereza). J Comp Psychol 123:136–150PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Schel AM, Machanda Z, Townsend SW, Zuberbühler K, Slocombe KE (2013) Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Anim Behav 86:955–965CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Schwartz JJ (1986) Male calling behavior and female choice in the neotropical treefrog. Ethology 73:116–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  58. Sidak Z (1967) Rectangular confidence regions for the means of multivariate normal distributions. J Am Stat Assoc 62:626–633Google Scholar
  59. Slocombe KE, Zuberbuhler K (2006) Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: responses to food types or food preferences? Anim Behav 72:989–999CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Smith WJ (1977) The behavior of communicating: an ethological approach. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  61. Spehar SN, Di Fiore N (2013) Loud calls as a mechanism of social coordination in a fission–fusion taxon, the white-bellied spider monkey (Ateles belzebuth). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67:947–961CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Struhsaker TT (1975) The red colobus monkey. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  63. Waser MS (1977) Individual recognition, intragroup cohesion, and intergroup spacing: evidence from sound playback to forest monkeys. Behaviour 60:28–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Wheeler BC, Fischer J (2012) Functionally referential signals: a promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evol Anthropol 21:195–205PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Wilson ML, Hauser MD, Wrangham RW (2007) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour 144:1621–1653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Wilson ML, Kahlenberg SM, Wells M, Wrangham RW (2012) Ecological and social factors affect the occurrence and outcomes of intergroup encounters in chimpanzees. Anim Behav 83:277–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wrangham RW (1977) Feeding behaviour of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Primate ecology. Academic Press, London, pp 503–538Google Scholar
  68. Wrangham RW, Conklin NL, Chapman CA, Hunt KD (1991) The significance of fibrous foods for Kibale forest chimpanzees. Philos Trans R Soc B 334:171–178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Wrangham RW, Clark AP, Isabirye-Basuta G (1992) Female social relationships and social organization of Kibale Forest Chimpanzees. In: Nishida T, McGrew WC, Marler P, Pickford M, de Waal FBM (eds) Human origins. The University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp 81–98Google Scholar
  70. Wrangham RW, Chapman CA, Clark-Arcadi AP, Isabirye-Basuta G (1996) Social ecology of Kanyawara chimpanzees: implications for understanding the costs of great ape groups. In: McGrew WC, Marchant L, Nishida T (eds) Great ape societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 45–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Wrangham RW, Conklin-Brittain NL, Hunt KD (1998) Dietary response of chimpanzees and cercopithecines to seasonal variation in fruit abundance. I. Antifeedants. Int J Primatol 19:949–970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Zuberbühler K (2001) Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell’s guenons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50:414–422CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Pawel Fedurek
    • 1
    • 2
  • Ed Donnellan
    • 3
  • Katie E. Slocombe
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of YorkYorkUK
  2. 2.Institute of BiologyUniversity of NeuchâtelNeuchâtelSwitzerland
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of SheffieldSheffieldUK

Personalised recommendations