Background

Frailty is a complex clinical condition that is intricately linked to the aging process and is accompanied by a decline in physiological function across multiple organ systems, with increased susceptibility to stressors [1, 2]. As the global population ages, the prevalence of frailty increases proportionately [3]. Beyond its role as a noteworthy predictor of mortality among older individuals, frailty is implicated in a spectrum of adverse health outcomes, including falls, delirium, and disability [4,5,6]. In recent years, frailty has garnered considerable attention as a public health concern and tremendous global health challenge.

The roles of sedentary behavior and sleep status in the etiology of frailty have been extensively investigated [7,8,9,10]. However, owing to limitations in the quality of available evidence, as well as the presence of potential reverse causality and residual confounding factors, observational studies have been unable to establish causal associations. To address this issue, randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have been conducted to elucidate cause-and-effect relationships [11]. Nonetheless, RCTs are often resource-intensive in terms of finances and manpower, and certain interventions may not be feasible or approved for assessment. In this context, Mendelian randomization (MR) is effective for estimating the causal effects of exposures on outcomes [12, 13]. By employing genetic variants that are robustly associated with the exposure of interest as instrumental variables (IVs) randomly assigned at conception, MR studies mitigate the confounding and reverse causality biases that are inherent in conventional observational studies [14, 15].

We conducted univariable and multivariable MR analyses to examine potential independent causal effects of leisure sedentary behaviors and sleep status on the frailty index (FI). Linkage disequilibrium score (LDSC) regression was used to investigate the genetic correlation between these causal traits.

Methods

Data source of exposure

Watching television, computer usage, and driving were identified as three distinct categories of sedentary behaviors [16]. To ascertain the extent of sedentary time, the participants were asked a set of three questions during their initial visit. These inquiries encompassed the following: “On a typical day, how many hours do you spend watching television?,” “In a typical day, how many hours do you spend using the computer? (Do not include using a computer at work),” and “On a typical day, how many hours do you spend driving?.” The daily duration of these sedentary behaviors served as a measure of exposure assessment. The study population comprised 408,815 individuals of European ancestry enrolled from the UK Biobank. The participants had an average age of 57.4 (± 8.0) years, and females constituted 54.3% of the cohort. Mean daily reported leisure television watching was 2.8 h (± 1.5), leisure computer use was 1.0 h (± 1.2) and driving was 0.9 h (± 1.0).

We acquired genetic instruments for sleep duration through a genome-wide association study (GWAS) conducted using the UK Biobank dataset [17]. Within the UK Biobank, the sleep duration assessment involved a specific inquiry: “How many hours of sleep do you get during every twenty-four hours (including naps)?” The results were scaled to per-hour increases in sleep duration. The study encompassed a cohort of 446,118 individuals with European ancestry, characterized by a mean age of 57.3 (± 8.0) years. Notably, women constituted 54.1% of the participant group. The analysis revealed mean self-reported habitual sleep duration was 7.2 h (± 1.1) per 24 h.

Genetic variants associated with snoring were obtained from the UK Biobank population [18]. During the assessment, participants were asked the following question: “Does your partner or a close relative or friend complain about your snoring?” Response options included “Yes,” “No,” “Don’t know,” or “Prefer not to answer.” Individuals who responded with “Don’t know” or “Prefer not to answer” were excluded from the dataset. Snoring data were available in 359,916 unrelated individuals of European descent after quality control. The prevalence of snoring in this sample was 37.3%.

Daytime napping is an uncontrollable sleep pattern. We acquired genetic variants associated with daytime napping from an extensive UK Biobank dataset comprising 452,633 participants [19]. During the assessment, the participants were asked, “Do you take a nap during the day?” Among the UK Biobank population, 38.2% and 5.3% of the respondents answered “sometimes” and “always,” respectively. Moreover, the average ages of the two groups were 58.5 (± 7.8) years and 60.2 (± 7.4) years, with females comprising 50.0% and 33.9% of each respective group.

Data source of outcome

We identified genetic variants associated with the FI using a GWAS meta-analysis. The GWAS included 164,610 participants from the UK Biobank and 10,616 from TwinGene [20]. The UK Biobank participants consisted of individuals of European descent, aged 60 to 70 years at baseline (mean 64.1, SD 2.8). The cohort included 84,819 females, accounting for 51.3% of the total participants. The TwinGene participants, a separate cohort comprising 10,616 individuals, were Swedish nationals aged 41 to 87 years (mean 58.3, SD 7.9), with 5,577 females (52.5%). The Rockwood FI, which is based on the deficit accumulation model, served as the outcome measure for frailty. Compliance with deficits was categorized using a score of 0 or 1 (with 0 indicating no deficit). The FI for each individual was calculated as the number of deficits divided by 49. A higher FI value indicates a greater degree of frailty. Our findings revealed that the mean proportions of deficits were 0.129 ± 0.075 and 0.121 ± 0.080, in UK Biobank and TwinGene participants, respectively.

The baseline characteristics of participants included in these above GWAS studies were presented in Additional file 2: Table S1.

Selection criteria for instrumental variables

To ensure the validity of each instrumental variable (IV), three key assumptions must be met: (1) relevance assumption: robust association between the instrument and the exposure; (2) independence assumption: the genetic variant is not linked to confounding factors influencing the exposure-outcome relationship [21]; and (3) exclusion restriction assumption: instruments solely impact the outcome through the exposure variable [22]. To fulfill the first assumption of MR, we identified single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) that achieved genome-wide significance (P < 5 × 10− 8). From this SNP set, we exclusively retained independent instruments with the most significant P-values, considering pairwise linkage disequilibrium (LD) and removing SNPs with r2 ≥ 0.001. We further evaluated the first assumption by computing the F-statistic [23]. Subsequently, we excluded SNPs with an F-statistic < 10 to avoid weak IV biases. Genetic variants from diverse studies were combined regarding their effects and palindromic SNPs were excluded. Proxy SNPs (r2 > 0.8) were used for instruments absent from the outcome dataset. The MR-Steiger filtering was also used to removes SNPs failing to explain significantly more variance in the exposure than in the outcome. To mitigate the bias arising from horizontal pleiotropy, we performed the Mendelian Randomization Pleiotropy RESidual Sum and Outlier (MR-PRESSO) outlier test, calculated the P value for each SNP, and excluded outlier SNPs [24]. The lead SNPs for the genetic instruments of exposure were presented in Additional file 2: Tables S2S7.

Univariable Mendelian randomization

We employed the inverse-variance-weighted (IVW) approach as the primary method for estimating causal effects, which allows calculation of the combined effect of all SNPs. Additionally, to ensure reliability and stability of the results, sensitivity analyses were conducted using MR-Egger [25], weighted median [26], and MR-PRESSO [24]. Heterogeneity in the main IVW and MR-Egger estimates was assessed by quantifying Cochran’s Q statistic, where P > 0.1 indicated the absence of heterogeneity among the instrumental variables, enabling the disregard of its influence on causal effect estimation [27]. For cases in which heterogeneity was detected, the IVW (multiplicative random-effects) approach was used to calculate the effect size. The Egger model intercept was used for statistical assessment of pleiotropy, with a deviation of 0, suggesting the presence of directional pleiotropy [28].

Multivariable Mendelian randomization

To validate the second core assumption of MR, which entails assessing the absence of associations between the IVs and confounders, we conducted a thorough search of the PhenoScanner database [29]. This search aimed to identify any previously reported significant associations (P < 5 × 10− 8) between the instrument SNPs and potential confounding factors. Notably, the PhenoScanner search identified associations between the instruments and traits related to obesity, alcohol consumption, and smoking. Consequently, we specifically selected SNPs associated with body mass index (BMI) [30], drinks per week [31], and cigarettes per day [31] from the publicly available GWAS summary statistics to conduct multivariable MR (MVMR) analyses to account for indirect pathways that may introduce correlated pleiotropy. Causal effects were estimated using the MVMR-IVW and MVMR-Egger methods. Heterogeneity among the selected genetic variants was assessed using Cochran’s Q statistic, and MVMR-Egger was employed to identify potential directional pleiotropy.

LDSC regression analysis

LDSC regression analysis is dependable and effective for detecting the shared genetic frameworks of intricate human characteristics [32]. It enables the estimation of disease heritability and the examination of genetic correlations using GWAS summary data. In the present study, we used GWAS summary data on the identified causal traits to assess their genetic associations. The LD reference panel, obtained from 1000 Genomes and developed by the researchers (source: https://github.com/bulik/ldsc), consisted of the European LD scores utilized in our analysis.

This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Using Mendelian Randomization (STROBE-MR) statement [33] (Additional file 1). All MR analyses were conducted using “TwoSampleMR,” “MRPRESSO,” “MendelianRandomization,” and “MVMR” in R software (version 4.0.2; the R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).

Results

Associations between leisure sedentary behaviors, sleep status and the FI

After ensuring quality control, we selected 138 SNPs related to watching television, 442 SNPs related to computer usage, 44 SNPs related driving, 62 SNPs related to sleep duration, 332 SNPs related to snoring, and 93 SNPs related to daytime napping as IVs. The MR study found that the genetic predisposition to spend time watching television (β = 0.26, 95% confidence interval [CI]:0.21–0.31, P = 3.98e-25) and daytime napping (β = 0.29, 95%CI: 0.18–0.41, P = 2.68e-07) were positively associated with the FI based on the IVW method, while the sleep duration (β = -0.18, 95%CI: -0.26, -0.10; P = 6.04e-06) was negatively associated with the FI (Table 1; Fig. 1). All MR results were robust in several sensitivity analyses (Table 1). There was no obvious heterogeneity for the genetic variants of time spent on driving (all P-values for Cochran’s Q > 0.1), whereas genetic instrumental variables of other exposures exhibited persistent heterogeneity (Additional file 2: Table S8). All P-values for the intercepts of MR-Egger tests were > 0.05 (Additional file 2: Table S9).

Table 1 Univariable MR results for the causal associations between leisure sedentary behaviors, sleep status and the FI
Table 2 Summary of the GWAS data used in the MR analyses
Fig. 1
figure 1

Scatter plot of the causal effect of watching television (A), sleep duration (B), and daytime napping (C) on FI using different MR methods

Multivariable MR analyses adjusting for confounders provided similar results and also suggested a positively causal effect of time spent on watching television (β = 0.27, 95% CI: 0.20–0.35, P = 1.19e-12) and daytime napping (β = 0.23, 95%CI: 0.10–0.36, P = 4.89e-04) and negative causal effect of sleep duration (β = -0.18, 95%CI: -0.27, -0.09; P = 1.55e-04) on the FI (Table 3). All directions and the statistical significance of the IVW results in MVMR were consistent with those of the MVMR-Egger sensitivity analysis, suggesting a low risk of bias due to horizontal pleiotropy (Table 3; Additional file 2: Table S9). The MVMR heterogeneity test validated sustained heterogeneity across the selected genetic variants (Additional file 2: Table S10).

Table 3 Multivariable MR results for the causal association of watching television, sleep duration, and daytime napping with FI

Genetic correlation between causal traits

We tested the positive genetic correlation between the time spent watching television (Rg = 0.43, P = 6.46e-48) and daytime napping (Rg = 0.25, P = 3.34e-21) and the FI. LDSC regression analysis revealed a negative genetic correlation between sleep duration (Rg = -0.20, P = 5.29e-10) and the FI.

Discussion

This MR study investigated the potential causal relationships between the leisure sedentary behaviors, sleep status and the FI. We found that time spent watching television, sleep duration, and daytime napping were causally associated with the FI. Multiple sensitivity analyses confirmed the robustness of these causal relationships. MVMR analyses demonstrated independent causal effects of watching television, sleep duration, and daytime napping on the FI, after adjustments for other confounders. We observed a significant genetic correlation between the time spent watching television, sleep duration, daytime napping, and the FI based on the LDSC regression.

Population-based investigations have consistently demonstrated that sedentary behavior contributes to adverse health outcomes. A meta-analysis revealed that reduced sedentary duration is significantly associated with a decreased risk of premature mortality, particularly among middle-aged and older adults, exhibiting a nonlinear dose-response pattern [34]. Considering the findings of our research and those from previous studies, reducing sedentary duration during leisure activities has potential benefits for mitigating the aging process.

Significant causal associations were observed between certain sleep status traits and the FI. Numerous studies have reported a relationship between sleep and aging. A study conducted in a Chinese population comprising 23,847 individuals revealed that maintaining a comprehensive and healthy sleep pattern was positively linked to a reduced risk of worsening frailty and an increased likelihood of improving frailty [10]. Another cross-sectional study utilizing data from the National Health and Aging Trends Study, identified difficulty initiating sleep as an independent risk factor for frailty [35]. Furthermore, a meta-analysis involving 313,651 participants from 20 cohort studies demonstrated that prolonged napping is associated with a higher risk of all-cause mortality [36]. After adjusting for relevant factors, we provide compelling evidence supporting significant causal relationships between sleep duration and daytime napping with the FI.

Our research emphasizes the genetic link between television-viewing time, sleep duration, daytime napping, and the FI. One notable strength of our study is the use of genetic data from a large sample population, which enhanced the reliability of our findings and minimized the impact of confounding factors. Furthermore, the robustness of IVW estimates in this study was supported by multiple MR sensitivity analyses, each incorporating different assumptions regarding genetic pleiotropy. Nevertheless, some limitations of this study should be acknowledged. First, our analysis assumed a linear association between risk factors and outcomes. Although the estimates reflect the presence and direction of the population-averaged causal effect, quantitative interpretations may be misleading if the actual relationship is nonlinear [37]. Second, genetic associations were derived from data of European populations, and caution should be exercised when generalizing these findings to other ethnic groups. Third, the MR analysis estimates were formulated to evaluate the causal impact of long-term exposure on outcomes and, therefore, may not always align precisely with clinical observations. Fourth, while we attempted to strictly adhere to the STROBE-MR guidelines, we were unable to provide all the recommended items because of the restricted information accessible from the utilized database.

Conclusion

In summary, we utilized MR techniques to provide valuable quantitative data on modifiable risk factors that causally influence the aging process. Understanding the causal effects of these risk factors on frailty holds considerable promise for elucidating the underlying mechanisms of the aging process and establishing potential strategies for preventing age-related diseases and promoting healthy aging. Notably, significant effects were observed for the time spent watching television, sleep duration, and daytime napping. These findings provide crucial insights into the determinants of biological aging and highlight potential areas for intervention to promote healthy longevity and attenuate the rate of biological aging.