1 Introduction

The AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety showed that an estimated 21% (6.400) of crashes in the USA involved a sleepy driver or a driver that had fallen asleep behind the wheel [1]. Driver sleepiness thus contributes to a significant number of traffic accidents, and continuous campaigns around the world warn for drowsy driving and point drivers to potential countermeasures to prevent being sleepy behind the wheel.

The effectiveness of several countermeasures for driver sleepiness has been scientifically investigated, including having a break, napping and the consumption of caffeinated beverages such as coffee or an energy drink. In particular, the effectiveness of caffeine to combat driver sleepiness and increase alertness has been the subject of many studies. For example, Horne and Reyner [2] examined the effects of caffeine on driving in sleep-deprived subjects. They drove for 30 min in a simulator, were given a placebo or a glucose-based energy drink (containing 160 mg caffeine) and drove for another 2 h. Thirty minutes after consumption of the energy drink, there were fewer driving incidents and reaction times were significantly improved when compared with the placebo condition. In a subsequent study applying a similar design, Reyner and Horne [3] confirmed that consumption of 250 ml of energy drink containing 80 mg caffeine significantly reduced the number of driving incidents. Subjective sleepiness was also significantly reduced. Several other studies also showed the positive effects of caffeinated beverages on driving performance in sleep-deprived drivers.

The current gold standard for outcome measure of driving tests is the standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP). SDLP, i.e., the weaving of the car, is an excellent outcome measure capable to demonstrate drug-induced impairment and dose–response effects, and has been applied successfully in drugs and driving research over the past 35 years [4, 5]. Using SDLP as an outcome measure, the effectiveness of caffeine has been demonstrated previously. For example, Mets et al. [6] examined the effects of energy drink (250 ml of Red Bull, containing 80 mg caffeine) on prolonged simulated highway driving. Subjects had to drive for 2 h in the simulator, had a 15-min break in which they consumed energy drink or a placebo drink, followed by another 2 h of driving. In a subsequent study with the same design, Mets et al. [7] examined the effects of coffee (80 mg caffeine) versus decaffeinated coffee. In both studies, in the caffeine condition, driving significantly improved in the 3rd and 4th hour (significantly reduced SDLP values when compared to the placebo condition), and driver sleepiness was significantly reduced.

In real life, it has been demonstrated that the consumption of caffeinated substances significantly reduces the chances of having traffic accidents. For example, Sharwood et al. [8] showed that Australian long-distance truck drivers who consumed caffeinated substances had a 63% reduced likelihood of having a traffic accident when compared to drivers that did not consume caffeinated substances.

Examining driving accident risk in an experimental setting (e.g., driving simulator) is troublesome, as under normal driving circumstances having an accident is a rare event. Thus, measuring accidents is not a useful outcome measure on driving tests. The relationship between SDLP and the risk of having an accident is unclear, although indirect evidence suggests such a relationship exists [9]. Therefore, the search for other proxy measures of accidents has continued. One of the proposed assays is number of lapses (i.e., periods of microsleep). It is assumed that during these short periods of inattention, the risk of having accidents is significantly increased. A lapse is a short period of inattention and is defined as a deviation from the mean lateral position for > 100 cm for 8 s or more [10]. An example of a lapse is given in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic representation of a lapse. Subjects were instructed to drive with a steady later position within the right traffic lane (see bottom figure). A lapse occurs if drivers deviate more than 100 cm from their chosen position for more than 8 s (see top figure)

In previous analyses, we showed that in healthy volunteers after intake of hypnotic drugs known to have residual effects, the number of lapses during a next-morning drive were significantly increased compared to a normal drug-free night of sleep [10]. Alternatively, in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) the number of lapses significantly reduced after treatment with methylphenidate [11].

It is important to demonstrate the efficacy of alertness-enhancing compounds using tests with relevance to real life activities which are compromised by sleepiness, such as prolonged highway driving. For this purpose, lapses are an ideal outcome measure. Therefore, the aim of the current study was to examine whether consumption of an energy drink while having a break has an effect of the number of lapses during prolonged simulated highway driving.

2 Methods

Data from Mets et al. [6] were re-analyzed. Twenty-four healthy volunteers participated in this double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover study. Subjects were included if they were healthy non-smokers, were moderate caffeine consumers (two to four glasses of caffeine-containing beverages per day, infrequent energy drink consumers (< 1 drink per month), and had regular sleeping hours, aged 21–35 years old. Subjects were included if they had a valid driver’s license for at least 3 years and drive a minimum of 5000 km per year. Subjects with sleep disorders or an Epworth Sleepiness Scale score above 10 were excluded from participation. Details on recruitment and inclusion and exclusion criteria are described by Mets et al. [6]. No formal ethical approval was required according to the Medical Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht for the present reanalysis. Ethics permission was obtained for the original two studies from the Medical Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht. Written informed consent was obtained from each subject before taking part in the study.

The study comprised a training session, and two test days. On each test day, subjects completed a 4-h simulated highway driving test, aiming to mimic the on-road driving test in actual traffic [12]. Driving tests were performed using STISIM Drive™ (version M300, Systems Technology, Inc., Hawthorne, CA, USA). To avoid practice effects, several variants of a 100-km highway driving test scenario, developed by EyeOctopus B.V., were used for this study.

Subjects were instructed to drive with a steady lateral position within the right lane while maintaining a constant speed of 95 km/h [4]. The primary outcome measure for the current analyses was the number of lapses.

After 2 h of driving, a 15-min break was scheduled. During the break, subjects consumed either 250 ml of energy drink (Red Bull, containing 21 g of sucrose, 5 g of glucose, 1 g of taurine, 80 mg of caffeine, 60 mg of glucuronolactone, 50 mg of inositol, and B vitamins) or a placebo energy drink (250 ml of Red Bull without caffeine, glucuronolactone, taurine, inositol and B-vitamins). The samples were provided by Red Bull GmbH. The latter was important to assure treatment blinding with regard to color and taste. The 15-min break was followed by another 2 h of driving.

Subjects were tested either in the morning (8:00–13:00 h) or afternoon (13:00–17:00 h) in a balanced manner and started each test day at the same time. Test days were separated by a wash-out period of at least 3 days.

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS, version 24. For each subject, and each condition, the number of lapses were calculated for each hour of driving. For each hour of driving, the non-parametric related samples Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to compare the number of lapses after consuming energy drink or placebo, and between the different hours of driving. Differences were considered significant if p < 0.05.

3 Results

Twenty-four subjects participated in the study. Three subjects were excluded due to protocol violations. Data from twenty-one subjects (nine men and twelve women) were used for the statistical analysis. Their mean (SD) age was 22.8 (1.4) years old.

Mean (SD) number of lapses after placebo and energy drink, and descriptive data are summarized in Table 1. The number of lapses after placebo and energy drink are visualized in Fig. 2.

Table 1 Number of lapses: descriptives
Fig. 2
figure 2

Number of lapses. Means and standard errors are shown. Compared to the second hour, after consuming energy drink, the number of lapses was significantly reduced in the third and fourth hour of driving. After placebo, the difference with the second hour was significant in the third hour of driving. However, in the fourth hour after placebo, the number of lapses were identical to those observed in the second hour

No significant differences were observed between the energy drink and placebo in the number of lapses observed in hour 1 (p = 1.00) and in hour 2 (p = 0.600).

Having a break, without consuming energy drink, significantly reduced the number of lapses in the third hour of driving when compared with the second hour of driving (p = 0.006). However, the number of lapses in the second hour of driving after placebo was identical (p = 1.000). When compared to the second hour of driving, consuming an energy drink during the break resulted in a significant reduction in lapses in the third hour (p = 0,012) and the fouth hour of driving (p = 0.041).

When directly comparing the two treatments, in hour 3, fewer lapses were observed after consuming energy drink when compared to the placebo drink, although the difference did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.129). In hour 4, significantly fewer lapses were observed after consuming energy drink when compared to the placebo drink (p = 0.024).

4 Discussion

This study showed that having a 15-min break in combination with consuming an energy drink significantly reduces the number of lapses during prolonged simulated highway driving. Thus, the consumption of energy drink showed to be an effective countermeasure for drowsy driving in non-sleep-deprived healthy subjects.

The number of lapses in the second hour of driving and fourth hour of driving after placebo was identical, suggesting that after 2 h of driving having a 15-min break is recommended per se.

Our findings are in line with the original analyses by Mets et al. [6], using SDLP as outcome measure. After consuming energy drink, SDLP was significantly reduced in both hour 3 and 4. In the current analyses, a significant reduction in the number of lapses was also seen in hour 3 and hour 4. In addition, the findings are in accordance with the significant reduction in driver sleepiness observed after consuming energy drink [6]. Our study confirms that the number of lapses increases over distance driven, and that having a break is an effective way to counteract drowsy driving and reduce the number of lapses. In combination with consuming an energy drink during the break, the reduction in lapses is significantly increased, illustrating the effectiveness of caffeinated beverages as countermeasure for drowsy driving.

A limitation of this study is the fact that we used a driving simulator and not tested the effects on a public highway in actual traffic. It is known that drivers may perform differently in a driving simulator, as no actual risks are involved [5]. This may also be reflected in reduced motivation to perform the test. Driving simulators also showed to be less sensitive in detecting relative modest drug effects when directly compared to on-road driving [13]. Further, sleepiness scores are usually higher in simulator settings when compared to on-road driving tests [14]. Future research should replicate these findings on-road in actual traffic. It would be interesting to record EEG activity during these driving tests and link these observations to the occurrence of lapses. This way it can be demonstrated that lapses are indeed short periods of inattention (i.e., microsleep) and how they directly relate to the driver’s performance.

In conclusion, consuming energy drink while having a break significantly reduces the number of lapses during prolonged simulated highway driving. Our findings support previous evidence that the consumption of caffeinated beverages contributes to a significant reduction in the likelihood of having traffic accidents.