Introduction

In recent years, much attention has focused on training the developing brain (see Jolles and Crone 2012 for a review), with the rationale being that training specific cognitive functions (i.e., attention and working memory) during development, can offer advantages in terms of learning capacity. Cognitive training interventions in children with neurodevelopmental disorders have demonstrated gains on trained and/or non-trained tasks (Kerns et al. 2017; Robinson et al. 2014). It is possible that neuroplasticity, which refers to the life-long process by which the brain adapts to changes in its environment (Pascual-Leone et al. 2005), may underlie these cognitive improvements. Training in adults primarily reshapes the existing neural landscape, while training in young children may influence the establishment of developing neural networks. It has therefore been suggested that childhood may be a sensitive period during which training has its largest effects (Jolles and Crone 2012).

Working memory has been the focus of cognitive training interventions in children and adolescents born very preterm (< 32 weeks of gestation) (Grunewaldt et al. 2013; Lohaugen et al. 2011; Everts et al. 2015), given they often report working memory problems (see Anderson 2014 for a review), which are particularly pronounced in those individuals born extremely preterm (EP; < 28 weeks of gestation) (Marlow 2004). Working memory problems are thought to underlie poor academic achievement in young people (Simone et al. 2017), which in turn affect their social and economic prospects later in life (Levine and Nourse 1998; Heath and Ross 2000). Previous working memory training studies in young preterm individuals have been conducted at preschool age (Grunewaldt et al. 2013), when cognitive resources to support training are not well developed, and in adolescence (Lohaugen et al. 2011), when working memory capacities are approaching adult levels, with potentially little scope for improvement (Jolles and Crone 2012; Gathercole 1999). In contrast, early school age may be a preferable period to train working memory, given that by this age, children have developed sufficient resources to support maturing working memory functioning. Further, at this age, brain regions that are involved in working memory (i.e., frontal and parietal cortices) are sufficiently developed to perform working memory tasks but are still maturing and can be influenced by the environment (through experience, exposure, and training), thereby allowing targeted interventions to utilize their residual neuroplastic capacity (Johnson 2011).

Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) is a fast, task-independent method for exploring the brain’s functional organization. The functional networks identified at rest closely correspond to networks that are active during the performance of specific tasks (Smith et al. 2009) and can predict task-induced brain activity or an individual’s task performance (Mennes et al. 2010; Zou et al. 2013). Most importantly, resting-state functional connectivity (rsfc) has been found to change as a result of learning (Lewis et al. 2009). In relation to working memory, results of previous studies showed that working memory performance was related to rsfc within central executive and salience networks (Engstrom et al. 2013), and within the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex of the default mode network (Sala-Llonch et al. 2012). However, to date, no papers have examined rsfc in relation to working memory training in EP children and/or extremely low birth weight (ELBW; < 1000 g birth weight). Studying training-induced rsfc changes in relation to training outcomes may help to increase understanding of the neural mechanisms associated with working memory in EP/ELBW children and aid in the refinement of interventions aimed at enhancing working memory capacity.

The current study used rs-fMRI to investigate training-induced neuroplasticity following a working memory training program (Cogmed Working Memory Training®) compared with a placebo training program in 7-year-old EP/ELBW children. Participants were part of a larger, double-blind randomized controlled trial. In the main trial, we failed to identify benefits of Cogmed on working memory up to 24 months post-training compared with the placebo program (Anderson et al. 2018); however, fMRI is powerful in detecting changes in hemodynamic responses even in the absence of observable effects on cognitive performance (Froudist-Walsh et al. 2015; Erk et al. 2011). We therefore hypothesized that training-induced changes in rsfc would be greater in the Cogmed group compared with the placebo group. We further explored whether training-induced changes in rsfc were associated with training-induced changes in cognitive performance, and whether this differed between treatment groups. We hypothesized that training-induced rsfc would be associated with cognitive improvements, regardless of group. Finally, associations between training-induced rsfc changes and improvements during the working memory training were explored within the Cogmed group. We hypothesized that greater training-induced rsfc changes would be associated with greater working memory training gains. These hypotheses are based on specific adaptive training effects previously reported that suggest a central role of load adaptation to support effective working memory gains (Klingberg et al. 2002, 2005; Holmes et al. 2009).

Materials and Methods

Study Participants

Participants in the current study were born at either < 28 completed weeks of gestation and/or with birth weights < 1000 g in the state of Victoria, Australia, in 2005 (Doyle et al. 2011, 2010), and were enrolled in a double-blinded, placebo-controlled randomized clinical trial of Cogmed Working Memory Training® (hereafter referred to as Cogmed) following parental consent (n = 91). The children were randomized to either the Cogmed training program group or the placebo group in a 1:1 ratio. The randomization schedule was generated by a biostatistician (K. L.) who was independent of the study, using block randomization with variable block sizes. The project coordinator (L. P.) and a research assistant managed the randomization. The participant and their family remained blinded to the assigned program allocation throughout the study. Other than the project coordinator and research assistant, all other members of the trial team were blinded to program allocation throughout the study.

Children were excluded if they had a severe intellectual, sensory, or physical impairment that affected their capacity to attend mainstream school (n = 16), were from families considered unable to comply with the training schedule (n = 2), or were not suitable for the study due to location or previous training exposure (n = 3). Of the 91 children enrolled in the trial, 42 children consented to optional MRI, completed working memory assessments, and had successful MRI scans both before (baseline) and 2 weeks after training (post-training). Loss of data included non-consent to the MRI, non-compliance to the MRI procedures, incomplete scans, and failure to return for follow-up neuroimaging. Of the 42 children who completed the working memory training baseline and post-training scans, 19 children were excluded due to excessive motion (> 0.35 mm relative mean displacement, approximately 0.7 mm frame displacement) for at least one time point, one child was excluded due to having a poor-quality T1-weighted image, and one child due to having dental braces which caused artifacts on the images. Therefore, the rsfc analyses were performed in 21 children with MRI data at both time points (Cogmed = 12; placebo = 9).

Cogmed Working Memory Training

Children were randomly assigned to receive either Cogmed or a placebo program. Cogmed includes a range of different computerized working memory activities that are practiced intensively (5 days per week for 5 to 7 weeks). The program is adaptive, such that the complexity level of individual activities varies with the child’s current working memory capacity on a trial-by-trial basis. The placebo program is identical to Cogmed, except that it is non-adaptive, with the complexity of activities set to an undemanding level throughout the training period. This design enabled us to explore possible neuroplastic changes specifically associated with adaptive working memory training (Anderson and Doyle 2008). Children attended assessments at the Royal Children’s Hospital (RCH), Melbourne, Australia, at both baseline and 2 weeks post-training.

MRI Acquisition

MRI at both baseline and post-training was undertaken without sedation using a Siemens 3 Tesla Magnetom Trio scanner and a 32-channel head coil at the RCH. Children underwent a 3D T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) sequence (0.85 mm sagittal slices, in-plane resolution 0.82 mm2, flip angle 9°, repetition time 1900 ms, echo time 2.27 ms, field of view 210 × 210 mm, matrix 256 × 256) and a rs-fMRI echo planar imaging sequence with prospective acquisition correction (PACE) (4.0 mm axial slices, in-plane resolution = 3.3 mm2, flip angle = 90°, repetition time = 2400 ms, echo time = 35 ms, field of view = 210 × 210 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, scan duration = 10 min).

Imaging Analysis

The researchers who analyzed the imaging data (C.E. J. T. and C. N.) were blinded regarding group assignment when conducting the imaging analysis. A customized template was created from the structural images of the 21 included children using Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs; http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/). The resting-state fMRI preprocessing steps were performed using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB) Software Library (FSL; www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) and included non-brain removal using Brain Extraction Tool, motion correction using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (Jenkinson et al. 2002), temporal filtering, slice-timing correction, spatial smoothing, normalization to the custom template using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (Jenkinson et al. 2002; Jenkinson and Smith 2001) and FMRIB’s Nonlinear Image Registration Tool (Andersson et al. 2010), and denoising using FMRIB’s independent component analysis–based Xnoiseifier (Salimi-Khorshidi et al. 2014; Griffanti et al. 2014). Group independent component analysis (ICA) was performed on the whole dataset (n = 21) using Group ICA of fMRI Toolbox (GIFT; http://icatb.sourceforge.net). GIFT uses a temporal concatenation approach, during which data reduction is performed via multi-staged principal component analysis and aggregation to generate common-group maps. These components are then back-projected onto each individual’s data to create subject-specific spatial maps with corresponding time courses (Calhoun et al. 2008). ICA was performed using the Infomax algorithm (Bell and Sejnowski 1995), and repeated 5 times with Icasso (Himberg et al. 2004) to maximize the stability of the derived components. Functional region of interests from the Functional Imaging in Neuropsychiatric Disorders lab (Shirer et al. 2012) and meta-analysis maps using the term “frontoparietal” from http://neurosynth.org were used as templates to identify components as resting-state networks. The FSL tool fslcc was used to find the component that had the highest cross-correlation with each functional network template. This component was visually inspected and determined as the network component if appropriate; if not, the component with the second highest cross-correlation was visually inspected. All networks were identified within the first two components. The following resting-state networks were identified: the salience network (SN; bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, middle frontal gyrus, insula, putamen, and thalamus), default mode network (DMN; bilateral medial prefrontal cortex, angular gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex, and hippocampus), dorsal attention network (DAN; bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, intraparietal sulcus, and lateral occipital cortex), precuneus network (PN; bilateral angular gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex, and precuneus), bilateral executive network (BEN; bilateral middle frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, superior parietal lobule, supramarginal gyrus, and left superior frontal gyrus and inferior temporal gyrus), and left and right central executive network (LCEN; left superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyrus, precuneus, and bilateral anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, RCEN; right middle and inferior frontal gyrus, angular gyrus, and supramarginal gyrus) (Fig. 1). Each identified network was represented by one ICA component; therefore, a total of seven components were included in the analysis. These resting-state networks were chosen because they have previously been identified as relating to working memory performance (Engstrom et al. 2013; Sala-Llonch et al. 2012; Hampson et al. 2006; Charroud et al. 2016; Fang et al. 2016). The strength of connectivity within individual networks was derived from the z-scored spatial maps and will be referred to as intra-network rsfc. The strength of connectivity between two networks (as estimated by the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between pairs of timecourses) will be referred to as inter-network rsfc. Subject-specific timecourses were detrended, despiked, and filtered using a fifth-order Butterworth low-pass filter with a high frequency cutoff of 0.15 Hz. All correlation coefficients were transformed to z-scores using Fisher’s transformation.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Resting-state networks of interest

The resting-state networks identified in this study were generated in the customized template space but are represented here on an MNI template with corresponding MNI coordinates.

Cognitive Measures

At both assessments, children completed selected subtests from the Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C) (Pickering and Gathercole 2001), namely Digit Recall, Word List Recall, Block Recall, Mazes Memory, and Backward Digit Recall. The Mister X subtest was completed from the Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA) (Alloway 2007). These measures were combined into two composite scores to represent immediate memory and working memory. The immediate memory composite score was a sum of the standardized scores of all WMTB-C subtests except the Backward Digit Recall subtest. The standardized scores of the Backward Digit Recall and Mister X subtests were summed to obtain the working memory composite score. The Cogmed Improvement Index score was used as a measure of improvement on the trained activities, which was available for the Cogmed group only. This score was obtained by calculating the difference between the Start index (i.e., the mean of the three best successful trials on days 2 and 3) and the Max index (i.e., the mean of the three best successful trials on the two best training days). All assessments were conducted blinded to knowledge of treatment group allocation by trained research assistants and psychologists at the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute in Melbourne, Australia.

Statistical Analysis

Training-induced changes (from baseline to post-training) in rsfc and memory composite scores were compared between the Cogmed and placebo groups, adjusting for corresponding baseline values of the measure of interest (e.g., participants’ change in Digit Recall was adjusted for their Digit Recall baseline scores). To adjust for baseline values, robust regression and a logistic weight function were used; residuals were then used for independent t tests and subsequent analysis. The association between training-induced changes in intra-/inter-network rsfc (predictor) and changes in memory composite scores (outcome) were explored using linear regression. We investigated whether these associations differed between treatment groups by testing for an interaction between treatment group and rsfc change in the linear regression models. Within the Cogmed group, additional analyses were performed to explore the association between training improvements and changes in rsfc. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg approach (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995). Findings that do not survive multiple comparison correction are referred to as “findings at a trend-level”.

Results

Study participants (n = 21) and non-participants (n = 70) had similar characteristics in terms of sex, gestational age, and birth weight; however, participants were slightly older in age compared with non-participants (participants: mean age ± standard deviation = 7.7 ± 0.6; non-participants: mean age ± standard deviation = 7.2 ± 0.4). Demographic and neonatal characteristics of the study participants are shown in Table 1. As expected, the two groups were similar with regard to their baseline characteristics.

Table 1 Demographic and neonatal characteristics of the study participants

There was little evidence (i.e., we were unable to reject the null hypothesis of prior to multiple comparison correction) of a difference in the changes in intra-network rsfc (Table 2), inter-network rsfc (Table 3), and immediate memory or working memory (Table 4), from baseline to post-training between the two treatment groups (aim 1). Changes in intra-network rsfc in DAN and DMN were greater in the Cogmed group compared with the placebo group at a trend-level (Table 2 and Fig. 2). Positive scores indicate higher functional connectivity at follow-up than at baseline, while negative scores indicate higher functional connectivity at baseline than at follow-up.

Table 2 Baseline to post-training changes in intra-network rsfc between the Cogmed and placebo groups
Table 3 Baseline to post-training changes in inter-network rsfc between the Cogmed and placebo groups
Table 4 Baseline to post-training changes in working memory between the Cogmed and placebo groups
Fig. 2
figure 2

Scatter plots of the intra-network rsfc changes in DAN and DMN. rsfc resting state functional connectivity, DAN dorsal attention network, DMN default mode network. The middle line indicates the mean, with each line below and above indicating one standard deviation.

There was evidence that precuneus intra-network rsfc changes were associated with working memory composite score changes in the Cogmed group (regression coefficient (95% CI) = 5.92 (2.31, 9.53), p = 0.004), but not in the placebo group (regression coefficient (95% CI) = − 0.69 (− 3.69, 2.32), p = 0.60) (interaction p = 0.05, Fig. 3). There was little evidence of associations between intra- or inter-network rsfc changes and working memory changes for the other networks investigated. There was also little evidence of any association between changes in intra- or inter-network rsfc and immediate memory composite scores.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Association between training-induced change in working memory composite score and precuneus network intra-network rsfc change, in the Cogmed and placebo groups. PN precuneus network, rsfc resting state functional connectivity.

In terms of training improvement in the Cogmed group, findings showed associations between higher Cogmed Improvement Index scores, which reflect the capacity to improve across training trials, and decreased baseline to post-training rsfc change within the RCEN (intra-network) rsfc (regression coefficient (95% CI) = − 17.17 (− 29.38, − 4.96), p = 0.011), and between the RCEN and the SN (inter-network) rsfc (regression coefficient (95% CI) = − 17.15 (− 29.62, − 4.69), p = 0.012) at a trend-level, as well as increased baseline to post-training rsfc change between the LCEN and the BEN (regression coefficient (95% CI) = 19.29 (− 0.32, 38.90), p = 0.053) at a trend-level (Fig. 4). There was little evidence of associations between intra- or inter-network rsfc change and Cogmed training improvement for any of the other networks investigated.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Cogmed Improvement Index and resting state functional connectivity (rsfc) changes. RCEN right central executive network, SN salience network, LCEN left central executive network, BEN bilateral central executive network

Discussion

Our findings demonstrated that changes in working memory and rsfc in EP/ELBW children from baseline to post-training were not different between the Cogmed and placebo groups. However, differences between groups were found in terms of the relationship between training-induced working memory and rsfc changes. Evidence of an association between changes in working memory and training-induced rsfc changes within the precuneus network was observed in the Cogmed group, but not in the placebo group. In relation to improvements on trained activities for the Cogmed group, associations were found for decreased rsfc within the right central executive network and between the right central executive and precuneus networks and increased rsfc between the left central executive and bilateral executive networks at a trend-level.

In the Cogmed group, we found an association between working memory change and rsfc connectivity change in the precuneus network. Greater improvement in working memory performance was mostly associated with increased positive rsfc change. The precuneus has previously been suggested to play a central role in performing tasks that involve first-person perspective, such as visuospatial imagery, self-referential processing, and episodic memory retrieval (Cavanna and Trimble 2006). It is also thought to have the highest resting metabolic rate within the DMN and is extensively connected to cortical and subcortical regions, suggesting a role in integrating both internally and externally driven information (Cavanna and Trimble 2006). For instance, increased rsfc has been previously reported between the precuneus and the DMN at rest, and between the precuneus and the right frontoparietal network during completion of a decision-making task (Utevsky et al. 2014). The observed association between greater precuneus intra-network rsfc change and greater working memory improvements following training in this study may be indicative of improved information integration through the precuneus. Similarly, a previous study showed that training-induced hemodynamic response changes in the precuneus during a cognitive training video game predicted performance improvement on a non-trained working memory task post-training in young healthy adults (aged 22, n = 45) (Nikolaidis et al. 2014). The authors suggested that improvement on tasks similar to training tasks or activities (referred to as near transfer) occurs when training-induced plasticity is evident in brain regions that are implicated in the non-trained task (Nikolaidis et al. 2014). Another study in young adults (aged 22, n = 15) reported that working memory improvements after practicing a working memory task (for 6 weeks) were associated with increased frontoparietal connectivity (including the precuneus in the parietal region), and decreased DMN connectivity (between the medial prefrontal cortex and the precuneus) (Jolles et al. 2013). However, the authors did not find these practice effects in a group of children (age 12, n = 9), possibly due to sample size limitations resulting in a lack of power to find such effects. Nonetheless, the results of the aforementioned studies highlight the dynamic interaction between task-related brain regions and the precuneus, as well as the importance of connectivity between networks to support working memory task performance.

The precuneus network is not commonly investigated in resting-state fMRI studies as it shows some overlap with the DMN. However, it has been suggested that the precuneus network may have a different role than the DMN, although the two networks converge with age, as shown in a cross-lifespan dataset (N = 126, 7–85 years old) (Yang et al. 2014). Therefore, it may be the case that the precuneus network is only distinguishable at a younger age, such as in extremely preterm children in our study. Another viewpoint is that the precuneus is not part of the DMN, as rsfc studies have found that the DMN terminates near the subparietal sulcus and exclusively involves the posterior cingulate cortex (Margulies et al. 2009; Buckner et al. 2008). Future studies in regard to the development of functional networks across age and specificity of the regions involved are needed.

In our study, although at a trend-level, greater improvements in the trained activities for the Cogmed group was associated with baseline to post-training rsfc changes within or between the following networks: (1) right central executive network, (2) right central executive and salience networks, and (3) left central executive and bilateral executive networks. The rsfc changes range from positive to negative values (i.e., a positive value means higher rsfc at follow-up than at baseline; a negative value means higher rsfc at baseline than at follow-up), which may reflect the shift in levels of cognitive effort in each individual. It is suggested that brain regions are initially more taxed when learning a new task, and with training, the task becomes less demanding and requires less activation (Buschkuehl et al. 2012). Therefore, the decrease of rsfc within the right central executive network along with Cogmed improvement possibly reflects reduced cognitive effort on working memory tasks post-training, as training may have increased children’s proficiency at performing such tasks. This interpretation is similar to another study in very preterm children (< 32 weeks’ gestation), which observed decreased hemodynamic responses in frontoparietal regions during a visual working memory task and increased memory performance after memory strategy training and intensive working memory practice (Everts et al. 2015). Similarly, children with and without developmental dyscalculia showed improved performance and reduced recruitment of task-related brain regions during a spatial number representation task after mental number line training (Kucian et al. 2011). Decreased hemodynamic responses in task-related brain regions have also been commonly reported in working memory training studies in adolescents and adults (Buschkuehl et al. 2012). Taken together with our findings, it seems that increased efficiency of working memory processes occurs after training, resulting in decreased recruitment of task-related brain regions (frontal and parietal regions).

Similar to our study, a randomized controlled trial assessed the effects of Cogmed compared with a placebo program on functional connectivity, but in typically developing children and using magnetoencephalography rather than fMRI. The study found training-induced rsfc increases between frontoparietal networks and other cortical areas, with working memory gains mirroring increases in rsfc (Astle et al. 2015). Our findings showed both increased and decreased inter-network connectivity in relation to improvement in trained activities, possibly highlighting the dynamic interaction between networks (although not surviving multiple comparison correction). These Cogmed-specific findings could be attributed to the adaptive nature of the Cogmed working memory training program. It is possible that adaptive training is more likely to induce brain-behavior changes compared with non-adaptive training. However, the causal relationship between adaptive training, working memory, and rsfc cannot be inferred from this study and warrants future research.

Greater intra-network rsfc changes in the DAN and DMN were found, at a trend-level, in the Cogmed group compared to the placebo group. However, rsfc changes in neither of these two networks were associated with working memory changes in this study, demonstrating that differences in rsfc changes between training groups can occur without associated changes in behavioral manifestation (Pascual-Leone et al. 2005). It is also possible that the behavioral changes occurred in other untested domains.

Ours is the first study to investigate the effects of Cogmed on brain function in EP/ELBW children. Using a functional network perspective, we were able to evaluate changes in network rsfc strength and interactions, rather than limiting to regional rsfc changes. Associations between training-induced rsfc change and changes in trained activities for only the Cogmed group were found, despite little evidence of group differences between the Cogmed and placebo groups in rsfc and working memory change. This is contrary to our hypothesis that associations between training-induced rsfc change and improvements in trained activities would be similar regardless of training group and suggests the importance of exploring within group neural-cognitive associations.

The current study was limited by sample size, and therefore, findings need to be interpreted with caution and re-evaluated with a larger sample. It is important to note that there was little evidence for differences in working memory between the Cogmed and placebo groups at 24 months post-training in the main trial which may also have limited our power (Anderson et al. 2018). It remains to be ascertained whether EP/ELBW children require a longer training period or more training sessions to result in greater behavioral effects (baseline to post-training working memory gains), due to reduced neuroplasticity (Pitcher et al. 2012). It should be noted that the Cogmed Improvement Index score, which we used to assess improvement in trained activities, does not necessarily reflect the final effect of training as the Max index is not necessarily from the last trial. This score may better represent the capacity of improvement across training trials. Additionally, especially in children, subject motion is an issue that may affect functional connectivity measures. In our analyses we used well-established ICA-based denoising methods that have been shown to be effective in reducing spatially specific motion artifacts and also some global artifacts, the two main sources of motion artifacts in resting-state data (Burgess et al. 2016).

The current study demonstrates that working memory performance following Cogmed was associated with selective functional connectivity changes in EP/ELBW children. Further studies are needed to better understand whether more extensive training, possibly eliciting greater neuroanatomical changes, can lead to behavioral improvements and support cognitive growth in vulnerable groups.