Skip to main content
Log in

Reaction Times and Observing-Responses in Equivalence Classes: Cognitive Processing and Fluency

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two three-member equivalence classes were formed with abstract three-dimensional objects that were observed by touch only by use of matching-to-sample (MTS) trials and the simple-to-complex protocol. Classes emerged immediately for one participant and with a delay in transitivity for the other. For all relational types, reaction time (RT) duration and observing-response (OR) frequency decreased with trial repetition to the very low asymptotes. For the derived relations, error-free decrements implied “learning” without feedback. The highly correlated ORs and RTs suggested that OR frequency accounted for RT duration. Also, information-gathering efficiency per OR increased with trial repetition. Thus, RT duration reflected OR frequency and cognitive processing. Initial RTs were long for baseline trials, much shorter for symmetry probes, intermediate for transitivity probes, and shortest for equivalence probes. More ORs occurred to samples than comparisons and the positive and negative comparisons produced similar OR frequencies. In addition, transitivity performances were predicted by frequency of sample-observing in previously administered baseline and symmetry probes. All findings were based on single-trial data. Finally, observing behavior may also influence the emergence of fluency.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, B. J., Fields, L., & Verhave, Y. (1993). Effects of the test order on the establishment and expansion of equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 43, 133–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Annett, J. M., & Leslie, J. C. (1995). Stimulus equivalence classes involving olfactory stimuli. The Psychological Record, 45, 439–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arntzen, E., & Haugland, S. (2012). Titration of limited hold to comparison in conditional discrimination training and equivalence testing. The Psychological Record, 62, 243–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arntzen, E., Petursson, P. I., Sadeghi, P., & Eilifsen, C. (2015). Equivalence class formation in accuracy or speed conditions: Immediate emergence, adduction, and retention. The Psychological Record, 65, 141–159. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0097-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, D., Browne, M., Smeets, P., & Roche, B. (1995). A transfer of functions and a conditional transfer of functions through equivalence relations in three- to six-year old children. The Psychological Record, 45, 405–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belanich, J., & Fields, L. (1999). Tactual equivalence classes formation and cross modal transfer. The Psychological Record, 49, 75–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P., Dickins, D. W., & Fox, S. R. A. (1993). Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46B, 187–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buffington, D. M., Fields, L., & Adams, B. J. (1997). Enhancing equivalence class formation by pretraining of other equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 47, 69–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, K. M. (1993). Stimulus equivalence and cross-modal transfer. The Psychological Record, 43, 567–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, K. M., Sidman, M., & De Rose, T. (1989). Contextual control of emergent `equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 29–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cowley, B. J., Green, G., & Braunling-McMorrow, D. (1992). Using stimulus equivalence procedures to teach name-face matching to adults with brain injuries. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 461–475.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DeGrandpre, R. J., Bickel, W. K., & Higgins, S. T. (1992). Emergent equivalence relations between interoceptive (drug) and exteroceptive (visual) stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 9–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dickins, D. D., & Dickins, B. J. A. (2009). Evidence from reaction times for an anticipatory process in symbolic delayed matching-to- sample. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 10, 167–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1995a). Stimulus control: Part I. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 51–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1995b). Stimulus control: Part II. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 253–270.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., Green, G., & Serna, R. W. (1993). Auditory successive conditional discrimination and auditory stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 103–114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2001). Supplemental measures and derived stimulus relations. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 19, 8–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eikeseth, S., & Smith, T. (1992). The development of functional and equivalence classes in high-functioning autistic children: the role of naming. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 123–133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., & Reeve, K. F. (2000). Equivalence classes and natural categories from the generalization of perceptual and relational classes. In J. C. Leslie & D. Blackman (Eds.), Issues in experimental and applied analyses of human behavior (pp. 59–84). Context Press.

  • Fields, L., & Verhave, T. (1987). The structure of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 317–332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T., & Newman, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345–358.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Reeve, K. F., Adams, B. J., & Verhave, T. (1991). Stimulus generalization and equivalence classes: a model for natural categories. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 305–312.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Newman, S., Adams, B. J., & Verhave, T. (1992). The expansion of equivalence classes through simple discrimination training and fading. The Psychological Record, 42, 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Landon-Jimenez, D. V., Buffington, D. M., & Adams, B. J. (1995). Maintained nodal–distance effects in equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 129–145.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Arntzen, E., & Doran, E. (2020a). Yield as an essential measure of equivalence class formation, other measures, and new determinants. The Psychological Record, 70, 175–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Doran, E., & Foxe, J. (2020b). Equivalence class formation when responding is separated from sample and comparison stimuli: Working memory, priming, and sorting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.651

  • Fienup, D. M., Wright, N. A., & Fields, L. (2015). Optimizing equivalence based instruction: Effects of training protocols on equivalence class formation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man: Part I. Brain, 88, 237–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, G. (1990). Differences in development of visual and auditory-visual equivalence relations. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95, 260–270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, G., Sigurdardottir, Z. G., & Saunders, R. R. (1991). The role of instructions in the transfer of ordinal functions through equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 287–304.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, S., & Arntzen, E. (2018). Eye movements during conditional discrimination training and equivalence class formation. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 22, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2018.1553379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56(1), 51–65. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0062474

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., Tilley, K. J., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Extending equivalence class membership to gustatory stimuli. The Psychological Record, 38, 473–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 119–137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Huziwara, E. M., Silva, M. J. M., Perez, W. F., & Tomanari, G. Y. (2015). Analyzing the stimulus control acquisition in simple discrimination tasks through eye movement. Psicologia: Reflexão e. Critica, 28, 603–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. R., & Layng, T. V. J. (1994). Breaking the structuralist barrier: Literacy and numeracy with fluency. American Psychologist, 47, 1485–1490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, I. T., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1966). Oculomotor patterns during the solution of visually displayed anagrams. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 447–451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C. H. (1991). Equivalence class formation influenced by the number of nodes separating stimuli. Behavioural Processes, 24, 219–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376-6357(91)90077-D

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C. H., Itkonen, T., & Indquist, K. (1994). Nodality effects during equivalence class formation: An extension to sight-word reading and concept development. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 673–683. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1994.27-673

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kirshner, H., & Sidman, M. (1972). Scanning patterns in aphasic patients during matching-to-sample tests. Neuropsychologia, 10, 179–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krekling, S., Tellevik, J. M., & Nordvik, H. (1989). Tactual learning and cross-modal transfer of an oddity problem in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 88–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazar, R. (1977). Extending sequence-class membership with matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 381–192.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindauer, M. S., Stergio, E. I., & Penn, D. L. (1986). Seeing and touching aesthetic objects: I. Judgements. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 24, 121–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, D. C., & Green, G. (1991). Development and cross-modal transfer of contextual control of emergent stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 139–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., Serna, R. W., Dube, W. V., & Stromer, R. (2000). Stimulus control topography and stimulus equivalence: Reconciling test outcomes with theory. In J. C. Leslie & D. Blackman (Eds.), Experimental and applied analysis of human behavior (pp. 85–110). Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, K. L., & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1986). The effect of negative stimulus presentations on observing-response rates. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 281–291.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Perez-Gonzalez, L. A., Spradlin, J. E., & Saunders, K. A. (2000). Learning set outcome in second order conditional discriminations. The Psychological Record, 50, 429–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pilgrim, C., & Galizio, M. (1996). Stimulus equivalence: A class of correlations or a correlation of classes? In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals. Elsevier Science B. V.

  • Rabinowitz, F. M. (1983). Cross-modal transfer and intermediate-size transposition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 223–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. A., & Orlian, E. K. (1991). Asymmetries in infant cross-modal transfer. Child Development, 62, 706–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., & Green, G. (1992). The nonequivalence of behavioral and mathematical equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 227–241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., & Green, G. (1999). A discrimination analysis of training-structure effects on stimulus equivalence outcomes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 117–137.

  • Saunders, R. R., Saunders, K. J., Kirby, K. C., & Spradlin, J. E. (1988). The merger and development of equivalence classes by unreinforced conditional selection of comparison stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 145–162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schenk, J. J. (1995). Complex stimuli in nonreinforced simple discrimination tasks: Emergent simple and conditional discriminations. The Psychological Record, 45, 477–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schusterman, R., & Kastak, D. (1993). A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) is capable of forming equivalence relations. The Psychological Record, 43, 823–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1952). A note on functional relations obtained with group data. Psychological Bulletin, 49, 263–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1960a). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluating experimental data in psychology. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1960b). Adventitious control by the location of comparison stimuli in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 173–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1980). A note on the measurement of conditional discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 285–289.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1992). Adventitious control by the location of comparison stimuli in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 173–182.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Authors Cooperative.

  • Sidman, M., Cresson, O., & J. (1973). Reading and cross-modal transfer of stimulus equivalences in severe retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77, 515–523.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: an expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Cresson, O., & J., & Willson-Morris, M. (1974). Acquisition of matching to sample via mediated transfer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 261–273.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sigurdardottir, S. G., Green, G., & Saunders, R. R. (1990). Equivalence classes generated by sequence training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 47–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. B., Dickins, D. W., & Bentall, R. P. (1996). The role of individual stimulus names in the emergence of equivalence relations II: The effects of interfering tasks prior to and after tests for emergent relations. The Psychological Record, 46, 109–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soares-Filho, P. S. D., Mosolino de Carvalho, L., Hamasaki, E. I. M., Campos, H. C., Perez, W. F., Arntzen, E., & Tomanari, G. Y. (2019). Manual-observing procedure: An alternative to the investigation of stimulus control and equivalence classes in matching-to-sample. The Psychological Record, 69, 165–174. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-00328-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spear, J., & Fields, L. (2015a). Effects of two parameters of joint stimulus control training on the induction of expressive writing: Learning to write without writing. Learning & Behavior, 43, 354–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spear, J., & Fields, L. (2015b). Learning to write without writing: Writing accurate descriptions of interactions after learning graph-printed description relations. Learning & Behavior, 43, 354–375. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0184-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, T. J., & Chase, P. N. (1996). Speed analyses of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 643–659.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spradlin, J. E., Cotter, V. W., & Baxley, N. (1973). Establishing a conditional discrimination without direct training: a study of transfer with retarded adolescents. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77, 556–566.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stollnitz, F. (1965). Spatial variables, observing-responses, and discrimination learning sets. Psychological Review, 72, 247–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stromer, R., McIlvane, W. J., & Serna, R. W. (1993). Complex stimulus control and equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 585–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tierny, K. J., De Largy, P., & Bracken, M. (1995). Formation of an equivalence class incorporating haptic stimuli. The Psychological Record, 45, 431–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varelas, A., & Fields, L. (2015). Induction of a generalized transitivity repertoire via multiple-exemplar training and staged testing. The Psychological Record, 65, 595–614. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-015-0129-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vie, A., & Arntzen, E. (2017). Talk-aloud protocols during conditional discrimination training and equivalence class formation. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 33(1), 80–97. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-017-0081-y

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, B., Karlan, G. R., & Spradlin, J. E. (1983). The development of derived stimulus relations through training in arbitrary-matching sequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 69–78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. A. (1984). Stimulus control and associative learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 469–483.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A., Cook, R. G., Rivera, J. J., Sands, S. F., & Delius, J. D. (1988). Concept learning by pigeons: Matching-to-sample with trial-unique video picture stimuli. Animal Learning and Behavior, 16, 436–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). The transfer of conditional sequencing through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50(2), 125–144.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., Greenway, D. E., & Dougher, M. J. (1994). Third-order equivalence classes. The. Psychological Record, 44, 411–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyckoff Jr., L. B. (1952). The role of observing-responses in discrimination learning. Part I. Psychological Review, 59, 431–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lanny Fields.

Ethics declarations

Data availability

Data can be obtained by contacting the second author.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants, including informed consent, were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research boards of the sites at which the cited experiments were performed, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fields, L., Belanich, J. Reaction Times and Observing-Responses in Equivalence Classes: Cognitive Processing and Fluency. Psychol Rec 73, 53–65 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-022-00519-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-022-00519-9

Keywords

Navigation