Skip to main content
Log in

A California Sea Lion (Zalophus Californianus) is Capable of Forming Equivalence Relations

  • Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

If a nonhuman animal matches the silhouette “crab” (A) to that of a “tulip” (B) and is further taught to match “tulip” (B) to the silhouette “radio” (C), will it immediately match “radio” (C) to “crab” (A)? To date formation of an equivalence relation of this type has not been demonstrated in animals. In our study, designed to give a sea lion match-to-sample experience with examples of sample and comparison stimuli switching roles, a 7-year-old female (Rio) was trained and tested with 30 potential classes, each consisting of 3 different shapes. Twelve of the 30 classes were used for training relational properties of symmetry and transitivity, and 18 classes were reserved for a final equivalence test. Following an initial failure to do symmetry on the first trial of novel relations (B→A: 8/12), Rio did symmetry (C→B: 11/12) and transitivity. (A→C: 11/12) before mastering equivalence on the first trial of 18 novel relations (C→A: 16/18). Results suggest that equivalence concepts are not mediated by language, but may be a prerequisite for linguistic competence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ADAMS, B. J., FIELDS, L., & VERHAVE, T. (1993). Effects of test order on intersubject variability during equivalence class formation. The Psychological Record, 43, 133–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • BHATT, R. S. (1988). Categorization in pigeons: Effects of category size, congruity with human categories, selective attention, and secondary generalization. Ph.D. thesis: Iowa City: University of Iowa.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, A. C. (1980). Autoclitic processes and the structure of behavior. Behaviorism, 8, 175–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, A. C. (1992). Learning ( 3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHENEY, D. L., & SEYFARTH, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’AMATO, M. R., SALMON, D., LOUKAS, E., & TOMIE, A. (1985). Symmetry and transitivity of conditional relations in monkeys (Cebus apella) and pigeons (Columba livia). Journal of the Experiment Analysis of Behavior, 44, 35–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • DUGDALE, N., & LOWE, C. F. (1990). Naming and stimulus equivalence. In D. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behavior analysis in theory and practice (pp. 115–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • GISINER, R., & SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. (1992). Sequence, syntax and semantics: Responses of a language-trained sea lion (Zalophus californianus) to novel sign combinations. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 78–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C. (1989). Nonhumans have not yet shown stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 385–392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HERMAN, L. M. (1988). The language of animal language research: Reply to Schusterman and Gisiner. The Psychological Record, 38, 349–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HERMAN, L. M. (1989). In which procrustean bed does the sea lion sleep tonight? The Psychological Record, 39, 19–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • KASTAK, D. A., & SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. (1992). Comparative cognition in marine mammals: A clarification on match-to-sample tests. Marine Mammal Science, 8, 414–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KASTAK, D., & SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. (1993). Immediate transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • KENNEDY, C. H. (1991). Equivalence class formation influenced by the number of modes separating stimuli. Behavioral Processes, 24, 219–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LIPKENS, R., KOP, P. F. M., & MATTHIJS, W. (1988). A test of symmetry and transitivity in the conditional discrimination of performances of pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 395–409.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MARKL, H. (1985). Manipulation, modulation, information, cognition: Some of the riddles of communication. In B. Holldobler & M. Lindauer (Eds.), Experimental behavioral ecology and sociobiology (pp. 163–194). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, D. (1986). “Gavagai” or the future history of the animal language controversy Cambridge, MA: Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAVAGE-RUMBAUGH, E. S. (1986). Ape language: From conditioned response to symbol. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAVAGE-RUMBAUGH, S., MC DONALD, K., SEVCIK, R. A., HOPKINS, W. D., & RUBERT, E. (1986). Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 115, 211–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., (1990). Stimulus equivalence and cross-modal perception: A testable model for demonstrating symbolic representations in bottlenose dolphins. In J. Thomas & R. Kastelein (Eds.), Sensory abilities of cetaceans (pp. 677–684). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., & GISINER, R. (1988). Artificial language comprehension in dolphins and sea lions: The essential cognitive skills. The Psychological Record, 38, 311–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., & GISINER, R. (1989). Please parse the sentence: Animal cognition in the Procrustean bed of linguistics. The Psychological Record, 39, 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J. & GISINER, R. (in press). Pinnipeds, porpoises and parsimony: Animal language research viewed from a bottom-up perspective. In R. W. Mitchell, H. L. Miles, & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals: The emperor’s new clothes? Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., GISINER, R., GRIMM, B. G., & HANGGI, E. B. (1993). Behavior control by exclusion and attempts at establishing semanticity in marine mammals using matching-to-sample paradigms. In H. Roitblat, L. Herman, & P. Nachtigall (Eds.), Language and communications: A comparative perspective (pp. 249–275). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., GRIMM, B. K., GISINER, R., & HANGGI, E. B. (1991). Retroactive interference of delayed “symbolic” matching-to-sample in California sea lions. Paper given at the Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society in San Francisco.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • SCHUSTERMAN, R. J., HANGGI, E. B., & GISINER, R. (1992). Acoustic signalling in mother-pup reunions, interspecies bonding, and affiliation by kinship in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). In J. Thomas, R. Kastelein, & A. Supin (Eds.), Marine mammal sensory systems (pp. 533–551). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1990). Equivalence relations: Where do they come from? In D. Blackman & H. Lejuene (Eds.), Behavior analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 93–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., RAUZIN, R., LAZAR, R., CUNNINGHAM, S., TAILBY, W., & CARRIGAN, P. (1982). A search for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of rhesus monkeys, baboons, and children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 23–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., & TAILBY, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • TERRACE, H. S. (1986). Nim: A chimpanzee who learned sign language. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • THOMAS, R. K. (1980). Evolution of intelligence: An approach to its assessment. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 17, 454–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • TOMONAGA, M., MATSUZAWA, T., FUJITA, K., & YAMAMOTO, J. (1991). Emergence of symmetry in visual conditional discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychological Reports, 68, 51–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WRIGHT, A. A., COOK, R. G., RIVERA, J. J., SANDS, S. F., & DELIUS, J. D. (1988). Concept learning by pigeons: Matching-to-sample with trial-unique video picture stimuli. Animal Learning and Behavior, 18, 287–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The experimental work reported in this paper was funded by contract N00014-85-K-0244 from the Office of Naval Research and by a grant from the Center for Field Research to Ronald J. Schusterman. Bob Gisiner, Brigit Grimm, Evelyn Hanggi, Michel Coulliais, and Ema Spada helped enormously during the early stages of this project and we thank them for their many contributions. Later on Colleen Reichmuth, Tammy Berger, Alyson Henschel, Leif Johnson and several student interns and assistants and many Earthwatch volunteers gave us invaluable assistance.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schusterman, R.J., Kastak, D. A California Sea Lion (Zalophus Californianus) is Capable of Forming Equivalence Relations. Psychol Rec 43, 823–839 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395915

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395915

Navigation