Skip to main content
Log in

On Quantitative Comparative Research in Communication and Language Evolution

  • Long Article
  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Quantitative comparison of human language and natural animal communication requires improved conceptualizations. We argue that an infrastructural approach to development and evolution incorporating an extended interpretation of the distinctions among illocution, perlocution, and meaning can help place the issues relevant to quantitative comparison in perspective. The approach can illuminate the controversy revolving around the notion of functional referentiality as applied to alarm calls, for example in the vervet monkey. We argue that referentiality offers a poor point of quantitative comparison across language and animal communication in the wild. Evidence shows that even the newborn human cry could be deemed to show functional referentiality according to the criteria typically invoked by advocates of referentiality in animal communication. Exploring the essence of the idea of illocution, we illustrate an important realm of commonality among animal communication systems and human language, a commonality that opens the door to more productive, quantifiable comparisons. Finally, we delineate two examples of infrastructural communicative capabilities that should be particularly amenable to direct quantitative comparison across humans and our closest relatives.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2006a) Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441:303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2006b) The alarm-calling system of adult male putty-nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Anim Behav 72:643–653

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Austin JL (1962) How to do things with words. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman R, Brown JV (1977) Behavior dialogues: an approach to the assessment of mother–infant interaction. Child Dev 49:195–203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr RG, Konner M, Bakeman R et al (1991) Crying in pKung San infants: a test of the cultural specificity hypothesis. Dev Med Child Neurol 33:601–610

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates E, Benigni L, Bretherton I et al (1979) The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom L (1970) Language development. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton I (1988) How to do things with one word: the ontogenesis of intentional message making. In: Smith MD, Locke JL (eds) The emergent lexicon: the child’s development of a linguistic vocabulary. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 225–260

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt A (1990) Speech acts, meaning and intentions: critical approaches to the philosophy of John R. Searle. de Gruyter, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth G (1996) Species typical aspects of manual pointing and the emergence of language in human infancy. Waseda University International Conference Center, Tokyo

  • Chen X, Green JA, Gustafson GE (2009) Development of vocal protests from 3 to 18 months. Infancy 14:44–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clay Z, Zuberbühler K (2009) Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos. Anim Behav 77:1387–1396

    Google Scholar 

  • Clay Z, Zuberbühler K (2011) Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. PLoS One 6(4):1–10

    Google Scholar 

  • Condillac EB (1756) An essay on the origin of human knowledge; being a supplement to Mr. Locke’s Essay on the human understanding (translation of Essai sur l’origine des connaissances humaines). J. Nourse, London

  • Crockford C, Boesch C (2003) Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Anim Behav 66:115–125

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1872) The expression of emotions in man and animals. John Murray, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dore J (1975) Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. J Child Lang 2:21–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörge FC (2004) Illocutionary acts: Austin’s account and what Searle made out of it. Ph.D. Thesis, Universität Tübingen, Tübingen

  • Ekman P, Friesen W (1978) The facial action coding system. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman R, Greenbaum CW, Yirmiya N (1999) Mother–infant synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Dev Psychol 35:223–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:262–275

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin BS, Warlaumont AS, Oller DK (2013) Infant volubility by random sampling from all-day recordings. Paper presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Chicago

  • Gardner RA, Gardner BT, Van Cantfort TE (eds) (1989) Teaching sign language to chimpanzees. SUNY Press, Albany

    Google Scholar 

  • Gershkoff-Stowe L (2002) Object naming, vocabulary growth, and the development of word retrieval abilities. J Mem Lang 46:665–687

    Google Scholar 

  • Gozoules H, Gozoules S, Ashley J (1995) Representational signaling in non-human primate vocal communication. In: Zimmerman E, Newman JD, Jürgens U (eds) Current topics in primate vocal communication. Plenum Press, New York, pp 235–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Green JA, Jones LE, Gustafson GE (1987) Perception of cries by parents and nonparents: relation to cry acoustics. Dev Psychol 23:370–382

    Google Scholar 

  • Green JA, Gustafson GE, Irwin JR et al (1995) Infant crying: acoustics, perception and communication. Early Dev Parent 4:161–175

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield PM, Savage-Rumbaugh ES (1990) Grammatical combination in Pan Paniscus. In: Parker ST, Gibson KR (eds) “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: comparative developmental perspectives. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 540–578

    Google Scholar 

  • Griebel U, Oller DK (2008) Evolutionary forces favoring contextual flexibility. In: Oller DK, Griebel U (eds) Evolution of communicative flexibility: complexity, creativity, and adaptability in human and animal communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 9–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Gustafson GE, Green JA (1991) Developmental coordination of cry sounds with visual regard and gestures. Infant Behav Dev 14:51–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockett CF (1960) The origin of speech. Human communication: language and its psychobiological bases. Readings from Scientific American. Freeman, San Francisco

  • Hockett CF, Altmann SA (1968) A note on design features. In: Sebeok TA (ed) Animal communication: techniques of study and results of research. Indiana University Press, Bloomington

    Google Scholar 

  • Lester BM, Boukydis CFZ (1992) No language but a cry. In: Papoušek H, Jürgens U, Papoušek M (eds) Nonverbal vocal communication. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 145–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Lester BM, Zeskind PS (1982) A biobehavioral perspective on crying in early infancy. In: Fitzgerald HEBML, Yogman MW (eds) Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics, vol 1. Plenum, New York, pp 133–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Lester BM, Boukydis CZ, Garcia-Coll CT et al (1994) Developmental outcome as a function of goodness of fit between the infant’s cry characteristics and the mother’s perception of her infant’s cry. Pediatrics 95:516–521

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz K (1951) Ausdrucksbewegungen höherer Tiere. Naturwissenschaften 38:113–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Macedonia JM, Evans CS (1993) Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology 93:177–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J, Harper D (2003) Animal signals. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Meadows D, Elias G, Bain J (2000) Mothers’ ability to identify infants’ communicative acts consistently. J Child Lang 27:393–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Ninio A, Snow C (1986) Language acquisition through language use: the functional sources of children’s early utterances. In: Levi I, Braine M, Schlesinger IM (eds) Categories and processes in language acquisition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 11–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK (1980) The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In: Yeni-Komshian G, Kavanagh J, Ferguson C (eds) Child phonology, vol 1., Production. Academic Press, New York, pp 93–112

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK (2000) The emergence of the speech capacity. Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK (2005) The natural logic of communicative possibilities: modularity and presupposition. In: Callebaut W, Rasskin-Gutman D (eds) Modularity: understanding the development and evolution of natural complex systems. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 409–434

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK, Eilers RE (1989) A natural logic of speech and speech-like acts with developmental implications. First Lang 9:225–243

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK, Eilers RE (1992) Development of vocal signaling in human infants: toward a methodology for cross-species vocalization comparisons. In: Papoušek H, Jürgens U, Papoušek M (eds) Nonverbal vocal communication. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 174–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK, Buder EH, Ramsdell HL et al (2013) Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and the emergence of language. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:6318–6323

    Google Scholar 

  • Otte D (1974) Effects and functions in the evolution of signaling systems. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 5:385–417

  • Owings DH, Morton ES (1998) Animal vocal communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Owings DH, Zeifman DM (2004) Human infant crying as an animal communication system. In: Oller DK, Griebel U (eds) The evolution of communication systems. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 151–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Owren MJ, Rendall D (2001) Sound on the rebound: bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling. Evol Anthr 10:58–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Owren MJ, Rendall D, Ryan MJ (2010) Redefining animal signaling: influence versus information in communication. Biol Philos 25:755–780

    Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek H, Papoušek M (1984) Qualitative transitions during the first trimester of human postpartum life. In: Prechtl HFR (ed) Continuity of neural functions from prenatal to postnatal life. Spastics International Medical Publications, London, pp 220–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Parr L, Waller BM, Heintz M (2008) Facial expression categorization by chimpanzees using standardized stimuli. Emotion 8:216–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce CS (1934) Pragmatism in retrospect: a reformulation. In: Hartshorne C, Weiss P (eds) The collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, vol 5. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack D (1971) Language in chimpanzee? Science 172:808–822

    Google Scholar 

  • Prescott R (1975) Infant cry sound: developmental features. J Acoustic Soc Am 57:1186–1191

    Google Scholar 

  • Price T (2013) Vocal communication within the genus Chlorocebus: insights into mechanisms of call production and call perception. Ph.D. Dissertation, Georg-August-Universität Göttingen

  • Quine WVO (1969) Ontological relativity and other essays. Columbia University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Rendall D, Owren MJ, Ryan MJ (2009) What do animal signals mean? Anim Behav 78:233–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh ES, MacDonald K (1988) Deception and social manipulation in symbol-using apes. In: Byrne RW, Whiten A (eds) Machiavellian intelligence. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 224–237

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips TC (2010) Animal communication: insights from linguistic pragmatics. Anim Behav 79:e1–e4

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips TC, Gurney J, Ivens A et al (2014) Combinatorial communication in bacteria: implications for the origins of linguistic generativity. PLoS One 9(4):e95929. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0095929

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle JR (1969) Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle JR, Vanderveken D (1985) Foundations of illocutionary logic. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Seibert J, Hogan A, Mundy P (1982) Assessing interactional competencies: the early social-communication scales. Inf Mental Health J 3:244–245

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1999) Production, usage, and response in nonhuman primate vocal development. In: Hauser MD, Konishi M (eds) The design of animal communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 391–417

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2010) Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain Lang 115:92–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070–1094

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon CT (1982) Linguistic and psycholinguistic approaches to primate communication. In: Snowdon CT, Brown CH, Petersen MR (eds) Primate communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 212–238

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon CT (1990) Language capacities of nonhuman animals. Yearb Phys Anthropol 33:215–243

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder L, Bates E, Bretherton I (1981) Content and context in early lexical development. J Child Lang 8:565–582

    Google Scholar 

  • Soltis J (2004) The signal functions of early infant crying. Behav Brain Sci 27:443–490

    Google Scholar 

  • Stegmann UE (2013) Animal communication theory: information and influence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN (1974) Mother and infant at play: the dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviors. In: Lewis M, Rosenblum LA (eds) The effect of the infant on its caregiver. Wiley, New York, pp 187–213

    Google Scholar 

  • Struhsaker TT (1967) Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 281–324

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2012) Referential mobbing calls elicit different predator-searching behaviours in Japanese great tits. Anim Behav 84:53–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Brandt S (2009) Flexibility in the semantics and syntax of children’s early verb use: a commentary on Naigle, Hoff, and Vear (2009). Monog Soc Res Child Dev 74:113–132

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick EZ, Cohn JF (1989) Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Dev 60:85–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick EZ, Als H, Brazelton TB (1977) Mutuality in mother–infant interaction. J Commun 27:74–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler BC, Fischer J (2012) Functionally referential signals: a promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evol Anthr 21:195–205

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeifman DM (2001) An ethological analysis of human infant crying: answering Tinbergen’s four questions. Dev Psychobiol 39:265–285

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2003) Referential signaling in non-human primates: cognitive precursors and limitations for the evolution of language. Adv Stud Behav 33:265–307

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research for this paper was funded by Grants R01 DC006099 and DC011027 from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders and by the Plough Foundation. Thanks go to Drew Rendall for helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to D. Kimbrough Oller.

Additional information

This article is dedicated to the memory of Michael Owren, with whom the work was jointly conceived. He died before contributing to the writing, but his numerous insights influenced this work deeply and will remain for us an enduring legacy of thoughtful interpretation and scientific integrity.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Oller, D.K., Griebel, U. On Quantitative Comparative Research in Communication and Language Evolution. Biol Theory 9, 296–308 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-014-0186-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-014-0186-7

Keywords

Navigation