A basic knowledge on primate color vision

Vision specialization of primates

Primates are generally regarded as vision-oriented mammals. The visual system of primates, especially that of anthropoids (simians) [catarrhines (humans, apes and Old World monkeys) and platyrrhines (New World monkeys)], is generally characterized by forward-facing eyes, a postorbital plate (a bony cup surrounding the eye), a fovea (a major central peak in density of cone photoreceptor cells in the retina), and increased representation of the visual centers in the brain cortex (Fleagle 2013). Forward-facing eyes are seen in all primates and enable stereoscopic vision. The postorbital plate is found in simians and prevents the chewing muscles from disrupting eye position, which could serve to improve visual acuity (Heesy et al. 2007). The simian fovea also allows for very high visual acuity. These features are essential for agile movement and saltatory locomotion from branch to branch and appear to have evolved together with other primate pattern traits, such as grasping hands and feet with nails and an opposable thumb/toe, retention of the collar bone allowing for a flexible forelimb movement, and the enlarged brain, for a predominantly arboreal and highly social life (Lambert 1987).

Among features of primate vision, special interest to evolutionary biologists is the unique evolution of trichromatic color vision, which arose from a dichromatic ancestor. Color vision is based on the ability to discriminate light by differences in the wavelength (or hue). At least two different spectral classes of cone photoreceptors are necessary in the retina to perceive differences of wavelength compositions (i.e. colors). Generally speaking, the number of discriminable colors increases as the number of spectrally distinct photoreceptors increases and as the spectral overlap among them is reduced (Vorobyev 2004).

L/M and S opsins and trichromatic color vision in primates

Vertebrate visual opsins are classified into five phylogenetic types, RH1 (rhodopsin or rod opsin for dim-light vision) and four cone opsins: RH2 (rhodopsin-like, or green), SWS1 (short wavelength-sensitive type 1, or ultraviolet-blue), SWS2 (short wavelength-sensitive type 2, or blue) and M/LWS (middle to long wavelength-sensitive, or red–green) (Yokoyama 2000). After a brief controversy, these five types are now considered to have been present in the common ancestor of all vertebrates including jawless fish (Collin et al. 2003, 2009; Davies et al. 2009a, b, 2012; Pisani et al. 2006; Yokoyama 2000). Thus, early vertebrates could already have had four-dimensional color vision (tetrachromacy). Placental mammals maintain only two types of cone visual opsins, SWS1 and M/LWS, in addition to the RH1 rod opsin, and are hence dichromatic in color vision (Jacobs 1993). Conventionally, in the case of therian mammals, SWS1 opsin is called “blue” or “S” opsin, with λmax at around 410–430 nm among primates, and M/LWS opsins are collectively called “red–green” or “L/M” opsins, with λmax at around 530–560 nm among primates (Kawamura et al. 2012).

Primates are the only exception among placental mammals in attaining trichromatic vision. This is made possible by spectral diversification of the L/M opsin alleles of the single-locus X-linked gene in a few lemuriform primates (Heesy and Ross 2001; Jacobs et al. 2002; Jacobs and Deegan 2003; Tan and Li 1999; Veilleux and Bolnick 2009) and in a majority of New World monkeys (Jacobs 1984, 2007; Matsumoto et al. 2014; Mollon et al. 1984). In this system, all males are dichromatic but females are either dichromatic or trichromatic (“allelic” or “polymorphic” trichromacy). In catarrhine primates and Alouatta (howler monkeys, a genus of New World monkeys) (Dulai et al. 1999; Jacobs et al. 1996a; Matsushita et al. 2014), trichromacy was achieved through juxtaposition (duplication) of the spectrally differentiated L/M opsin genes on the same X chromosome (Jacobs and Nathans 2009; Surridge et al. 2003). In this system, both males and females are trichromatic (“routine” trichromacy).

‘Three-sites’ rule and variation of primate L/M opsins

The majority of the spectral variation of primate L/M opsin subtypes is explained by amino acid composition at the residue 180, 277 and 285 (‘three-sites’ rule) (Hiramatsu et al. 2004; Matsumoto et al. 2014; Yokoyama and Radlwimmer 1998, 1999, 2001; Yokoyama et al. 2008). The λmax of the L/M opsins with serine, tyrosine and threonine at residues 180, 277 and 285, respectively (denoted SYT), are expected to be approximately 560 nm (Yokoyama et al. 2008). The λmax values of L/M opsins with other three-site combinations can be predicted by subtracting 5, 10 and 17 nm from 560 nm in the case of alanine, phenylalanine and threonine at residues 180, 277 and 285, respectively (Yokoyama et al. 2008). In addition, interactions between these mutations are estimated to be −2 nm for S180A/T285A, +1 nm for Y277F/T285A and +4 nm for S180A/Y277F/T285A (Yokoyama et al. 2008).

All eight possible combinations of the three sites are reported for primate L/M opsins (Fig. 1). Among them, major five subtypes which spread widely in New World monkeys (SYT, AYT, AFT, AYA, AFA) are estimated to have an antique origin in the common ancestor of New World monkeys (Boissinot et al. 1998). Among the five, AYT and AYA are shared with strepsirhines and tarsiers, and SYT and AFA are shared with catarrhines (Fig. 1). Among the other three, SFT is found only in pithciids (uakari) (Corso et al. 2016) and atelines (spider monkeys, woolly monkeys and muriquis) (Hiramatsu et al. 2005; Matsumoto et al. 2014; Talebi et al. 2006). SYA is found only in howler monkeys as a relatively common recombinant variant (Matsushita et al. 2014) and in humans as a rare recombinant variant (Hayashi et al. 2006) between SYT and AFA. SFA is found only in non-human catarrhines as a rare recombinant variant (Onishi et al. 1999, 2002) and in humans as a common recombinant variant (Deeb 2006) between SYT and AFA. Deviation of observed λmax values from those expected from the ‘three-sites’ rule has been at most 4 nm (Matsushita et al. 2014; Yokoyama et al. 2008). Exceptions to the ‘three-sites’ rule are SYT, SFT and AFT alleles of atelines (Fig. 1), which are short-wave shifted and devoid of the spectral effect of S180A due to unique mutations Y213D and N294K (Matsumoto et al. 2014).

Fig. 1
figure 1

L/M opsin subtypes in primates distinguished on the basis of the ‘three-sites’ composition. At each amino acid site, longer-wave residue (S at 180, Y at 277, T at 285) is indicated with red and the shorter-wave residue (A at 180, F at 277, A at 285) is indicated with green. The expected λmax values are given to each subtype according to the ‘three-sites’ rule. The major five subtypes found in Platyrrhini are boldfaced and other subtypes are indicated with smaller font. In Lemuriformes, only four species (Varecia variegata, V. rubra, Propithecus coquereli, and Eulemur macaco flavifrons) have been reported to retain two subtypes as alleles, while other species examined to date have either one of the two (Heesy and Ross 2001; Jacobs et al. 2002; Jacobs and Deegan 2003; Tan and Li 1999; Veilleux and Bolnick 2009). In Tarsiiformes, extant species have either one of the two subtypes but their common ancestral species is suspected to have had both subtypes as alleles (Melin et al. 2013b; Tan and Li 1999). In Pitheciidae of New World monkeys (NWM), six alleles have been found from bald uakari (Cacajao calvus) (Corso et al. 2016). In Atelinae of NWM, λmax values of SYT, SFT and AFT are significantly short-wave shifted (λmax indicated in parentheses) from the expectation due to additional mutations and are highlighted with blue (Matsumoto et al. 2014). In Atelinae, two alleles are typically found in each species: SYT and SFT in Ateles, SYT and AFT in Lagothrix lagotricha (Matsumoto et al. 2014) and SYT and SFT in Brachyteles (AFA is only found in Brachyteles hypoxanthus) (Talebi et al. 2006). In Alouattinae, AFT and SYA are recombinant variants recently reported (Matsushita et al. 2014). In Old World monkeys (OWM) and apes, SFA is reported as a rare recombinant variant in a macaque species Macaca fascicularis (Onishi et al. 1999, 2002) and in chimpanzee Pan troglodytes (Terao et al. 2005). In humans, a variety of variants are reported (Deeb 2005; Hayashi et al. 2006). AYT is reported as a rare recombinant variant for Saimiri boliviensis in Cebinae (Cropp et al. 2002) but omitted here for simplicity

Preceding machineries enabling primate trichromacy

X chromosome locality of L/M opsin genes

The therian L/M opsin genes are X-linked, which is extremely important for primates to attain the trichromacy. In the lemuriform primates and New World monkeys with two or more alleles of single-locus L/M opsin genes, these alleles are expressed mutually exclusively by virtue of random X chromosomal inactivation (lyonization) in females and there is only one X chromosome (hemizygosity) in males. Given the uniform presence of the S opsin gene, females having two different spectral alleles of the L/M opsin gene become trichromats, whereas females having two identical L/M opsin alleles and males, due to the hemizygosity of the X chromosome, are dichromats. Thus, the allelic trichromacy is achieved without invention of a special mechanism for allelic exclusion of gene expression.

Midget ganglion pathway

Among mammals, only primates are equipped with the midget ganglion pathway in the retina, which enables finer resolution of the spectral signals from two spectral classes of L/M cone cells. The midget pathway receives input from only one (in simians) to approximately five (e.g. in bushbabies, a nocturnal lorisiform prosimian) L/M cone cells in the center of a midget ganglion receptive field and compares it to inputs from surrounding L/M cones, allowing high spatial acuity (Martin 1998; Yamada et al. 1998). This neuronal pathway for spatial resolution serves as the evolutionary precursor for the L–M color opponent mechanism, which enables the primate trichromacy instantly when spectral differentiation emerges among L/M cones (Jacobs and Nathans 2009). Although opsin gene renovation appears to confer immediate changes in color perception (Jacobs et al. 2007) without the midget ganglion pathway, evolution of trichromacy may not have been favored in other mammals with coarse spatial resolution even if a similar spectral differentiation of opsin subtypes would have occurred (Surridge et al. 2003; Vorobyev 2004). The absence of trichromatic color vision in other diurnal mammals with similar diets may attest to the acuity requirement of trichromacy (Melin et al. 2014a).

LCR of juxtaposed L/M opsin genes in catarrhines

In catarrhine primates where L and M opsin genes are juxtaposed on the X chromosome, an additional mechanism is required to selectively express only one gene from the array on one X chromosome. This is achieved through a process of stochastic interaction of a locus control region (LCR), situated upstream of the L/M ospin gene array, with the promoter of only one gene from the array in one cone cell (Smallwood et al. 2002; Wang et al. 1999). The LCR is originally an enhancer element for the single-copy M/LWS opsin gene (Wang et al. 1992). Thus, the gene duplication at the downstream of the LCR enabled catarrhine primates to achieve mutually exclusive expressions of the L and M opsin genes. Hence, primate trichromacy was realized serendipitously through pre-existing situations: X-chromosomal locality of the L/M opsin gene and midget ganglion pathway as well as in catarrhine primates the L/M opsin gene duplication outside the LCR.

In howler monkeys the L and M opsin genes are also juxtaposed (Jacobs et al. 1996a). Unlike catarrhine primates, however, the howlers’ LCR is duplicated together with the opsin gene (Dulai et al. 1999). Nevertheless, mutually exclusive expression of the L and M opsins has been supported by a ganglion recording and microspectrophotometry (MSP) (Silveira et al. 2014). The trichromacy of both male and female howler monkeys is also supported by a behavioral experiment (Araujo et al. 2008). The substantially high peak cone density is observed in the foveal pit of howler monkeys which could be tied with high visual acuity and routine trichromacy (Muniz et al. 2014). These observations imply that howler monkeys have evolved an unknown extra machinery to attain the mutually exclusive expression of the juxtaposed L/M opsin genes and routine trichromacy.

Visual opsins and the evolutionary origin of primate color vision

S opsin loss and monochromacy

Variation of S and L/M opsin genes results in various modes of color vision (monochromacy, dichromacy, polymorphism with dichromacy and trichromacy, polymorphic trichromacy, and uniform trichromacy) at various taxonomic levels among primates. The monochromacy (colorblindness) arises in all species of Lorisiformes (lorises and galagos/bushbabies) and Aotus (owl monkeys, a genus of New World monkeys) and in various species of cheirogaleid prosimians of Lemuriformes [in genera Phaner (fork-marked lemurs), Cheirogaleus (dwarf lemurs) and Allocebus (hairy-eared mouse lemur)] due to loss of S cone or loss of functional S opsin gene by deleterious mutations (Deegan and Jacobs 1996; Jacobs et al. 1993, 1996b; Kawamura and Kubotera 2004; Levenson et al. 2007; Tan et al. 2005; Veilleux et al. 2013; Wikler and Rakic 1990). These species all share a nocturnal activity pattern.

However, nocturnality does not appear to be a sufficient condition to lose color vision; many other nocturnal primates retain S opsin gene and maintain dichromacy (Jacobs 2013). In fact, the functionality of S opsin gene appears to be maintained by purifying selection in these species (Kawamura and Kubotera 2004; Perry et al. 2007; Tan et al. 2005; Veilleux et al. 2013). Retaining dichromacy in nocturnal strepsirrhines and tarsiers has been suggested as an evidence against the conventional view that ancestral primates were nocturnal, for a hypothesis that the ancestral primates were diurnal or cathemeral and that nocturnality has evolved several times, first in the lorisiforms but much later in other lineages, reflecting different time periods of functional relaxation among lineages (Tan et al. 2005).

Dichromacy and nocturnality

The diurnal/cathemeral origin hypothesis is losing a support due in part to a recent population genetic study which detected a signature of on-going purifying selection maintaining the S opsin gene in lemuriform nocturnal aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis) (Perry et al. 2007). Aye–ayes have a short-wave shifted S opsin (λmax at 406 nm) (Carvalho et al. 2012) and it is reported that twilight is enriched in short-wavelength (bluish) light with sufficient intensity for aye-ayes with the short-wave-shifted S opsin to perform cone-mediated color vision for their twilight activities (Melin et al. 2012). Another study regards the openness of forest canopy to the sky and the nocturnal activity under moonlight as the main factor influencing the retention of S opsin and color vision in nocturnal lemuriform prosimians (Veilleux et al. 2013). Nocturnal light intensity, particularly short-wave light, is much greater in open canopy forests than in the understory of closed canopy forests (Veilleux and Cummings 2012). Veilleux et al. (2013) found that lemuriform nocturnal species under open canopy habitats generally experience strong purifying selection to maintain the S opsin gene, while, in contrast, those under closed canopy habitats experience weaker purifying selection or a relaxation of selection on it. These studies suggest that dichromatic color vision can be compatible with the nocturnality of ancestral primates.

Origin of trichromay under dim light

Among strepsirrhines, occasional trichromacy due to the allelic polymorphism of the single-locus X-linked L/M opsin gene has been observed in two diurnal lemurid species [black and white ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata) and red ruffed lemurs (V. rubra)], one diurnal indriid species [Coquerel’s sifaka (Propithecus coquereli)] and one cathemeral lemurid species [blue-eyed black lemurs (Eulemur macaco flavifrons)] (Fig. 1) (Heesy and Ross 2001; Jacobs et al. 2002; Jacobs and Deegan 2003; Tan and Li 1999; Veilleux and Bolnick 2009). Occasional trichromacy is also suspected in the last common ancestor of crown tarsiers (Fig. 1) which is considered to be active in low light, due to the existence of hyper-enlarged eye orbits in the genus (Melin et al. 2013b). With the findings that full moonlight and twilight in tropical forest are sufficient for cone-mediated color vision (Melin et al. 2012), origin of primate trichromay has recently been suggested in activities under dim (mesopic) light conditions (Melin et al. 2013b). Although more data on genetic variation of opsin genes and color vision are necessary for diurnal, cathemeral and nocturnal prosimians, these recent studies challenge the traditional and simplistic view of the diurnal origin of primate trichromacy.

Visual opsin variation in New World monkeys and evolutionary significance of primate color vision

Overview and general implications

New World monkeys are known with their extensive inter- and intra-species variation of color vision (Fig. 1). As introduced above, howler monkeys (Alouatta) attained the sex-independent trichromacy through juxtaposition of the spectrally differentiated L/M opsin genes on the same X chromosome (Dulai et al. 1999; Jacobs et al. 1996a). On the other extreme, owl monkeys (Aotus), the sole nocturnal anthropoid primates, are the cone monochromacy due to the loss of functional S opsin gene (Jacobs et al. 1993, 1996b; Levenson et al. 2007; Wikler and Rakic 1990). All the other 14 genera encompassing all the three platyrrhine families (Cebidae, Atelidae and Pitheciidae) (Wildman et al. 2009) are reported to have allelic polymorphism of the single-locus X-lined L/M opsin gene (Fig. 1) and exhibit color vision variation, i.e. a mixed population of female and male dichromats and female trichromats (Jacobs 2007; Matsumoto et al. 2014).

A wide variation of allelic composition occurs among them (Fig. 1), ranging from two alleles, seen typically in Ateles (spider monkeys) and Lagothrix (woolly monkeys) (Hiramatsu et al. 2005; Hiwatashi et al. 2010; Jacobs and Deegan 2001; Matsumoto et al. 2014), up to six alleles in Cacajao calvus (bald uakari) (Corso et al. 2016). In Pithciidae which Cacajao belongs to, similarly many alleles (five alleles) are suspected for Callicebus moloch (dusky titi monkeys) by an electroretinogram study (Jacobs and Deegan 2005) although three alleles have been identified in Callicebus brunneus (brown titi monkeys) by a nucleotide sequence analysis (Bunce et al. 2011b). Three-allele composition is widely observed in Cebidae (SYT, AFT, AFA in Cebinae, SYT, AYT, AYA in Calitrichinae) (Fig. 1) (de Lima et al. 2015; Jacobs 2007; Matsumoto et al. 2014).

Because of the extensive intra-specific diversity of color vision, New World monkeys are the excellent model to study the utility and evolutionary significance of primate color vision. Different L/M opsin alleles confer different phenotypes on trichromacy and on dichromacy. Generally, larger spectral separation between two L/M opsins in trichromats results in higher red–green chromatic resolution (Melin et al. 2009, 2014b). Likewise, larger spectral separation between S and L/M opsins in dichromats results in higher blue–yellow chromatic resolution (Osorio et al. 2004). Thus, New World monkeys are suited to evaluate the performance difference not only between trichromacy and dichromacy but also between different trichromat phenotypes and between different dichromat phenotypes.

Based on a simplistic view of selective advantage on higher dimension and resolution of color vision in primates, a number of predictions can be made on the variation of L/M opsin subtypes, foraging performance, reproductive success, and so on for different color vision phenotypes in New World monkeys. Recent studies have tested these predictions, with some supported and others not. Emerging is a more complex and condition-dependent nature of utility and evolution of primate trichromatic color vision.

Unexpected hybrid L/M opsins in howler monkeys

The juxtaposition of L and M ospin genes in howler monkeys was originally reported to consist of the longest (λmax at ~560 nm) and the shortest (λmax at ~530 nm) wave subtypes of L/M opsins in primates, respectively, enabling “normal” and routine trichromacy as seen in catarrhines (Jacobs et al. 1996a). Thus, the finding was taken as a supporting evidence of the evolutionary advantage of trichromacy in primates, regarding polymorphic color vision in most New World monkeys as an intermediate stage of primate evolution from dichromacy to trichromacy with the spectrally most separated L/M opsin subtypes (Bowmaker et al. 1987; Jacobs et al. 1996a).

However, a recent study of natural populations of mantled howlers in Costa Rica and Nicaragua (Alouatta palliata) and Yucatan black howlers in Belize (A. pigra) found a hybrid L/M opsin gene in each species (“Apa_ML” and “Api_LM”, corresponding to AFT and SYA in Fig. 1, respectively) with ~10 % of frequencies (Matsushita et al. 2014). The λmax of the values of the reconstituted hybrid photopigments are ~546 nm [Matsushita et al. (2014) and our unpublished observation], which should result in mildly “anomalous” trichromats in humans’ term (Deeb 2005) and comparable to those seen in Cebinae carrying an intermediate-λmax allele, who are successful in discriminating stimuli using Ishihara pseudo-isochromatic plates (Saito et al. 2005a). Thus, on the contrary to the prediction, the attained “normal” trichromacy is not maintained in howler monkeys.

Unequal allele frequencies of L/M opsins

If trichromacy is simply the best in fitness, allele frequencies of the L/M opsin subtypes are expected to be equal to maximize the number of trichromats. In the platyrrhine species with the single-locus L/M opsin alleles and color vision polymorphism, however, accumulated data have proven that this is not the case. Studies on the wild populations of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) and black-handed spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) (Hiramatsu et al. 2005; Hiwatashi et al. 2010) and on other spider monkeys (Jacobs and Deegan 2001) and muriquis (Talebi et al. 2006) have found that the longest-wave allele is most frequent and the shortest-wave allele is least frequent. Though with different patterns, unequal allele frequencies are also observed in squirrel monkeys (Cropp et al. 2002) or callitrichines (marmosets and tamarins) (Surridge et al. 2005). Deviation from equality could result from selective neutrality among alleles (Hartl and Clark 2007) and also from a selection not simply maximizing the number of trichromacy. A population genetic study of nucleotide sequence variation revealed that the spectrally differentiated L/M opsin alleles are indeed maintained by selection in the wild populations of capuchin and spider monkeys (Hiwatashi et al. 2010).

The skewed allele frequencies toward longer-wave alleles across the spider monkeys, muriquis and capuchins could imply that the selection consists of complex opposing processes. For trichromats, red–green color discrimination would be greater in individuals having the longest and the shortest wave-sensitive L/M alleles than in individuals having an intermediate wave-sensitive allele. On the other hand, for dichromats, the blue–yellow color resolution would be worst in individuals having the shortest wave-sensitive L/M allele and be best having the longest wave-sensitive allele (Osorio et al. 2004). Thus, the longest wave-sensitive allele would be favored by both trichromats and dichromats, whereas the shortest one would be favored only by trichromats and disfavored by dichromats. Thus, the observed common skew toward longer-wave alleles could indicate that trichromat benefit does not always surpass opposing dichromat benefit and that different alleles could be maintained by different demands among vision types.

However, caution is required to draw a general conclusion because shorter wave alleles can be favored by dichromats in a context to distinguish bluish fruits from background leaves in their long-distance vision (Melin et al. 2014b). Bluish fruits tend to be small and their importance could be larger for small-bodied primates such as squirrel monkeys and callitrichines. This may explain why the longer-wave skewed pattern is not obvious in these species. Shorter wave alleles are also found to be favorable over short distances in computer simulation studies of primate foraging tasks (Melin et al. 2013a; Rowe and Jacobs 2004, 2007), although increased utility of other senses, such as luminance vision and olfaction, could lessen their advantage during short range foraging (Hiramatsu et al. 2008, 2009; Melin et al. 2009).

A complexity is also manifested in the evolutionary history of ateline L/M opsin alleles, which appears to favor thrichromacy on one side but not on the other side. In most atelines, the shortest-wave allele AFA has been lost or is rare (Fig. 1) (Matsumoto et al. 2014), which could imply that dichromat benefit surpasses the opposing trichromat benefit. On the other hand, the spectral separation between the remaining two alleles (SYT and SFT in Ateles and Brachyteles; SYT and AFT in Lagothrix) is enlarged by mutations occurred in the ateline common ancestor, resulting in significant improvement of discriminating conspicuous dietary fruits from leaves in the natural habitat of spider monkeys under both bright and dim light conditions (Matsumoto et al. 2014), which would benefit trichromts. An explanation satisfying both could be that trichromats may tolerate the loss of the shortest-wave allele if the spectral separation of the longest- and intermediate-wave alleles is still sufficient in discriminating stimuli (Saito et al. 2005a) and in foraging performance as shown in capuchin monkeys (Melin et al. 2009). Conversely, in callitrichines, the spectral separation between the longest and the intermediate alleles is comparable to deuteranomalous human trichomats severely impaired in red–green chromatic discrimination (Deeb 2006) (~5 nm; Fig. 1). This may also explain why the shortage of shortest-wave allele is not obvious in callitrichines.

Behavioral studies and evaluation of trichromacy advantage

Limited support or contradictive observations for trichromacy advantage

Superior color discrimination abilities of trichromacy are demonstrated in behavioral experiments for captive New World monkeys (Caine et al. 2003; Pessoa et al. 2005; Saito et al. 2005a; Smith et al. 2003b). Finding fruits amid tropical foliage has long been proffered as an adaptive explanation for primate trichromacy. Nevertheless, field observations of free-ranging animals have provided only limited support for simple trichromacy advantage. In a mixed-species troop of saddleback (Saguinus fuscicollis) and mustached (S. mystax) tamarins, trichromats are further from their neighbors during vigilance than their dichromatic conspecifics. This is explained as resulting from the potentially better perception of predation risk in trichromats (Smith et al. 2005). In a population of white-faced capuchin monkeys (C. capucinus), dichromats sniff more figs and take longer foraging sequences than trichromats, especially for cryptic figs, and the trichromat phenotype with the most spectrally separated L/M opsin alleles shows the highest acceptance index for conspicuous figs (Melin et al. 2009). However, there are no differences in feeding rates among phenotypes (Melin et al. 2009).

Results of other behavioral observations of wild New World monkeys have produced equivocal results or results contradictory to the predictions from the trichromat advantage hypothesis. In the wild mixed-species troops of tamarins, the color vision types (dichromatic or trichromatic) do not show a consistent effect on the leadership of the troops to feeding trees (Smith et al. 2003a). In other social groups of tamarins (S. imperator imperator and S. fuscicollis weddelli) no significant difference is detected between females (thought to consist of trichromats and dichromats) and males (all dichromats) in their ability to locate or discriminate between feeding sites (Dominy et al. 2003a). In a population of capuchin monkeys (C. capucinus), no significant difference is detected between trichromats and dichromats in feeding or energy intake rates (Vogel et al. 2007). In another population of the same capuchin monkey species, no difference is detected between dichromats and trichromats in time spent foraging on different food types (Melin et al. 2008). In a free-ranging social group of black-handed spider monkeys, dichromats are not inferior to trichromats in frequency, accuracy, and unit-time intake efficiency of detecting fruits (Hiramatsu et al. 2008). This is explained because the luminance contrast of fruits to background leaves is the main determinant of fruit detection in both dichromats and trichromats on the basis of colorimetric measurement of fruits and background leaves (Hiramatsu et al. 2008). In this social group of spider monkeys, irrespective of color vision phenotypes, the monkeys sniff and reject visually cryptic fruits more often than visually conspicuous fruits, implying that color vision is not the sole determinant for ingestion or rejection of fruits (Hiramatsu et al. 2009).

Dichromat advantage

Even dichromat advantage is reported in foraging for camouflaged insects in wild capuchins (Melin et al. 2007, 2010) and in wild and captive tamarins (Smith et al. 2012) and in foraging under low light intensity in captive marmosets (Caine et al. 2010). In fact, dichromats are reported to superior to trichromats at breaking camouflage caused by variegated backgrounds (Morgan et al. 1992; Saito et al. 2005b). These findings of observational studies in natural environments suggest that the superior ability of trichromats to see the red–green color contrast may not translate into a selective advantage.

Direct evaluation of fitness effect of trichromacy

Fedigan et al. (2014) tested whether color vision phenotype is a significant predictor of female fitness in a population of wild capuchins, using 26 years of long-term survival and fertility data. No advantage to trichromats over dichromats for three fitness measures (fertility rates, offspring survival and maternal survival) was found. This finding suggests that a selective mechanism other than simple trichromat advantage (heterozygote advantage) is operating to maintain the color vision polymorphism. More attention should be directed to testing in field studies the alternative mechanisms of balancing selection proposed to explain opsin polymorphism: niche-divergence, frequency-dependence and mutual benefit of association (Fedigan et al. 2014).

Revising conditions of trichromacy advantage

Recent findings imply that the adaptive value of primate trichromacy is conditional rather than universal, depending on the specific ecological demands on animals in their environments (Kawamura et al. 2012). The question does remain about what exactly these conditions are. Fruits, young leaves, predators and social signals are the main influential visual targets suggested for trichromacy evolution in primates (Allen 1879; Changizi et al. 2006; Dominy and Lucas 2001; Fernandez and Morris 2007; Kamilar et al. 2013; Lucas et al. 1998; Osorio and Vorobyev 1996; Pessoa et al. 2014; Regan et al. 1998, 2001; Sumner and Mollon 2000, 2003; Surridge et al. 2003; Vorobyev 2004). Viewing distance of these objects in tropical foliage is also an important factor in primate color vision (Sumner and Mollon 2000). Although trichromatic color vision is useful for short-range tasks (Parraga et al. 2002), detecting fruits from a distance has long been suggested to confer a more important selective advantage to trichromatic primates (Bompas et al. 2013; Caine 2002; Regan et al. 2001; Sumner and Mollon 2000). However, an observer cannot definitively know when a monkey has detected an object from a distance, and most investigations of primate color vision have been directed to inspection and ingestion behaviors of foods already at close (<2 m) to moderate (<6 m) distances (Caine and Mundy 2000; Hiramatsu et al. 2008; Melin et al. 2009; Smith et al. 2003b; Vogel et al. 2007). Thus, the likely advantage of trichromacy on long-range foraging has been devoted to little attention.

In an effort to address this gap, Melin et al. (2014a, b) reevaluated the trichromacy advantage on fruit foraging of wild capuchin monkeys from a distance by theoretically analyzing computer-simulated conspicuity of fruits from background leaves. In the simulation, trichromatic phenotypes correctly discriminate ca. 70–80 % of the total dietary fruit spectra in a tropical forest. In contrast, less than one-third of the fruits were discriminable to any of dichromatic phenotypes. This general pattern held for the most heavily consumed diet items, preferred foods, or seasonally critical species eaten during periods of overall food dearth. Furthermore, modeled-trichromatic phenotypes are able to discriminate the vast majority of small patch species, anticipated to provide a high finder’s reward. These small resources are suggested to play a critical role in the adaptive value of trichromacy (Bunce et al. 2011a; Melin et al. 2014b).

Uniform and normal trichromacy in catarrhine primates and exceptional variation in human color vision

Non-human catarrhines contrasting to platyrrhines

The L and M opsin genes of catarrhine primates are highly similar in nucleotide sequence (~96 % identity) and are closely juxtaposed (Nathans et al. 1986). Thus, they are intrinsically susceptible to recombination and gene conversion between them which could cause hybrid L/M opsin genes, gene loss and gene multiplication (Balding et al. 1992; Deeb et al. 1994; Drummond-Borg et al. 1989; Dulai et al. 1994; Ibbotson et al. 1992; Jorgensen et al. 1990; Shyue et al. 1994; Verrelli and Tishkoff 2004; Winderickx et al. 1992, 1993; Zhao et al. 1998; Zhou and Li 1996). Furthermore, a recombination hot-spot chi element is conserved in the exon 3 among primates (Winderickx et al. 1993). Nevertheless, contrasting to platyrrhine primates, the incidence of color vision variation is remarkably low in non-human catarrhine primates (Jacobs and Williams 2001; Onishi et al. 1999; Terao et al. 2005). Among 744 male long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) examined, only three were found to have a single hybrid L/M opsin gene (SFA in Fig. 1) and to be dichromats (Hanazawa et al. 2001; Onishi et al. 1999). Among 58 male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), one was found to have a hybrid L/M opsin gene (SFA in Fig. 1) in addition to one normal M opsin gene on the X chromosome and to be an anomalous (protanomalous) trichromat (Saito et al. 2003; Terao et al. 2005). Thus, frequencies of color vision variants in male long-tailed macaques and male chimpanzees can be calculated to be ~0.4 and ~1.7 %, respectively. These frequencies could be overestimated because no variants were found in 455 male monkeys from other macaque species (Onishi et al. 1999) and the chimpanzees examined were from limited numbers of breeding colonies (Terao et al. 2005). Other researchers have reported an absence of color vision defects in Old World monkeys and apes (Hiwatashi et al. 2011; Jacobs and Williams 2001; Verrelli et al. 2008).

Multiple copies of M opsin genes are likely to increase the frequency of unequal recombination events. In humans multiple M copies are found in 66 % of males of European origin (Drummond-Borg et al. 1989) and 56 % of Japanese males (Hayashi et al. 2001). Regarding non-human catarrhines, some studies report that multiple M copies are rare (Onishi et al. 1999, 2002; Terao et al. 2005; Verrelli et al. 2008) yet other studies report that they are common (Dulai et al. 1994; Hiwatashi et al. 2011; Ibbotson et al. 1992). Thus, among Old World monkeys and apes, there seems to be no clear trend on the copy number variation of M opsin gene.

A study of gibbon population samples showed that gene conversion has homogenized L and M opsin genes in introns (Hiwatashi et al. 2011). However, purifying selection against the homogenization has protected the nucleotide difference between L and M opsin genes in centrally located exons, exons 3 and 5 in particular, which include the spectrally crucial ‘three-sites’ (Hiwatashi et al. 2011). This confirms that gene conversions (and perhaps other forms of recombination) do occur between L and M opsin genes in non-human catarrhines but the genes are eliminated from the population by natural selection if gene conversions affect the gene region relevant to spectral difference between L and M opsins. In nonhuman catarrhine primates, even mildly anomalous trichromats have not been found, suggesting a severe selective disadvantage on color vision variants.

The strict conservation of the normal trichromacy in non-human catarrhines is in sharp contrast to New World monkeys. The higher frequency of anomalous trichromacy in New World howler monkeys implies that the selective pressure to maintain “normal” trichromacy is lower in the Neotropics (Matsushita et al. 2014). However, in New World monkey species with polymorphic color vision, genetic studies have shown that the spectrally different alleles of the L/M opsin gene are actively maintained by balancing selection (Boissinot et al. 1998; Hiwatashi et al. 2010; Surridge and Mundy 2002). It is an open question whether the selection is for maintaining (1) simply heterozygotes of L/M opsin alleles, i.e. trichromacy per se; (2) dichromacy and trichromacy; or (3) subtypic variation in dichromacy and/or trichromacy. It is also an open question as to whether the difference between nonhuman catarrhines (uniform and normal trichromacy) and platyrrhines (polymorphic color vision) is attributable to a (1) biogeographic differences among continents, e.g., the severity of seasonality, or a prevalence of drably colored fruits and asynchronous species, e.g., figs and palm fruits (Dominy et al. 2003b); (2) dietary variability, e.g., degree of dependence on insects, leaves, or colorful fruits and different food patch sizes (Melin et al. 2014b); (3) variation in social color signals (Changizi et al. 2006; Fernandez and Morris 2007; Kamilar et al. 2013).

Uniqueness of humans in color vision

Among catarrhine primates with routine and normal trichromacy, humans constitute a notable exception, in which deletion and multiplication of L/M opsin genes and creation of hybrid L/M opsin genes cause relatively high incidence of dichromacy and anomalous trichromacy, approximately 3–8 % of males being color vision “defects” (Bosten et al. 2005; Deeb 2006; Hood et al. 2006; Sharpe et al. 2006; Verhulst and Maes 1998; Verrelli and Tishkoff 2004). Many humans have multiple copies of the M opsin gene in the L/M opsin gene array where the most upstream gene is typically L and the others are M. Only the upper two genes are expressed and when a hybrid gene occupies either position, it causes anomalous trichromacy (Hayashi et al. 1999). When there is only one L/M opsin gene on an X chromosome or when the two positions are occupied by the same genes, this causes dichromacy (red–green colorblindness: more specifically, protanope when L is lost, and deuteranope when M is lost). These are typically found in men because women have two X chromosomes and thus are more likely to have a “normal” gene array in either one. There are rare cases of individuals, irrespective of sex, who lack functional blue cones (tritanopes, <1:10,000) due to mutations in S opsin gene on chromosome 7 (Sharpe et al. 1999).

The high incidence of color vision variation in humans can be interpreted most conservatively as a result of relaxation of the selective constraint to maintain the spectral difference between the L and M opsin genes. Alternatively, by one step further, adaptive explanations may be possible. Females with a hybrid L/M opsin likely have normal L and M opsins and are able to perceive finer chromatic distinction (Jameson et al. 2001) (a question remains, however, why this is not selected in non-human catarrhines). Studies of New World monkeys provide a profound inference on human color vision variation. The persistence of dichromats in the human population may reflect, as noted above, some advantage in achromatic visual tasks and in having different color vision morphs in a population. Humans are atypical primates, having largely left the foliated environments of forest some million years ago. Then, approximately two million years ago hominins started to devise stone tools and included hunted meat as a considerable portion of their diet. Finally, the increased brain size eventually led to the development of agriculture some thousand years ago and the building of a modern industrial society only a few hundred years ago. The persistence of color vision variation in humans could be related to any of these major events: life outside forest reduces the need for color vision, hunting might have benefited by the presence of dichromatic group members, gathering might have benefited by the presence of hybrid L/M opsins or large agricultural or industrial societies could isolate humans from selection against dichromacy and anomalous trichromacy. It would be crucial to infer when the color vision polymorphism is spread into population as today in human evolution by analyzing L/M opsin nucleotide diversity collected from global ethnic groups.

Finally, color vision is not the sole sense showing diversity among and within species of primates including humans. Interplay of sensory modalities gathers a recent attention in the study of sensory evolution in primates (Hiramatsu et al. 2009).