Mindful parenting --non-judgmental and present-centred awareness in parent-child interactions– has been a subject of considerable interest during the last decade (Duncan et al., 2009). Studies have revealed that mindful parenting remarkably reduces children’s internalising and externalising behaviours while promoting prosocial behaviours (e.g., Cheung et al., 2021; Wong et al., 2019) and life satisfaction (Liu et al., 2021). As such, it has become crucial to identify the sources of individual differences in mindful parenting. Here, building on the process of the parenting model (Belsky, 1984; Taraban & Shaw, 2018), we suggested a model of the determinants of mindful parenting, namely, a process of mindful parenting model. We empirically tested the direct and indirect associations of parent characteristics, child characteristics, and family social environment with mindful parenting, exploring the moderating role of culture. Moreover, further extending previous literature, we included both child and parent perspectives of mindful parenting.

Mindful parenting

Duncan et al. (2009) define mindful parenting as parental attention to parent-child interaction, as well as compassionate, non-reactive, and non-judgemental parental awareness and acceptance of self and child. They suggest that mindful parenting promotes child management and parenting practices, parent-child affection, and parental well-being and ultimately influences child outcomes, especially during the transition to adolescence (Duncan et al., 2009). Various studies have found small-to-moderate positive effects of mindful parenting interventions on children’s outcomes (i.e., internalising and externalising symptoms; Bögels et al., 2014; Emerson et al., 2021; Meppelink et al., 2016; Potharst et al., 2019). However, our understanding of the mindful parenting process lags behind the broader parenting literature, particularly since little is known about the determinants of mindful parenting. This may be partly due to the silent assumption that the determinants of mindful parenting are similar to those of other kinds of parenting behaviour (e.g., Baumrind, 1966; Parent & Forehand, 2017). Yet, mindful parenting behaviours are seen as distinct from these other parenting behaviours – hereon we refer to mindful parenting and ‘traditional models of parenting’, as per Duncan et al. (2009)’s conceptualisation and as is commonly used in the mindful parenting literature (Geurtzen et al., 2015; Pan et al., 2019) -- and we argue that this assumption should be tested empirically. First, mindful parenting concerns monitoring and controlling one’s own emotions, behaviours, and attention as a parent during parent-child interaction, whereas more traditional models of parenting refer to those of children. Second, mindful parenting describes “here-and-now” parenting, where parents pay deliberate attention to parent-child interaction; thus, it requires fundamental mindfulness skills (Duncan et al., 2009). Finally, mindful parenting sees parenting as a journey parents learn from their children, whilst the more traditional approaches tend to assume parents are the ‘experts’ (Kabat-Zinn & Kabat-Zinn, 1997).

Moreover, to our best knowledge, no study has examined the determinants of children’s perceptions of mindful parenting, although previous research on the traditional parenting models showed differences between the determinants of parent and child perceptions of parenting (Cheung & Theule, 2019; Gerdes et al., 2003, 2007). We aimed to fill this research gap, using multiple informants of mindful parenting (i.g., parents and their children) to identify the determinants of mindful parenting. For two main reasons, it is important to assess different perspectives on mindful parenting in this context. First, a multi-informant approach allows the examination of determinants of mindful parenting as perceived from both sides of the relationship. This affords a more complete picture of the mindful parenting process in families, accounting for the subjectivity of experience (Boyce et al., 1998; Schaefer, 1965). Children’s subjective experiences of parenting are robust predictors of child outcomes (Danese & Widom, 2020; Zhou et al., 2021), and identifying determinants of child-reported mindful parenting may improve our understanding. Second, a multi-informant approach may also increase the validity of the mindful parenting process model by minimising bias in self-report of parenting (Morsbach & Prinz, 2006; Schofield et al., 2016) and common-method variance where determinants are also parent-reported (Burk & Laursen, 2010). As such, simultaneously uncovering the determinants of parent and child perceptions of mindful parenting is essential for understanding the full picture of the mindful parenting process in families.

Determinants of (mindful) parenting

Belsky (1984) established a ground-breaking theoretical framework for explaining the determinants of parenting, positing that parenting is multiply determined by parent characteristics (e.g., personality, psychopathology), child characteristics (e.g., temperament), and family social environment (e.g., marital quality, social support). The current study focuses on the determinant roles of parental psychological well-being, child negative emotionality, and social support, as well as potential mechanistic pathways for mindful parenting.

According to Belsky (1984), parental psychological well-being is central to the parenting process, in part directly influencing parenting. Indeed, empirical research has supported parental psychological distress (e.g., depression) as a parental risk factor for maladaptive fathering (for meta-analysis, see Cheung & Theule 2019) and mothering (for meta-analysis, see Goodman et al., 2020; Lovejoy et al., 2000). For mindful parenting, studies have revealed that parental psychological distress can also be undermining (e.g., Cheung et al., 2021; Corthorn & Milicic, 2016; de Bruin et al., 2014; Fernandes et al., 2021; Henrichs et al., 2021). It is suggested that parental psychological distress threatens mindful parenting by impairing capabilities in essential features of mindful parenting, such as emotion regulation, awareness, and present-moment attention. For instance, parents with psychological distress may be less likely to self-regulate during parent-child interaction due to their impaired emotion regulation skills (Kerns et al., 2017; Lovejoy et al., 2000). In addition, parents with higher depressive symptoms may be less attuned and sensitive in their parenting interaction and, as such, be less able to notice emotions of themselves and those of their children (Coyne et al., 2007; Lovejoy et al., 2000) as well as being less aware of the impact of their behaviours on their children’s emotions (Coyne et al., 2007).

In terms of direct effects on parenting, child characteristics, particularly child temperament, are also suggested to play an active role in the parenting process according to Belsky’s model. Subsequent empirical research has supported this model, consistently showing that child negative emotionality -- an intensive and frequent expression of negative emotions by the child– undermines parenting (for a meta-analysis, see Paulussen-Hoogeboom et al., 2007). Regarding the effect of child temperament on mindful parenting specifically, however, findings are more inconsistent. For example, infants’ negative emotionality has been shown to have no cross-sectional (Gartstein, 2021) or longitudinal association (Henrichs et al., 2021) with parent-reported mindful parenting, while preschool children’s “difficult” temperaments have been shown to have a negative impact on mindful parenting (Corthorn & Milicic 2016; Lo et al., 2018) as have those in school-age (aged 6–13; Moreira et al., 2021). It is, therefore, possible that child temperament interacts with child age to predict mindful parenting. To our knowledge, there is no previous research examining the association of child temperament with mindful parenting in adolescents.

Social support is one of the salient environmental factors that may directly determine parenting behaviours (Belsky, 1984), and has repeatedly been shown to increase parental warmth (Lippold et al., 2018), sensitivity (Lee et al., 2020), and involvement (Hamme Peterson et al., 2010), as well as decrease parental hostility (Lippold et al., 2018) and over-reactivity (Taraban et al., 2019). We suggest that social support may also be crucial to mindful parenting as it helps parents regulate their emotional responses to their children (Marroquin, 2011). Indeed, Bögels and Restifo (2014) state that social support is an essential theme in their mindful parenting intervention. So far, however, only one study has empirically examined the relationship between social support and parent-reported mindful parenting, finding that parents who perceived more social support also reported more mindful parenting in a sample of kindergarteners and primary schoolers (Wang & Lo, 2020). Given this promising finding, it is warranted to assess the effect of social support on mindful parenting in adolescents as well.

Indirect effects

As well as the direct effects of parenting determinants, a key theme for Belsky’s model of parenting process involves the indirect effects of these determinants via parental psychological well-being. For example, parental psychological distress is seen as a potential mechanism by which child negative emotionality affects parenting, since parenting children with high negative emotionality is more stressful than parenting children with low negative emotionality (Mulsow et al., 2002). Similarly, emotional, instrumental, and informative support provided to parents by available social networks (e.g., spouses, family, friends, or professionals) may be a key determinant of parenting, posited to be mediated by parents’ psychological well-being (Belsky, 1984). These mechanistic pathways for determinants of traditional parenting model are supported by empirical research (negative emotionality: e.g., Laukkanen et al., 2014; Xing et al., 2017; social support: e.g., Lippold et al., 2018; Östberg & Hagekull, 2000; Taylor et al., 2015), but are neglected for mindful parenting. We hypothesised similar mechanisms for mindful parenting.

Parenting in context

Updating Belsky’s framework, Taraban and Shaw (2018) emphasised the interacting impact of contextual factors (e.g., support, socioeconomic status (SES), and culture) in the parenting process, suggesting that the direct and indirect influences of determinants on parenting may differ across contexts (Bornstein et al., 2007; Taraban et al., 2019). This remains unexplored for mindful parenting.

An important contextual determinant of parenting is culture, shaping parenting behaviours (for review, see also Lansford 2022). Importantly, culture has been suggested to have a moderating role, altering the associations between determinants and parenting (Taraban & Shaw, 2018), although the research is scarce and inconclusive. For example, Japanese mothers have been shown to be more rejective than Korean mothers (Son et al., 2020), and Chinese immigrant mothers to be more non-supportive than European American mothers (Yang et al., 2020) while dealing with temperamentally “difficult” children. Similarly, school social support was related to less harsh parenting behaviours in Dominican-American but not Mexican-American parents (Serrano-Villar et al., 2017). In contrast, no cultural differences were observed in the association between child temperament and parental psychological control between Chinese and Korean immigrant mothers, with “easier” child temperament associated with less psychological control in both cultures (Cheah et al., 2016). Likewise, the association of parental well-being with parental psychological control between Chinese and Korean immigrant mothers (Cheah et al., 2016), as well as the associations of family support with positive parenting between Mexican and Dominican Americans (Serrano-Villar et al., 2017) have been shown to be comparable, implying that personal and contextual sources are determinants of parenting regardless of culture. Taken together, what little cross-cultural research there is testing Taraban and Shaw’s (2018) model on the determinants of traditional parenting model, reveals inconsistent findings.

Cross-cultural studies of mindful parenting are even more limited, considering ethnic minorities within the same country and reporting comparable correlates of mindful parenting, yet are restricted in their power to consider the question (Park et al., 2020). To our knowledge, no study has yet directly compared the mindful parenting process as moderated by culture. Addressing this gap, we tested whether culture interacts with social support, child temperament, and parental psychological distress to shape mindful parenting. Specifically, we were interested in comparing collectivist (Türkiye) and individualist (UK) cultures because these different cultural values have been considered one of the most influential factors in the parenting process (Bornstein, 2012). Due to the limited existing literature, our comparisons of determinants across cultures were exploratory only. As the concept of mindfulness itself is claimed to be universal (Kabat-Zinn, 2005), however, we expected the mindful parenting levels of parents to be similar in both cultures.

Current study

In a sample of UK and Türkiye parents and children, we examined the overall hypothesis that mindful parenting is multiply determined by parent characteristics (i.e., parents’ psychological distress), child characteristics (i.e., negative emotionality), and family social environment (i.e., social support), and that psychological distress would provide a mechanism through which determinants have influence. Specifically, we hypothesised that (1) parents’ social support would directly and indirectly predict mindful parenting through parental psychological distress, (2) child negative emotionality would directly and indirectly predict mindful parenting through parental psychological distress, and (3) culture would play a moderating role in the process of mindful parenting. Moreover, we further expand the literature by exploring the determinants of both parent- and child-reported mindful parenting. The proposed process of the mindful parenting model is given in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Proposed path model of the mindful parenting process

Methods

Participants

One hundred and ten parent-child dyads completed the English questionnaires, and 174 parent-child dyads completed the Turkish version of the questionnaires. Twenty-one participants were excluded as they did not meet the eligibility criteria (not UK/Türkiye residence (n = 3/n = 2), children not between 11 and 16 years old (n = 4), self-reported mental health issue (n = 3), not living full-time with their children (n = 5) or because they failed to complete at least 80% of the questionnaires (n = 4).

Thus, the final sample composed of 101 UK parent-child dyads [90 mothers (89.1%) and 11 fathers (10.9%)) of 57 girls (56.4%), 43 boys (42.6%) (and one data missing)] and 162 Turkish parent-child dyads [151 mothers (93.2%), 11 fathers (6.8%); 87 girls (53.7%) and 75 boys (46.3%)]. The mean age of UK parents was 45.89 years (ranged 28 to 69; SD = 6.54), and of Turkish parents was 43.07 years (ranged 29 to 55; SD = 5.08). UK parents were significantly older than Türkiye parents (t = 3.917, p < .001). UK parents had between one and five children (M = 2.15; SD = 0.81), and Turkish parents had between one and eight children (M = 1.99; SD = 0.92). The mean age of the target children was 13.06 years (SD = 1.64) in the UK and 13.28 years (SD = 1.65) in Türkiye. Parents in both subsamples were well-educated (82.1% of UK parents and 67.5% of Turkish parents hold an undergraduate or higher degree). UK parents reported a mean score of 6.80 (SD = 1.77; ranged from 1 to 10) and Turkish parents 6.75 (SD = 1.66; ranged from 2 to 10) on the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status (Adler et al., 2000). There were no significant differences in child age (t = -1.046, p = .297), the number of children parents had (t = 1.447, p = .149) or perceived SES (t = 0.255, p = .779) between cultures, and samples did not differ by child sex [χ2(1) = 0.271, p = .602] or parent gender [χ2(1) = 1.365, p = .243].

Procedure

Parents were recruited through targeted online social media groups (Twitter, Instagram, Facebook) between March and July 2021 using Qualtrics Survey Software. To be eligible for the study, (1) parents had to have at least one child aged 11–16 years living with them full time, as well as (2) parents and their target children had to have no diagnoses of learning disability, (neuro)developmental or mental-health disorder, (3) had to reside in the UK or Türkiye, and (4) had to be native or fluent in English or Turkish. Parents’ consent and children’s assent were obtained. At the end of the questionnaires, participants were given debriefing information, including contact details of researchers and available mental health support organisations. The BLINDED Research Ethics Committee granted ethical approval (Protocol number BLINDED).

Measures

Demographic information

Parents reported their age (years), gender, marital status, the highest level of educational qualification, number of children, relationship with the target child, whether they lived with the child full-time, and child’s age (years) and sex. The Macarthur Scale of Subjective Social Status (Adler et al., 2000) was used to evaluate parent-perceived SES. The scale has one item for which individuals rate their perceived SES on a ladder with ten rungs scored 1 to 10; higher scores indicate higher levels of perceived SES.

Mindful parenting

Parents’ and children’s perceptions of mindful parenting were assessed using total scores of the 18-item Mindful Parenting Inventories for Parents (MPIP) and Children (MPIC; BLINDED). The MPIP/MPIC were recently translated and adapted for Turkish. Specifically, items were translated into Turkish by two independent clinical psychologists, forming the preliminary versions of the Turkish inventories. These were then independently back-translated by a Turkish clinical psychologist knowledgeable about mindfulness, and a bilingual and bicultural psychology student not knowledgeable about the subject of the scale (Van Widenfelt et al., 2005). Neither of the back-translators had seen the original English versions before translation. The final version of the Turkish MPIP/MPIC was sent to two Turkish parents and children before data collection to assess the comprehensibility of the items, before full validation of the inventories was conducted in a Turkish sample of parents and their children aged 11–16 (BLINDED).

Parents and children rated their perceptions of mindful parenting on a five-point scale from “never true” (1) to “always true” (5). Eight negative items of the MPIP/MPIC were reverse scored, such that higher scores indicate higher levels of mindful parenting. Example items include, “I quickly become defensive when my child and I argue/My mother/father quickly becomes defensive when we argue” and “I accept that my child has opinions that are different from mine/My mother/father accepts that I have opinions that are different from hers/his.” In both cultures, reliability was good (MPIP: αUK = 0.89, αTR = 0.88; MPIC αUK = 0.91, αTR = 0.89).

Parental psychological distress

Parents’ psychological distress was measured using the total scores of the 21-item version of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond 1995; Sarıçam, 2018). Parents reported their psychological distress on a four-point scale ranging from “Did not apply to me at all” (0) to “Applied to me very much or most of the time” (3). Example items include, “I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feeling at all”, “I was aware of dryness of my mouth”, and “I found it hard to wind down.” DASS-21 had excellent internal reliability in the UK (α = 0.95) and Türkiye (α = 0.93).

Child negative emotionality

“Emotionality” Subscale of The Emotionality Activity Sociability Temperament Survey (EASTS; Buss & Plomin, 1984; Eyüpoğlu, 2006) was used to measure parent perceptions of child negative emotionality. A total of six items were rated by parents on a five-point scale ranging from “Not characteristic/typical” (1) to “Very characteristic/typical” (5) (e.g., My child reacts intensely when upset). The scale demonstrated good internal reliability in the UK (α = 0.89) and Türkiye (α = 0.79).

Social support

The total score of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Eker et al., 2001; Zimet et al., 1988) was used to evaluate parents’ perceptions of social support. Parents reported perceived social support from parents, family, friends, and specific others on a seven-point scale from “Very Strongly Disagree” (1) to “Very Strongly Agree” (7). Sample items include, “My friends really try to help me”, “I can talk about my problems with my family”, and “There is a special person in my life who cares about my feelings.” Internal reliabilities were excellent both in the UK (α = 0.96) and Türkiye (α = 0.93).

Data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 29.0 and AMOS 29.0. Data were missing completely at random in UK parents [χ2(175) = 196.711, p = .258], Türkiye parents [χ2(59) = 48.673, p = .829] and Türkiye children [χ2(28) = 37.068, p = .117]. There were no missing in UK children’s data. The expectation maximisation method was used to handle parents’ missing data in continuous variables (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). We investigated relationships between variables using Pearson’s correlations. Independent samples t-tests were used to assess mean level differences between the UK and Türkiye samples. We conducted multiple-group path analysis (with Emulisrel correction) to test the hypothesised process of the mindful parenting model (see Fig. 1) and the invariance of the model across cultures (Byrne, 2016). Sufficient statistical power was provided by the sample size for the analysis (i.e., 100 participants per group; Kline, 2005).

We used the Comparative Fit Index (CFI ≥ 0.90), Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA ≤ 0.08), and Standardised Root-Mean-Square Residual (SRMR ≤ 0.09) to evaluate the model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Chi-square change between unconstrained and constrained models was examined to test equivalence between UK and Türkiye models. Insignificant chi-square change between unconstrained and constrained models indicates noninvariant paths across cultures (Byrne et al., 1989; Kline, 2005). We also examined changes between the models in CFI using the cut-off criteria of − 0.005 recommended for invariance testing in small samples (Chen, 2007). In the case of cultural inequivalence, we identified variant paths that needed to be freely estimated between groups by constraining only one path to be equal at a time. Finally, we analysed direct and indirect effects using 5000 bias-corrected bootstrapped samples with 95% confidence intervals.

Results

Preliminary results

Table 1 presents correlations, descriptive statistics, and group comparisons for all study variables. There were no mean differences between the UK and Türkiye samples in MPIP (t = -1.251, p = .212) or MPIC (t = 0.40, p = .688) total scores. A significant cultural mean difference between the UK and Türkiye was found only in child negative emotionality; Turkish parents reported higher child negative emotionality than UK parents (t = -5.95***, p < .001). As given in Table 1, correlations within cultures were small to moderate in effect size and in expected directions. In the UK, Pearson correlation analysis revealed that both parent and child perceptions of mindful parenting were negatively associated with child negative emotionality (r = − .38, p < .001; r = − .37, p < .001, respectively) and with parental psychological distress (r = − .34, p < .001; r = − .23, p < .001, respectively). In Türkiye, for both parent and child perceptions, mindful parenting was positively associated with parental social support (r = .23, p = .003; r = .22, p = .006, respectively) and negatively associated with parental psychological distress (r = − .39, p < .001; r = − .29, p < .001, respectively).

Table 1 Correlations and descriptive statistics of the study variables

Note that, in the following multiple-group path analysis, we allowed covariances between the error terms of the parent- and child-report mindful parenting because their correlations were high (see Table 1). The effects of parent age and parent gender on MPIP were controlled in the model as parent age for the Türkiye sample (r = .18, p = .026) and parent sex for the UK sample (r = .21, p = .034; 1 = mother, 2 = father) were related to mindful parenting.

Multiple group path analysis

Total effect model

We tested the total effects of social support and child negative emotionality on MPIP and MPIC across cultures. The unconstrained model showed good fit to the data [χ2(4) = 6.323, χ2/df = 1.581, CFI = 0.984, RMSEA = 0.047, SRMR = 0.044]. We then constrained all paths in the model to be equal across groups (i.e. cultures). Compared to the unconstrained model, the constrained model fit was worse in the constrained model [χ2(10) = 16.726, χ2/df = 1.673, CFI = 0.953, RMSEA = 0.051, SRMR = 0.072; ∆χ2(6) = 10.403, p = .109, ∆CFI = − 0.031]. Although the chi-square change was insignificant, as CFI significantly reduced in the constrained model, we concluded that not all paths should be treated as equal. We found that the paths from child negative emotionality to both MPIP and MPIC were variant across cultures, as such, freely estimated those variant paths across groups [χ2(8) = 9.311, χ2/df = 1.164, CFI = 0.991, RMSEA = 0.025, SRMR = 0.058; ∆χ2(4) = 2.989, p = .560, ∆CFI = 0.007]. According to the results, the paths between social support and MPIP (b = 0.06, p = .014) and MPIC (b = 0.08, p = .009) were significant both in the UK and Türkiye. However, the path between child negative emotionality and MPIP (bUK = − 0.17, p < .001; bTR = − 0.04, p = .391) and MPIC (bUK = − 0.26, p < .001; bTR = − 0.02, p = .765) were significant in the UK only.

Direct and indirect effect model

The unconstrained multiple group mediation path model in which social support and child negative emotionality predicted MPIP and MPIC through parental psychological distress had a good model fit to the data [χ2(8) = 15.372, χ2/df = 1.922, CFI = 0.968, RMSEA = 0.060, SRMR = 0.058)]. We then constrained paths in the model to be equal across groups. Compared to the unconstrained model, the constrained model fit was worse in the constrained model [χ2(18) = 29.379, χ2/df = 1.632, CFI = 0.951, RMSEA = 0.049, SRMR = 0.088); ∆χ2(10) = 14.007, p = .173, ∆CFI = − 0.017]. Although the chi-square change was insignificant, as CFI significantly reduced in the constrained model, we concluded that not all paths should be treated as equal. Again, we found that the paths from child negative emotionality to both MPIP and MPIC were variant across cultures; as such, we freely estimated those variant paths across groups for the subsequent analysis [χ2(16) = 23.481, χ2/df = 1.468, CFI = 0.968, RMSEA = 0.042, SRMR = 0.078; ∆χ2(8) = 8.109, p = .423, ∆CFI = 0.000]. Finally, we constrained the covariance between MPIP and MPIC to be equal across groups. The cross-reporter association of mindful parenting was invariant between the UK and Türkiye [χ2(17) = 24.079, χ2/df = 1.416, CFI = 0.970, RMSEA = 0.040, SRMR = 0.077; ∆χ2(1) = 0.589, p = .440, ∆CFI = 0.002].

As given in Table 2, social support was not directly associated with parent-reported (b = 0.01, p = .607) or child-reported mindful parenting (b = 0.04, p = .201) in either the UK or Türkiye (see also Fig. 2). However, social support indirectly predicted MPIP (ab = 0.04, p < .000) and MPIC (ab = 0.04, p = .002) through parental psychological distress in both cultures. In the UK only, child negative emotionality directly predicted MPIP (bUK = − 0.12, p = .012; bTR = − 0.02, p = .642) and MPIC (bUK = − 0.22, p = .001; bTR = 0.00, p = .988). Yet, it indirectly predicted MPIP (ab = − 0.03, p = .001) and MPIC (ab = − 0.03, p = .002) through parental psychological distress in both cultures.

Table 2 Direct and indirect effects
Fig. 2
figure 2

Unstandardized path coefficients obtained in hypothesised multiple-group path analysis. Note. All paths were constrained to be equal across the UK and Türkiye except for the thick lines, which were significant for the UK samples only (left). Dashed lines represent insignificant regression weights. DASS = Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, MPIP = Mindful Parenting Inventory for Parents, MPIC = Mindful Parenting Inventory for Children.

Discussion

The current study aimed to explore the determinants of mindful parenting in the UK Türkiye, basing our expectations on Belsky’s (1984) model. We tested total as well as direct and indirect associations of parent characteristics (psychological distress), child characteristics (negative emotionality), and family social environment (parental perceived social support) with mindful parenting in UK- and Türkiye-based parents and their children aged 11–16 years. In addition, this study also aimed to identify culture-general and culture-specific aspects of these associations grounded in Taraban and Shaw’s (2018) model. Furthermore, we expanded on previous mindful parenting research by using multiple informants of mindful parenting (i.g., parents and their children). Overall, our study showed that both parent and child perspectives of mindful parenting are multiply determined. The associations of child negative emotionality and social support with both perspectives on mindful parenting were mediated by parental psychological distress. However, culture had a moderating role in this process. As discussed below, child negative emotionality was not a direct determinant of mindful parenting in Türkiye, although otherwise, the processes did not differ across countries.

First, in line with but going beyond earlier studies comparing ethnic minorities within the same country (e.g., “white people” and “people of colour”; Parent et al., 2016a, b; Park et al., 2020), we found no significant differences in parent-reported mindful parenting between the UK and Türkiye. Further, for the first time, our study suggests that child reports mirror this finding, further supporting the idea that mindful parenting is a “culture-free” skill (McCaffrey et al., 2017). Moreover, although counter to previous research comparing the UK and Türkiye (Aytac & Pike, 2018; Kortantamer, 2011), there were no significant cultural differences in parent-reported perceived social support or psychological distress in our sample. This may be because our samples from the two countries were similar in sociodemographics. However, parents in Türkiye reported higher levels of child negative emotionality than parents in the UK. This finding is consistent with previous research, where children in less individualistic countries (e.g., Türkiye) reported higher levels of negative affectivity than their counterparts in more individualistic countries (e.g., Finland; Slobodskaya et al., 2019). This might be because children’s expression of emotions is considered more normative in more individualistic cultures (Cho et al., 2022; Friedlmeier et al., 2011), resulting in British parents reporting less negative emotionality in their children compared to their Turkish parents (Aytac et al., 2019).

Second, our results showed that while the total, direct and indirect effects of child negative emotionality on MPIP and MPIC were significant in the UK, only its indirect effect was significant in Türkiye. That is, the culture did not moderate the indirect effect of children’s negative impact on mindful parenting. As hypothesised and in line with previous findings (Laukkanen et al., 2014; Xing et al., 2017), increased child temperamental difficulty predicted higher levels of psychological distress in parents, which in turn resulted in less mindful parenting in both cultures. However, perhaps more interestingly, child negative emotionality directly determined mindful parenting in the UK but not in Türkiye despite the higher negative emotionality of Turkish children reported by parents. This finding is somewhat consistent with recent research showing that child temperament did not predict abusive parenting and only weakly predicted coercive parenting in Turkish mothers (Gölcük & Kazak-Berument, 2021). Given that certain temperament tendencies of children are considered “tolerable” in some cultures while “difficult” in others (Harkness & Super, 1996; Son et al., 2020), the explanation can be that, compared to Türkiye parents, UK parents were assumably more sensitive to children’s temperament. Thus, children’s negative affect impaired UK parents’ mindful parenting over and above its effect via parental well-being.

However, these findings contradict the common view that parents in more individualistic cultures respond more supportive and less unsupportive to children’s expression of (negative) emotions (Cho et al., 2022; Friedlmeier et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2020). This may reflect the cross-cultural difference unique to mindful parenting beyond traditional models of parenting. Indeed, we know from previous research that Turkish parents may display more “mindful” attitudes towards the expression of negative emotions than European parents, e.g., “it is okay to feel angry” (Corapci et al., 2018, p. 273). Therefore, although Turkish parents reported more child negative emotionality, they might be able to stay mindful in the face of the expression of these emotions. Further research is needed to explore the culture-specific link between child temperament and mindful parenting.

Third, social support was correlated with MPIP and MPIC in Türkiye but not in the UK. Yet, the multiple-group path model showed that these differences were negligible. Accordingly, the total effects of social support on MPIP and MPIC were significant in both cultures. This result is in line with earlier studies showing that parents who perceive more social support showed more mindful (Wang & Lo, 2020) and positive parenting but less negative parenting (Lippold et al., 2018; Taraban et al., 2019), as well as that this relationship is similar across cultures (Serrano-Villar et al., 2017). Moreover, in both cultures, parental psychological distress fully mediated the associations between parents’ social support and MPIP and MPIC. Namely, social support did not directly predict parent- or child-report mindful parenting after accounting for parental well-being; it only indirectly affected mindful parenting by first reducing parental psychological distress. These results fit well with Belsky’s (1984) argument that the direct effect of social support on parenting is not as strong as its indirect effect through parental psychological well-being.

Taken together, our results show that mindful parenting is multiply determined by parent characteristics, child characteristics, and family social environment regardless of the reporter. Moreover, this process is somewhat similar across cultures. Our results also partially support Belsky’s (1984) claim that these determinants do not have an equal influence on parenting. To be specific, ‘parental psychological distress is a better determinant of mindful parenting than social support, which itself is a stronger determinant than child negative emotionality’ (Belsky, 1984; p. 63); the former was supported in both cultures, while the latter was supported only in Türkiye, as discussed above.

Limitations and future directions

As previously stated, despite the increasing research on mindful parenting, cross-cultural differences and child perceptions of mindful parenting have been understudied. This study is the first to directly compare the mindful parenting process in two cultures using dyadic parent-child data. Yet, first, our findings may be somewhat limited by the samples consisting predominantly of mothers and their typically developed children aged 11–16 years. As such, further work is needed to generalise the findings to different types of families. Note that we invited both mothers and fathers; therefore, such homogeneity of our samples as in previous studies (e.g., Kim et al., 2019; Lo et al., 2018; Moreira et al., 2018) may reflect that mothers are still primary caregivers of children. It seems that fathers need to be specifically targeted to better explore the mindful parenting process in fathers.

Second, self-report scales of parenting may cause biased results. Parents, especially from relatively more collectivist cultures such as Türkiye, may tend to self-report socially desirable parenting behaviours (Bernardi, 2006; Bornstein et al., 2015). Although our study increases the validity of the measurement using both parent and child reports, studies considering observational scales (e.g., Mindful Parenting Observational Scales; Geier et al., 2012) are warranted to capture a full picture of mindful parenting (Morsbach & Prinz, 2006).

Third, this study did not examine potentially confounding influences at the societal level, such as parenting beliefs and values or religion. These would be useful additions for, future studies so as to understand the origins of cross-cultural similarities and differences in the determinants of mindful parenting.

Fourth, we cannot establish causality due to the cross-sectional nature of our data. For example, the association between child temperament and mindful parenting has been suggested to be bidirectional, such that mindful parenting may decrease negative emotionality in children, and in turn, child negative emotionality may augment mindful parenting (Lengua & Kovacs, 2005). Child temperament and parental psychological well-being have also been shown to affect each other bidirectionally; as such, increased parental distress might be the risk factor for child negative emotionality (Wiggins et al., 2014). Moreover, the mindful parenting model has proposed that parental well-being is an outcome, rather than a predictor, of mindful parenting (Anand et al., 2021; Duncan et al., 2009). We encourage future research to use genetically informed (e.g., Oliver, 2015) and/or cross-lagged panel designs (Kenny, 2005) to explore directionality.

Implications

Notwithstanding these limitations, this study has valuable implications for mindful parenting research and practice. For example, considering the multiple risk factors for low mindful parenting, it may be best practice for non-clinical mindful parenting interventions to target especially high-risk parents, such as those with low social support and psychological well-being, as well as those with “difficult” children (Cowling & Van Gordon, 2022).

Using the multiple-group approach, moreover, we revealed culture-specific and culture-generic determinants of parenting. Here, our results imply that parental psychological well-being is perhaps the most critical determinant in the process of mindful parenting, showing its mediating role in the link from child temperament and social support to parents’ and children’s perceptions of mindful parenting parental both in the UK and Türkiye. Therefore, we suggest that preventive and therapeutic mindful parenting interventions for non-clinical samples (Potharst et al., 2021) may essentially focus on parents’ psychological well-being.

However, we also showed that parental vulnerability to child negative emotionality might vary across cultures, endorsing the importance of using cross-cultural mindful parenting research. We thus hope our results may encourage further cross-cultural research to reveal differences/similarities in the determinants of mindful parenting. Thereby, interventions in a given culture can be revised for parents with a lower likelihood of adopting mindful parenting rather than relying solely on mindful parenting models (Kil & Antonacci, 2020) and interventions derived from mostly western families (Anand et al., 2021).