Introduction

It is estimated that 50 million people are living with dementia and that dementia costs approximately 818 billion USD worldwide (Alzheimer’s Disease International, 2015). Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the leading cause of dementia and accounts for approximately 70-80% of diagnoses (Mendez, 2021; Moustafa, 2021). AD is defined as a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that impacts factors such as declarative memory, episodic memory, executive functioning, spatial and visuoperceptual processing, and language (McKhann et al., 2011; Mendez, 2021). The initial symptoms of AD generally appear in older adults (over 65 years); however, early-onset AD can begin between 30 and 60 years of age (National Institute on Aging, 2017). Despite the overwhelming burden of the disease, the causes of AD are unknown and current treatments are largely unsuccessful in preventing or slowing disease progression (Ang et al., 2020; Cutsuridis & Moustafa, 2017; Cutsuridis & Moustafa, 2016; Rasmussen & Langerman, 2019; Warren & Moustafa, 2022). Accordingly, there is a critical need to understand the progression of AD.

AD diagnoses are generally received in the later stages of the disease when the neurological damage is deemed irreversible (Hill et al., 2020; Rasmussen & Langerman, 2019). In turn, it is difficult to understand the progression of the disease. However, it is now recognized that AD may begin decades before the manifestation of clinical symptoms (Dubois et al., 2016a). In 2011, The National Institute on Aging and Alzheimer’s Association workgroup (NIA-AA) outlined a three-stage clinical diagnostic criterion for AD to understand how the disease progresses from normal cognition to dementia (Jack et al., 2011; Sperling et al., 2011). The workgroup defined these three stages as preclinical AD, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and AD dementia (Jack et al., 2011). Since 2011, a conceptual shift has occurred that considers the three AD stages existing on a continuum spanning from asymptomatic to significantly impaired (Dubois et al., 2016b; Jack et al., 2018). In turn, medical professionals and researchers increasingly acknowledge that targeting preclinical symptoms and early-stage cognitive decline could delay AD and prevent an impending public health crisis (Eppig et al., 2020). Developments in early treatment and diagnosis can also be beneficial to individuals via improving their quality of life, healthcare systems via decreasing the intensity and cost of care, and caregivers via preserving individuals cognitive and functional ability (for more information see, Dubois et al., 2016a, b; Leifer, 2003; Weimer & Sager, 2009).

Preclinical AD is an early stage of the disease where symptoms are clinically asymptomatic but precede longitudinal cognitive decline (Aisen et al., 2017; Budson & Solomon, 2012a). Studying preclinical AD is difficult as it requires highly detailed measures and because individuals often present as clinically ‘normal’ (Aisen et al., 2017; Budson & Solomon, 2012b; Sperling et al., 2011). In the literature, neuropsychological (e.g., cognitive tests), neuroimaging, and biomarker measures are frequently combined to detect early-stage AD (i.e.., preclinical AD) and predict MCI to AD conversion (Brown, 2015). In clinical practice, neuropsychological tests are the predominant methods used to identify AD. However, these cognitive and behavioral tests do have some inherent limitations. For example, commonly used multi-domain cognitive tests are often over-generalized and lack the specificity and sensitivity to detect subtle cognitive changes in early-stage AD (Arevalo-Rodriguez et al., 2021). Moreover, many functional and cognitive tests can only assess the later-stage symptoms of AD. However, research suggests that some cognitive measures—such as progressive episodic-memory decline—can be effectively captured by domain-specific tests and can be used to detect early-stage AD (El Haj et al., 2015; Warren et al., 2021). Nonetheless, the early stages of AD, such as individuals with a significant memory concern (SMC), are under-researched.

Prior research

Prior research has focused on three general types of neuropsychological tests for AD assessment. Specifically, the literature often studies specific cognitive tests of one domain (e.g., episodic memory), tests of functional impairment, and cognitive batteries. Regarding specific tests, the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) is the most common measure used in the literature. The RAVLT is a psychometrically rigorous measure of episodic memory, widely used in dementia and pre-dementia assessment. Previous research has shown that the RAVLT can differentiate between individuals categorized as cognitively normal (CN), SMC, MCI, and AD (Estevez-Gonzalez et al., 2003). More recently, Moradi et al. (2017) found a strong association between RAVLT scores and early-stage AD-related brain atrophy. Moreover, Warren et al. (2021) found that the RAVLT could predict conversion from MCI to AD. In turn, prior research suggests that the RAVLT may be an effective tool in highlighting subtle episodic-memory decline in individuals with SMC at risk of progressing to MCI (Moradi et al., 2017).

Functional impairments such as difficulty performing activities of daily living (ADL) are also used to study the stages of AD (McKhann et al., 2011). However, these functional tests are often developed for late-stage dementia and can lack the sensitivity required to detect the subtle functional impairments in SMC and MCI (Knopman & Caselli, 2012). To overcome this late-stage bias, researchers suggest using self-report and informant-report measures more sensitive to the early stages of AD (Knopman & Caselli, 2012). For example, self-report may be useful in capturing early functional decline in SMC, and informant-report measures may be beneficial for capturing decline during MCI due to the overt anosognosia accompanying the disorder (Knopman & Caselli, 2012; Ryan et al., 2019). Instrumental ADLs (IADL) are functional activities such as cooking, transportation, and managing finances that allow an individual to live independently (Marshall et al., 2012). As IADL relies heavily on cognitive functioning, they are generally detected during the MCI stage (Marshall et al., 2012). However, IADL measures sensitive to subtle functional decline may also help detect SMC (Marshall et al., 2012). The Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ) (Pfeffer et al., 1982) is a subjective, informant-rated test of IADL. Research has shown the total FAQ score can discriminate between CN, MCI, and AD dementia (Marshall et al., 2012). However, due to floor effects, the FAQ has previously failed to capture early functional changes in CN individuals at-risk of AD (Marshall et al., 2015). Subsequently, further research is required to understand the ability of functional tests to detect the early stages of AD, such as SMC.

Cognitive batteries or general cognitive tests are also commonly used to assess AD. These general tests are the most common clinical assessment tool and usually contain various cognitive measures. Multiple general tests have been used in the literature, such as the Everyday Cognition Questionnaire (ECog), Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – Cognitive Scales (ADAS-Cog), Montreal Cognitive Assessment Scale (MoCA), and the Trail Making Test, Part B (TMT-B). The ECog (Farias et al., 2008) is a self and informant-rated questionnaire that assesses subjective perception of functional performance across memory, language, semantic knowledge, visuospatial abilities, and executive functioning. These domains have been found to correlate with objective cognitive measures, AD biomarkers, and neuroimaging indicators of AD (Farias et al., 2008). Longitudinal studies have further demonstrated that baseline ECog performance in CN individuals can predict subsequent progression to MCI (Farias et al., 2013). Similarly, the ADAS-Cog is a neuropsychological assessment that can determine the severity of the cognitive dysfunction or decline in individuals with AD or MCI (Rosen et al., 1984). The two versions—the ADAS-Cog-11 and ADAS-Cog-13—assess memory, language, praxis, and orientation to determine an individual’s level of cognitive functioning (Skinner et al., 2012). The MoCA has also been used to assess cognition in MCI and dementia. It is a short screening tool that evaluates an individual’s attention, working memory, episodic memory, executive function, language, and visuospatial abilities. It has been shown to have high sensitivity and specificity for determining whether individuals have MCI (Nasreddine et al., 2005). Lastly, the TMT-B measures executive function, including the cognitive processes of working memory, inhibition, and attention. A decline in executive function usually occurs in the early stages of AD and contributes to cognitive impairment (Baudic et al., 2006). To our knowledge, the TMT-B has not been applied to SMC and evidence suggests that it cannot significantly distinguish CN from MCI (Papp et al., 2014). In conclusion, multiple general tests are widely used to assess MCI and AD dementia. However, further research is required to understand the ability of general tests to detect the early stages of AD, such as SMC.

The current study

Prior research has shown that neuropsychological tests can detect the middle-to-late stages of the AD continuum, such as MCI and AD. However, there is less research on the early stages of the disease, such as SMC and early-stage MCI (EMCI). Accordingly, we plan to evaluate the use of neuropsychological tests to identify the various stages of the AD continuum. Accordingly, in this study, we will investigate whether commonly used cognitive and functional assessments can predict CN, SMC, EMCI, late-stage MCI (LMCI), and AD dementia (for more information on the characteristics and differences between EMCI and LMCI see, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, 2008, p. 27). We have specifically chosen to use the RAVLT, ECog, FAQ, ADAS-Cog, MoCA, and TMT-B as these measures are widely used but lack application to early-stage AD. Specifically, we examine whether these measures can predict the progression from CN, SMC, EMCI, LMCI, and AD. In turn, we hypothesize that the RAVLT (i.e., immediate, learning, forgetting, or forgetting percentage), ADAS-cog (i.e., ADAS-11 and ADAS-13), FAQ, ECog, and MoCA will predict progression towards AD. That is, for every unit increase in these test scores, there will be a significant increase in the odds of an individual progressing toward AD.

Methods

Data source and acquisition

We obtained the data for this study from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) dataset. ADNI recruits its participants through its website, media advertisement (newspaper, radio, and social media), and third-party health care providers. ADNI collects and deidentifies data at various ADNI sites across North America using trained clinicians. As in prior studies (Alashwal et al., 2020; Moustafa et al., 2021; Warren et al., 2021), The ADNI data can be categorized into four different collection periods, ADNI-1 (2004 to 2009), ADNI-GO (2009 to 2011), ADNI-2 (2011 to 2016), and ADNI-3 (2016 to 2021). Each of these stages involves the addition of new measures and participants as well as the continued testing of some prior participants. Access to the ADNI dataset was obtained via a prior application, and institutional ethics approval was obtained from our university’s Human Research Ethics Committee in April 2021. Therefore, this study was in compliance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later addenda. For security, we stored the data on a password-protected computer that was only accessible to the research team.

Participants

This study includes 596 participants aged 55 to 90 years old who identified as Male (n = 287) or Female (n = 309). Of these participants, 140 were diagnosed as CN, 191 as SMC, 162 as EMCI, 68 as LMCI, and 35 as AD. These diagnoses are confirmed by ADNI clinicians using the Cognitive Change Index (CCI), Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR), the Logical Memory subtest (LMT) of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, the Mini-mental State Exam (MMSE), and criteria from the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association Alzheimer’s Criteria (NINCDS-ADRDA). It is important to note that our SMC participants could be operationalized as individuals with Subjective Cognitive impairments because of the tests used in this study. However, we maintain the use of the term SMC as it is the diagnostic term used by ADNI in our data. For more information on the diagnostic criteria, see the ADNI procedures manual (Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, 2017). Before the main analysis, we ran a sample size analysis using the software G*Power (Faul et al., 2009; Faul et al., 2007). We found that a sample size of 178 would be sufficient to detect if the neuropsychological assessments could predict AD progression (f2 = .15, power = 0.95; n-predictors =11, α = .05).

Scales

Our analysis used six neuropsychological scales to detect cognitive and functional decline across the AD spectrum. These scales are outlined below.

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)

The RAVLT (Rey, 1964) is an episodic-memory test that assesses participants’ ability to learn 15 nouns across five learning trials, to recall the words immediately following an interference word list, and to recognize and recall words after a 30-minute time delay. Four summary scores are derived from RAVLT raw scores, including the following: Immediate (participants’ recall immediately following the first list learning trial), Forgetting (number of words forgotten across all trials), Learning (the number of correct words recalled across all trials), and Percent Forgetting (percentage quantification of the RAVLT forgetting score). The RAVLT is widely used in AD research because it targets episodic-memory decline, a fundamental aspect of early-stage AD progression (Warren et al., 2021).

Everyday Cognition (ECog)

The ECog (Farias et al., 2008) is a 39-item questionnaire that assesses the subjective perception of everyday cognitive functioning and performance across six domains: memory, language, semantic knowledge, visuospatial abilities, and executive functioning. Disfunctions across these six domains are markers of the neuropsychological decline (e.g., cognitive and functional decline) observed in AD. The ECog includes identical self-report and study-partner versions. Participants rate perceived ability on tasks compared to their ability ten years prior. The questionnaire is rated on a four-point scale where one is “better or no change compared with ten years earlier,” and four is “consistently much worse.” A total self-report score, and total study-partner score, are calculated by averaging the ratings across the 39 items.

Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ)

The FAQ (Pfeffer et al., 1982) is a 10-item, informant-rated measure of IADL. Decline in IADL is a significant marker of AD decline as discussed in the introduction above (p. 4). In the FAQ, each item is scored on a three-point scale where higher scores indicate greater impairment (0 = normal; 1 = has difficulty but does by self; 2 = requires assistance; 3 = dependent). No established cut-off score signifies functional impairment; however, research suggests a score of ≥6 out of a possible 30 (Nitrini et al., 2004).

Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – Cognitive Scales (ADAS-Cog)

The ADAS-Cog-11 (Rosen et al., 1984) measure consists of 11 subtests that assess numerous cognitive abilities whose impairment is associated with AD (e.g., executive function). The ADAS-Cog-13 includes these 11 subtests and two additional tests to assess executive and daily functioning associated with MCI. The measures take approximately 30-45 minutes to administer using a test kit. ADAS-Cog-11 scores range from 0 to 70, and ADAS-Cog-13 scores are from 0 to 85.

Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)

The MoCA (Nasreddine et al., 2005) is a cognitive screening instrument developed to detect mild cognitive impairment through assessing multiple cognitive domains with 11 tasks. The scores of all subtests are summed, and an additional point is given if an individual has completed 12 years or less of formal education. The maximum score possible is 30 points, and scores of 26 and above are considered normal. It takes ten minutes to administer using paper and a pencil. Unlike other scales, the MoCA was created specifically for MCI and is sensitive to mid-to-early stages symptoms of AD, such as short-term memory recall and executive functioning.

Trail Making Test, Part B (TMT-B)

The TMT-B (Reitan, 1955) task requires an individual to create an ascending pattern of alternating numbers and letters quickly and accurately. Mistakes are pointed out to participants who should correct them immediately, and the final score is based on the task completion time. It is completed in under 300 seconds, and the average score is 75 seconds, while a deficient score is greater than 273 seconds. The TMT-B assesses neuropsychological mechanism such as task switching and visual attention that decline with AD progression.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS for Windows (version 28.0). Frequencies and percentages are used for categorical variables and means, SDs, and 95% confidence intervals are reported for continuous variables. Pearson correlation coefficients are reported between all variables. The generalized linear model in SPSS was used to conduct an ordinal logistic regression to determine if the neuropsychological assessments predicted cognitive and functional decline across the AD spectrum.

Results

The descriptive statistics and correlations for the variables included in the ordinal regression are presented in Table 1. Notably, these findings indicate that the ADAS-11 and ADAS-13 are strongly correlated. Therefore, only the ADAS-13 was used in the following ordinal regression model.

Table 1 The mean, standard deviation, 95% confidence interval, and Pearson correlations for age and the ADNI neuropsychological assessment for AD

Multi-collinearity

The correlation coefficients in Table 1 indicate the presence multi-collinearity amongst some of the variables (e.g., ADNI 11 & ADNI 13; forgetting and forgetting %). To further investigate, VIF and tolerance values were obtained for each of the IVs in the hypothesised model. The analysis showed that the assumption of multicollinearity was violated (i.e., VIF > 5 and TOL < 0.10) for ADAS-11 (VIF = 18.22, Tol = .06), ADAS-13 (VIF = 24.35, Tol = .04), Forgetting (VIF = 6.34, Tol = .16), Forgetting % (VIF = 0.01, Tol = .10). After removing ADAS-11 and forgetting percentage the assumptions for multi-collinearity were met (i.e., all variables had a TOL > .10 and VIF <5).

Ordinal regression

An ordinal regression was conducted to test if gender, age, and neuropsychological assessments can predict the cognitive and functional decline across the AD spectrum (i.e., cognitive normal, Subjective Memory Complaints, early mild cognitive impairment, late cognitive impairment, and AD). Note that from the complete ADNI data set (N = 2129), only 596 participants had data for all the Neuropsychological assessments at baseline. Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics for each variable across all AD levels (i.e., CN, SMC, EMCI, LMCI, AD).

Table 2 The mean, standard deviation, 95% confidence interval, the ADNI neuropsychological assessment across all AD levels

The omnibus model for the ordinal regression was statistically significant χ2 (11, 596) = 455.23, p < .001; and the results confirmed the model was a good fit for the data, Pearson χ2 (2369) = 2053.60, p = .867. The results of the ordinal regression are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 Ordinal regression predicting participant’s cognitive and functional decline across the AD spectrum

As presented in Table 3, the model’s predictive power was improved by age, ADAS-13, RAVLT learning, FAQ, MOCA, Ecog-patient, and Ecog-partner. However, gender, RAVLT immediate, RAVLT forgetting, and TMT-B scores did not contribute significantly to the model. The odds ratio for age indicated that for every single unit increase in age, there was a small 2.00% significant decrease in the probability that an individual would progress towards AD. For RAVLT learning, there was a significant 8.30% decrease in the odds that a person would progress towards AD. There was a significant 13.69% increase in the odds that a person would progress towards AD for every unit increase in FAQ. A single unit increase in MOCA resulted in a significant 8.91% decrease in the odds that an individual would progress towards AD. For every single unit increase in a participant’s self-reported cognitive decline, there was a significant 152.37% increase in the odds of progressing towards AD. Finally, there was a significant 274.13% increase in the odds that a person would progress towards AD for every single unit increase in partner reported cognitive decline.

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to distinguish CN, SMC, EMCI, LMCI, and AD groups using cognitive and functional tests (the RAVLT, ECog, FAQ, ADAS-Cog, MoCA, and TMT-B). We focused on these measures because identifying differences in cognition and function could have potential real-world implications for detecting early-stage AD. We hypothesized that—out of the tests studied—the RAVLT (i.e., immediate, learning, forgetting, or forgetting percentage), ADAS-cog (i.e., ADAS-11 and ADAS-13), FAQ, ECog, and MoCA would predict progression towards AD. Our results provide evidence that some cognitive and functional tests can successfully predict the stages of the AD continuum (i.e., ADAS-13, RAVLT learning, FAQ, MOCA, and ECog). However, other neuropsychological assessments were not predictive of the stages of AD (i.e., RAVLT immediate and forgetting, and TMT-B).

Cognitive and functional measures in different dementia stages

We investigated the ability of the RAVLT to predict the different stages of the AD continuum (i.e., CN, SMC, EMCI, LMCI, and AD groups). As episodic-memory decline is an early symptom of AD, it was hypothesized that RAVLT scores would predict the progression towards AD. Surprisingly, this hypothesis was only confirmed for the RAVLT learning but the forgetting sub-scales were not predictive of the progression towards AD. Our findings support previous research that found that the RAVLT can differentiate between CN individuals and individuals with MCI and AD, suggesting that RAVLT scores accurately reflect underlying dementia pathology (Estevez-Gonzalez et al., 2003; Moradi et al., 2017; Warren et al., 2021). However, our findings did not support previous research suggesting that individuals with SMC have accelerated episodic-memory decline compared to healthy individuals without SMC (Samieri et al., 2014). Building on previous research, the current study found that the RAVLT is also sensitive to subtle episodic-memory differences between SMC and EMCI as well as EMCI and LMCI and may be a useful measure to pinpoint where an individual is on the AD spectrum. Therefore, future research should aim to develop measures of episodic memory sensitive to differentiating between healthy controls and SMC to capture at-risk individuals earlier in the disease progression.

We also investigated the ability of the ECog to predict the different stages of the AD continuum (i.e., CN, SMC, EMCI, LMCI, and AD groups). As predicted, results showed that the ECog Total self-report strongly predicted AD categories. Indeed, for every single unit increase in a participant’s self-reported cognitive decline, there was a 166.60% increase in the odds of progressing towards AD. The ECog partner reported cognitive decline was the strongest predictor within our model. For example, for every additional single unit increase in partner reported cognitive decline, there was a significant 307.40% increase in the odds that a person would progress towards AD. Contrary to our hypothesis, our results showed that the ECog Total study-partner was a significant predictor and the strongest predictor of the AD categories. These findings oppose previous research that suggests self-report measures are less viable the further individuals’ decline (Marshall et al., 2012). These findings suggest that subjective self-report measures may be important in detecting SMC or early-stage AD and that informant-report measures may be more viable for middle to late-stage AD. Accordingly, both measures should be used to effectively capture the transitions between the various stages of the AD continuum (Marshall et al., 2012).

We assessed the ability of the FAQ to predict differences in IADL across the AD continuum. Results confirmed our hypothesis and showed that the FAQ significantly predicted the progression toward AD. This finding supports previous research that found the FAQ could discriminate between IADL scores for CN, MCI, and AD dementia (Marshall et al., 2012). However, due to floor effects, the FAQ is not sensitive enough to capture early functional changes between healthy controls and individuals with SMC (Marshall et al., 2015). This result may be because the FAQ is an entirely informant-based measure and may not be appropriate for use in CN individuals for whom self-report may be more accurate. There is a critical need for IADL measures that can detect subtle changes in otherwise healthy individuals who report SMC. Accordingly, future research should consider administering a self-report version of the FAQ (Ryan et al., 2019), investigate whether individual items of the FAQ can distinguish CN and SMC, and develop scales that focus exclusively on complex ADL tasks in early-stage AD (Marshall et al., 2012).

Regarding the ADAS-Cog, we hypothesized that the ADAS-13 would predict the different stages of the AD Continuum. This hypothesis was based on research that demonstrated that the ADAS-Cog-11 can effectively differentiate between CN and those with MCI or early AD (Zainal et al., 2016). Similarly, research conducted by Skinner et al. (Skinner et al., 2012) using the ADNI dataset demonstrated that the ADAS-Cog-13 was a valid measure with high responsiveness for detecting MCI and AD. The current study’s results supported the hypothesis that the ADAS-Cog-13 scores significantly predict the five stages of AD included in our analysis. This finding also supports the notion that all five groups have been shown to have differing levels of cognitive impairment (Roberts et al., 2009). From these findings, we recommend that the ADAS-Cog-13 be used in clinical practice where individuals present with minor memory issues, as it can differentiate between the levels of cognitive functioning expected throughout the AD continuum.

We predicted that the MoCA would be useful in predicting the stages of AD, as some researchers have used it as a diagnostic measure to separate those with SMC, EMCI, LMCI, or AD (Larner, 2019). A meta-analysis (Breton et al., 2019) demonstrated that the MoCA has good diagnostic accuracy for identifying individuals with MCI. Our research showed that the MoCA could predict the AD continuum in line with the existing literature. For example, our results support previous findings that a decline in cognitive functioning—as detected by the MOCA—is a significant predictor of the progression toward AD. (Montejo Carrasco et al., 2017). Therefore, the findings supported our prediction, as the tool can correctly identify the progression of cognitive decline between all five AD continuum groups. In contrast, we expected that the TMT-B would be unable to classify individuals with SMC correctly. Specifically, research has suggested that the TMT-B cannot significantly differentiate between individuals who are CN, have MCI (Papp et al., 2014), or those with MCI or AD (Ashendorf et al., 2008). Our findings supported the previous literature as we observed that the TMT-B scores were not a significant predictor of AD progression. Accordingly, the results from our study call into question the use of the TMT-B as diagnostic measures in research and as screening tools for SMC in clinical practice.

Strengths

While extensive research has been carried out to investigate AD and MCI, there is limited evidence on SMC overall. A particular strength of the current study is that we identified four existing tools that are significant predictors of the progression from SMC to AD (i.e., ADAS-13, RAVLT – learning, FAQ, MoCA, and ECog). Our study also determined measures that cannot predict disease progression accurately (i.e., RAVLT immediate and forgetting, and TMT-B). This finding is important as the measures investigated are used in routine assessments of cognitive impairment; if clinicians know which tools can accurately detect SMC, they could be used when less severe memory issues are suspected. This change in practice would likely reduce the burden on the individual and the healthcare system, as fewer individuals would require a full AD or MCI assessment. We also suggest that cognitive measures which assess recall of instructions or visual words be further investigated to define their usefulness in SMC assessments. Research must continue to be conducted in this area so a clearer link between cognition and SMC can be established, and the definition of SMC can be streamlined to ensure consistency in assessment and diagnosis. This research is important in caring for the hundreds of thousands of dementia patients living with memory decline, as early SMC interventions may help prevent further deterioration into the later stages of dementia, for which there currently are no treatments or cures.

Limitations

Despite the beneficial clinical implications of this research, there are some limitations. Firstly, while the measures studied are used to assess function and cognition, they are only part of the diagnostic process; these diagnoses also require reports from the individual and their carers, together with neuroimaging and neuropsychological assessments. This scope means that cognitive tests alone, while useful, cannot be relied upon for complete diagnoses of any level of impaired cognitive functioning. Similarly, most measures do not consider a person’s ability before cognitive impairment or other issues such as vision or English language proficiency. These are important factors that can impact performance and indicate whether there is a true decline in the cognitive domain of memory or whether the person’s poor performance can be attributed to a different cause. Regarding the data, the ADNI sample is a highly characterized Northern American sample and does not represent the general population. All groups showed a considerably high mean level of education (15–16 years), which may affect the generalisability of the results. Some measures that rely on subjective-report (such as the ECog and the FAQ) may also be inherently biased and have been criticized as they do not assess an individual’s objective abilities (Marshall et al., 2012). Similarly, the information provided by study partners may be influenced by confounding factors, including; whether the informant lives with the participant, the relationship status between participant and informant, and the number of hours a week they spend together and in what circumstances (Ryan et al., 2019). These limitations should be considered when generalizing our results.

Conclusions and future directions

It is predicted that the prevalence of dementia will increase across the globe. Research is therefore focused on earlier diagnosis and intervention at the preclinical stage. This research requires using sensitive cognitive measures that are fast and inexpensive to administer by clinicians in a primary health care setting (Marshall et al., 2012). Such tests can be used as initial AD screeners to identify at-risk individuals for follow-up assessments using biomarker tests. As the current study highlights, commonly used cognitive scales in AD clinical trials (such as some RAVLT measures, the ECog, and FAQ) reliably detect decline as individuals transition from SMC, to MCI, to AD; however, these scales are less sensitive to capturing subtle decline between healthy individuals and those with SMC in preclinical AD. Therefore, developing highly sensitive measures that emphasize episodic-memory decline, complex cognitive functioning, and IADL tasks in preclinical AD will help identify those at risk of AD and allow for earlier treatment. We hope this research can help clinicians identify the early stages of AD, such as SMC, and encourage further research into neuropsychological tests for the AD continuum.