Skip to main content
Log in

Beyond a Crime Gene: Genetic Literacy and Correctional Orientation

  • Published:
American Journal of Criminal Justice Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Is there a “crime gene”? This question has been answered by the scientific community, and the response is a definitive “no.” Yet, it is unclear whether this information has been communicated to the general public. Furthermore, it is unclear whether people’s views about the genetics of crime influence their perceptions of the way offenders should be treated. This study uses attribution theory to understand how perceptions of the role of genetic factors in criminal behavior influence beliefs about the punishment, redeemability, and rehabilitation of offenders. Drawing on a national sample of White respondents (N = 392), this study finds that only a small proportion believe there is a single crime gene. Compared to other respondents, those who believe crime is caused by a single gene believe that punishment should be weakened, are less supportive of rehabilitation efforts, and believe that offenders are less capable of redemption. Implications of these findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. These scale characteristics correspond with previous research using the care/harm scale (see, e.g., Chowdhury, 2019; Smith, Alford, Hibbing, Martin, & Hatemi, 2017; Yilmaz & Saribay, 2017).

  2. At the request of reviewers, the SUR model was estimated with the genetic literacy variable recoded as a dichotomy between the “gene X environment interaction” response and all others. Additionally, a reviewer suggested collapsing the “single gene” and “several genes” responses into one category when estimating this model. Neither of these models produced estimates for genetic literacy that were substantively different from the models presented in Table 3, so we opted to present the SUR model because it had greater explained variation in the dependent variables than the models estimated using the coding of this variable as suggested by reviewers. These results are available upon request.

References

  • Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amazon Mechanical Turk. (2018). How it works. Retrieved from https://www.mturk.com/how-it-works.

  • Appelbaum, P. S., Scurich, N., & Raad, R. (2015). Effects of behavioral genetic evidence on perceptions of criminal responsibility and appropriate punishment. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 21(2), 134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aspinwall, L. G., Brown, T. R., & Tabery, J. (2012). The double-edged sword: Does biomechanism increase or decrease judges' sentencing of psychopaths? Science, 337(6096), 846–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkan, S. E., & Cohn, S. F. (2005). Why whites favor spending more money to fight crime: The role of racial prejudice. Social Problems, 52(2), 300–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, J. C., Liu, H., Motz, R. T., Tanksley, P. T., Kail, R., Beckley, A. L., et al. (2019). The propensity for aggressive behavior and lifetime incarceration risk: A test for gene-environment interaction (G × E) using whole-genome data. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 49, 101307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, J. C., & Tielbeek, J. (2018). The behavioral genetics of aggression and violent behavior. In A. Vazsonyi, D. Flannery, & M. DeLisi (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Violent Behavior and Aggression (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsley, D. A., Kuh, E., & Welsch, R. E. (1980). Regression diagnostics: Identifying influential data and sources of collinearity. New York: Wiley.

  • Binkley, J. K. (1982). The effect of variable correlation on the efficiency of seemingly unrelated regression in a two-equation model. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 77(380), 890–895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobo, L. D., & Johnson, D. (2004). A taste for punishment: Black and white Americans’ views on the death penalty and the war on drugs. Du Bois Review: Social Science Research on Race, 1(1), 151–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, M. J. (1997). The southern subculture of punitiveness? Regional variation in support for capital punishment. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 34(1), 25–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunner, H. G., Nelen, M., Breakfield, X. O., Ropers, H. H., & van Oost, B. A. (1993). Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase a. Science, 262(5133), 578–580.

  • Burton, A. L., Cullen, F. T., Burton, V. S., Graham, A., Butler, L. C., & Thielo, A. J. (2020). Belief in redeemability and punitive public opinion: “Once a criminal, always a criminal” revisited. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 47(6), 712–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Taylor, A., & Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582), 851–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chabris, C. F., Lee, J. J., Cesarini, D., Benjamin, D. J., & Laibson, D. I. (2015). The fourth law of behavior genetics. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 304–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, L., & Krosnick, J. A. (2009). National surveys via RDD telephone interviewing versus the internet. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(4), 641–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, B. Y., & Heine, S. J. (2015). The double-edged sword of genetic accounts of criminality: Causal attributions from genetic ascriptions affect legal decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(12), 1723–1738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chowdhury, R. M. (2019). The moral foundations of consumer ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 158, 585–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chudy, J. (2017). Racial sympathy in American politics (unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

  • Chudy, J. (2018, February 16). Racial sympathy and its limits. Paper presented at the research in American politics workshop, Boston University: Boston.

  • Chudy, J. (2020). Racial sympathy and its political consequences. Journal of Politics. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1086/708953.

  • Cochran, J. K., Boots, D. P., & Heide, K. M. (2003). Attribution styles and attitudes toward capital punishment for juveniles, the mentally incompetent, and the mentally retarded. Justice Quarterly, 20(1), 65–93.

  • Cohen, D. M. (2019). Justice, not jailbreak: The context and consequence of the first step act. Victims & Offenders, 14(8), 1084–1098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conley, D., & Fletcher, J. (2018). The genome factor: What the social genomics revolution reveals about ourselves, our history, and the future. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  • Cullen, F. T., Clark, G. A., Cullen, J. B., & Mathers, R. A. (1985). Attribution, salience, and attitudes toward criminal sanctioning. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 12, 305–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cullen, F. T., & Gilbert, K. E. (1982). Reaffirming rehabilitation. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiIulio, J. J., Jr. (1995, November 27). The coming of the super-predators. Weekly Standard, 1(11), 23–28.

  • Dodd, S. (2018). The punitive woman? Gender differences in public attitudes toward parole among an australian sample. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 62(10), 3006–3022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, P. (2016). Incarceration nation: How the United States became the most punitive democracy in the world. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, P. K. (2014). The public's increasing punitiveness and its influence on mass incarceration in the United States. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 857–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falco, D. L., & Turner, N. C. (2014). Examining causal attributions towards crime on support for offender rehabilitation. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 630–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Filindra, A., & Kaplan, N. J. (2016). Racial resentment and whites’ gun policy preferences in contemporary America. Political Behavior, 38(2), 255–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J. (1991). Regression diagnostics: An introduction. Vol. 79, Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

  • Gordon, R. A., & Anderson, K. S. (1995). Perceptions of race-stereotypic and race-nonstereotypic crimes: The impact of response-time instructions on attributions and judgments. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 16(4), 455–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gromet, D. M., & Darley, J. M. (2011). Political ideology and reactions to crime victims: Preferences for restorative and punitive responses. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 8(4), 830–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannan, K., Cullen, F. T., Butler, L., Burton, A. R., Graham, A., & Burton, V. S. (2020). Racial sympathy and support for capital punishment: A case study in concept transfer. Preprint https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/xybj9.

  • Harrison, L. A., & Esqueda, C. (2001). Race stereotypes and perceptions about black males involved in interpersonal violence. Journal of African American Men, 5(4), 81–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of personal relations. New York: Wiley.

  • Hewitt, J. K. (2012). Editorial policy on candidate gene association and candidate gene-by-environment interaction studies of complex traits. Behavior Genetics, 42(1), 1–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Innocence Project. (2020). DNA exonerations in the United States: Fast Facts. Retrieved February 12, 2020 from https://www.innocenceproject.org/dna-exonerations-in-the-united-states/.

  • Johnson, D. (2008). Racial prejudice, perceived injustice, and the black-white gap in punitive attitudes. Journal of Criminal Justice, 36(2), 198–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. (2009). Anger about crime and support for punitive criminal justice policies. Punishment & Society, 11(1), 51–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

  • Leverentz, A. (2011). Neighborhood context of attitudes toward crime and reentry. Punishment & Society, 13, 64–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maruna, S., & King, A. (2009). Once a criminal, always a criminal? “Redeemability” and the psychology of punitive public attitudes. European Journal of Criminal Policy and Research, 15(1–2), 7–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson-Crotty, S., Peterson, D. A., & Ramirez, M. D. (2009). Dynamic representation(s): Federal criminal justice policy and an alternative dimension of public mood. Political Behavior, 31(4), 629–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Hear, M. M., & Wheelock, D. (2016). Public attitudes toward punishment, rehabilitation, and reform: Lessons from the Marquette law school poll. Federal Sentencing Reporter, 29(1), 16–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliphant, J. B. (2016, September 29). Support for death penalty lowest in more than four decades. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/09/29/support-for-death-penalty-lowest-in-more-than-four-decades/

  • Oliphant, J. B. (2018, June 11). Public support for death penalty ticks up. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/06/11/us-support-for-death-penalty-ticks-up-2018/

  • Ouellette, H. M., Applegate, B. K., & Vuk, M. (2017). The public’s stance on prisoner reentry: Policy support and personal acceptance. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 42, 768–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Page, B. I., & Shapiro, R. Y. (1983). Effects of public opinion on policy. American Political Science Review, 77(1), 175–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickett, J. T. (2019). Public opinion and criminal justice policy: Theory and research. Annual Review of Criminology, 2, 405–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickett, J. T., & Baker, T. (2014). The pragmatic American: Empirical reality or methodological artifact? Criminology, 52, 195–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Platt, A. M. (1969). The child savers: The invention of juvenile delinquency. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., & Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rafter, N., Posick, C., & Rocque, M. (2016). The criminal brain: Understanding biological theories of crime (2nd ed.). New York: NYU Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, D. J. (1980). Conscience and convenience: The asylum and its alternatives in progressive America. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabatello, M., & Appelbaum, P. S. (2017). Behavioral genetics in criminal and civil courts. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 25(6), 289–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, J. E., Davies, E. H., & Pope, D. B. (2013). Collecting DNA at arrest: Policies, practices, and implications, final technical report. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scurich, N., & Appelbaum, P. (2016). The blunt-edged sword: Genetic explanations of misbehavior neither mitigate nor aggravate punishment. Journal of Law and the Biosciences, 3(1), 140–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, R. Y. (2011). Public opinion and american democracy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(5), 982–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silver, J. R. (2017). Moral foundations, intuitions of justice, and the intricacies of punitive sentiment. Law and Society Review, 51(2), 413–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silver, J. R., & Silver, E. (2017). Why are conservatives more punitive than liberals? A moral foundations approach. Law and Human Behavior, 41(3), 258–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sloas, L. B., & Atkin-Plunk, C. A. (2019). Perceptions of balanced justice and rehabilitation for drug offenders. Criminal Justice Policy Review, 30(7), 990–1009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, J. R., Martin, N. G., & Hatemi, P. K. (2017). Intuitive ethics and political orientations: Testing moral foundations as a theory of political ideology. American Journal of Political Science, 61(2), 424–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • State v. Waldroup. E2010–01906-CCA-R3-CD (Tenn. Crim. App. 2011).

  • Tabery, J. (2014). Beyond versus: The struggle to understand the interaction of nature and nurture. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tielbeek, J. J., Johansson, A., Polderman, T. J., Rautiainen, M. R., Jansen, P., Taylor, M., ... & Viding, E. (2017). Genome-wide association studies of a broad spectrum of antisocial behavior. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(12), 1242–1250.

  • Thielo, A. J., Graham, A., & Cullen, F. T. (2020). Opt-in internet surveys. In J. C. Barnes & D. R. Forde (Eds.), The encyclopedia of research methods and statistical techniques in criminology and criminal justice. New York, NY: Wiley. (In press).

  • Tuch, S. A., & Hughes, M. (2011). Whites’ racial policy attitudes in the twenty-first century: The continuing significance of racial resentment. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 634(1), 134–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkheimer, E. (1998). Heritability and biological explanation. Psychological Review, 105(4), 782–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unnever, J., Cullen, F., & Jonson, C. (2008). Race, racism, and support for capital punishment. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and justice: A review of research (Vol. 37, pp. 45–96). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Unnever, J. D., Cochran, J. K., Cullen, F. T., & Applegate, B. K. (2010). The pragmatic American: Attributions of crime and the hydraulic relation hypothesis. Justice Quarterly, 27(3), 431–457.

  • Unnever, J. D., & Cullen, F. T. (2010). The social sources of Americans’ punitiveness: A test of three competing models. Criminology, 48(1), 99–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Updegrove, A. H., Boisvert, D. L., Cooper, M. N., & Gabbidon, S. L. (2020). Criminological explanations, race, and biological attributions of crime as predictors of Philadelphia area residents’ support for criminal justice policies. Crime & Delinquency. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011128720931437.

  • Vassos, E., Collier, D. A., & Fazel, S. (2014). Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of genetic association studies of violence and aggression. Molecular Psychiatry, 19(4), 471–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, T. J., Bell Holleran, L., & Silver, J. R. (2019). Applying moral foundations theory to the explanation of capital jurors’ sentencing decisions. Justice Quarterly, 36(7), 1176–1205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeulen, E., Henneman, L., van El, C. G., & Cornel, M. C. (2014). Public attitudes towards preventive genomics and personal interest in genetic testing to prevent disease: A survey study. European Journal of Public Health, 24(5), 768–775.

  • Visscher, P. M., Wray, N. R., Zhang, Q., Sklar, P., McCarthy, M. I., Brown, M. A., & Yang, J. (2017). 10 years of GWAS discovery: Biology, function, and translation. American Journal of Human Genetics, 101(1), 5–22.

  • Walsh, A. & Beaver, K. M. (Eds.). (2009). Biosocial criminology: New directions in theory and research. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G. (1984). Current issues in attribution theory and research. Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 427–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (2010). Attribution theory. In P. Peterson, E. Baker, & B. McGraw (Eds.), International encyclopedia of education (Vol. 6, pp. 558–563). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wertz, J., Caspi, A., Belsky, D. W., Beckley, A. L., Arseneault, L., Barnes, J. C., et al. (2018). Genetics and crime: Integrating new genomic discoveries into psychological research about antisocial behavior. Psychological Science, 29(5), 791–803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, E. A., Love, A. C., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., Quigley, J., & Lee, J. J. (2019). Free will, determinism, and intuitive judgments about the heritability of behavior. Behavior Genetics, 49(2), 136–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wlezien, C., & Soroka, S. N. (2016). Public opinion and public policy. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia, Politics: Oxford University. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.74.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Yilmaz, O., & Saribay, S. A. (2017). Activating analytic thinking enhances the value given to individualizing moral foundations. Cognition, 165, 88–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zellner, A. (1962). An efficient method of estimating seemingly unrelated regressions and tests for aggregation bias. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 57(298), 348–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amanda Graham.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Racial Resentment

How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements: (1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree)

  1. 1.

    It is really a matter of some people not trying hard enough; if Blacks would only try harder, they could be just as well off as Whites

  2. 2.

    Irish, Italians, Jewish, and many other minorities overcame prejudice and worked their way up. Blacks should do the same without any special favors

  3. 3.

    Over the past few years, Blacks have gotten less than they deserve (R)

  4. 4.

    Generations of slavery and discrimination have created conditions that make it difficult for Blacks to work their way out of the lower class (R)

Redeemability

How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements: (1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree)

  1. 1.

    Given the right conditions, the average offender can turn their lives around and become law-abiding citizens

  2. 2.

    The average offender can go on to lead productive lives with help and hard work

  3. 3.

    The average offender is so damaged that they can never lead productive lives (R)

  4. 4.

    The average offender is unlikely to change for the better (R)

  5. 5.

    Despite their best efforts, the average offender just can’t manage to go back to living straight (R)

  6. 6.

    It’s possible for the average offender to change and lead a law-abiding life

Rehabilitation

How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements: (1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree)

  1. 1.

    It is a good idea to provide treatment for offenders who are supervised by the courts and live in the community

  2. 2.

    Rehabilitation programs should be available even for offenders who have been involved in a lot of crime in their lives

  3. 3.

    All rehabilitation programs have done is to allow criminals who deserve to be punished to get off easily (R)

  4. 4.

    It is important to try to rehabilitate adults who have committed crimes and are now in the correctional system

  5. 5.

    I would not support expanding the rehabilitation programs that are now being undertaken in our prisons (R)

Offender Exclusion

How much do you agree or disagree with the following statements: (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree)

  1. 1.

    If someone is convicted of a crime, they should be permanently excluded from sitting on a jury, even after they have paid their debt to society.

  2. 2.

    If I owned a company, I would never hire an ex-offender because I could never really trust them.

  3. 3.

    More often than not, it is a good idea to put criminal records on the Internet for anyone to see.

  4. 4.

    When prisoners return to society, we should avoid them and let them try to make it on their own.

Racial Sympathy

How much sympathy do you have for the person described in each of the scenarios below?

  1. 1.

    Michael is a young black man who lives in a Midwestern city. One day Michael is crossing the street and jaywalks in front of cars. Some local police officers see Michael jaywalk and stop and question him. Michael argues that he was just jaywalking and is otherwise a law-abiding citizen. The police officers feel that Michael is being uncooperative and so they give him a pat down to see if he is carrying any concealed weapons. Michael is very upset by this treatment.

  2. 2.

    Milford is a mid-sized city in the Northeast. The main bus depot for the city is located in the Whittier section of Milford, a primarily black neighborhood. Whittier community leaders argue that the concentration of buses produces serious health risks for residents; they point to the high asthma rates in Whittier as evidence of the bus depot’s harmful effects. The Milford Department of Transportation officials, who are mostly white, state that Whittier is the best location for the depot because it is centrally located and many Whittier residents take the bus. Furthermore, it would be expensive to relocate the bus depot to a new location. Whittier community leaders are very upset by the Department’s inaction.

  3. 3.

    Tim is a white man who owns a hair salon. His business is growing rapidly and so he decides to place an advertisement to hire new stylists. In the advertisement, he writes that interested applicants should come for an interview first thing next Monday. When he arrives at the salon on Monday, he sees a line of seven or eight people waiting outside the door, all of whom appear to be black. He approaches the line and tells the applicants that he’s sorry, but the positions have been filled. The applicants are upset; they feel they have been turned away because of their race.

  4. 4.

    Mrs. Lewis, a white woman with young children, posts advertisements for a nanny on community bulletin boards. She receives many inquiries and decides to interview all applicants over the phone. Mrs. Lewis is most impressed with a woman named Laurette, who has relevant experience, is an excellent cook, and comes enthusiastically recommended. Mrs. Lewis invites Laurette over for what she expects will be the final step of the hiring process. When Laurette arrives, Mrs. Lewis is surprised to see that Laurette is black. After Laurette’s visit, which goes very well, Mrs. Lewis thanks her for her time but says that she will not be offered the job. When Laurette asks why, Mrs. Lewis says that she doesn’t think that her children would feel comfortable around her. Laurette is upset about Mrs. Lewis’ actions.

Responses: 1 = a great deal of sympathy, 2 = a lot of sympathy, 3 = some sympathy, 4 = a little sympathy, 5 = I do not feel any sympathy.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Graham, A., Barnes, J., Liu, H. et al. Beyond a Crime Gene: Genetic Literacy and Correctional Orientation. Am J Crim Just 47, 485–505 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12103-020-09595-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12103-020-09595-5

Keywords

Navigation