Skip to main content
Log in

Children’s understanding of large-scale mapping tasks: an analysis of talk, drawings, and gesture

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research examined how children represent motion in large-scale mapping tasks that we referred to as “motion maps”. The underlying mathematical content was transformational geometry. In total, 19 children, 8- to 10-year-old, created motion maps and captured their motion maps with accompanying verbal description digitally. Analysis of the responses included a fine-grained coding of their drawing, oral description, and hand gestures used while describing. In addition, the classroom teacher (second author) also assessed the drawings and the oral responses. Results indicate that low achieving children produced fewer objects in their drawings, fewer gestures, and fewer verbal descriptions when engaging in a large-scale mapping task compared to high achieving children. Moreover, these children were found to use a series of connected small-scale maps to construct the larger-scale representation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alibali, M. W., & DiRusso, A. A. (1999). The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development, 14(1), 37–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, K. L., Casey, M. B., Thompson, W. L., Burrage, M. S., Pezaris, E., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2008). Performance on middle school geometry problems with geometry clues matched to three different cognitive styles. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2(4), 188–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botzer, G., & Yerushalmy, M. (2008). Embodied semiotic activities and their role in the construction of mathematical meaning of motion graphs. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 13(2), 111–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language in babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 85–107). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, K.-E., Sung, Y.-T., Chen, Y.-L., & Huang, L.-H. (2008). Learning multiplication through computer-assisted learning activities. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(6), 2904–2916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, K., Huttenlocher, J., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). 25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: A current theoretical perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(6), 1033–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clearfield, M. W. (2004). The role of crawling and walking experience in infant spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(3), 214–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H. (2004). Geometric and spatial thinking in early childhood education. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. Di Biase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 267–298). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106(2), 1047–1058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238(4833), 1556–1557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S. (1989). Young children’s understanding of the correspondence between a scale model and a larger space. Cognitive Development, 4(2), 121–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devichi, C., & Munier, V. (2013). About the concept of angle in elementary school: Misconceptions and teaching sequences. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 32(1), 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, S. B., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1259–1268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K., Tversky, B., & Taylor, H. A. (2000). Using space to describe space: Perspective in speech, sign, and gesture. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 2(3), 157–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erbas, A. K., & Yenmez, A. A. (2011). The effect of inquiry-based explorations in a dynamic geometry environment on sixth grade students’ achievements in polygons. Computers and Education, 57(4), 2462–2475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrara, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Newcombe, N. S., Golinkoff, R., & Lam, W. S. (2011). Block talk: Spatial language during block play. Mind, Brain and Education, 5(3), 143–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feyereisen, P., & Havard, I. (1999). Mental imagery and production of hand gestures while speaking in younger and older adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23(2), 153–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, A., & Newcombe, N. S. (2012). Getting the big picture: Development of spatial scaling abilities. Cognitive Development, 27(3), 270–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gagatsis, A., & Shiakalli, M. (2004). Ability to translate from one representation of the concept of function to another and mathematical problem solving. Educational Psychology, 24(5), 645–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Göksun, T., Goldin-Meadow, S., Newcombe, N., & Shipley, T. (2013). Individual differences in mental rotation: What does gesture tell us? Cognitive Processes, 14(2), 153–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holden, C. (2006). Hunter-gatherers grasp geometry. Science, 311(5759), 317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, V., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Development of sensitivity to geometry in visual forms. Human Evolution, 23(3), 213–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, C., & Lehrer, R. (2000). Teacher appropriation and student learning of geometry through design. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 31(1), 71–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, K. (2000). Providing a foundation for deductive reasoning: Students’ interpretations when using dynamic geometry software and their evolving mathematical explanations. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 44(1/2), 55–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendrick, M., & McKay, R. (2004). Drawings as an alternative way of understanding young children’s constructions of literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 4(1), 109–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(2), 54–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, M., Jankovic, I. N., & Pahj, M. (1982). Principles of spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 111(2), 157–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mammarella, I. C., Giofrè, D., Ferrara, R., & Cornoldi, C. (2012). Intuitive geometry and visuospatial working memory in children showing symptoms of nonverbal learning disabilities. Child Neuropsychology, 19(3), 235–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martínez, J. F., Stecher, B., & Borko, H. (2009). Classroom assessment practices, teacher judgments, and student achievement in mathematics: Evidence from the ECLS. Educational Assessment, 14, 78–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGillicuddy-de Lisi, A. V., & de Lisi, R. (1981). Children’s strategies in imagining spatio-geometrical transformations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4(2), 201–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Möhring, W., Newcombe, N.S., & Frick, A. (2014). Zooming in on spatial scaling: Preschool children and adults use mental transformations to scale spaces. Developmental Psychology, 50(5), 1614–1619.

  • Ontario Ministry of Education. (2010). Growing success: Assessment, evaluation, and reporting in Ontario schools. First edition, covering grades 1 to 12. Toronto: Queen’s Printer for Ontario.

  • Ontario Ministry of Education and Training/OMET. (2005). The Ontario curriculum grades 1–8 mathematics, revised. Toronto: Queen’s Printer for Ontario.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ping, R. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Hands in the air: Using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1277–1287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pruden, S. M., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2011). Children’s spatial thinking: Does talk about the spatial world matter? Developmental Science, 14(6), 1417–1430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quaiser-Pohl, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2004). The relationship between spatial abilities and representations of large-scale space in children–a structural equation modeling analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(1), 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2009). “No! He starts walking backwards!”: Interpreting motion graphs and the question of space, place and distance. ZDM - The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41(4), 467–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saads, S. M. L., & Edwards, C. W. (1997). Understanding shapes and space from reflection and rotation. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 28(3), 437–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sauter, M., Uttal, D. H., Alman, A. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2012). Learning what children know about space from looking at their hands: The added value of gesture in spatial communication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(4), 587–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shusterman, A., Ah Lee, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Cognitive effects of language on human navigation. Cognition, 120(2), 186–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, N., Healy, L., & Sales, C. O. R. (2009). Time for telling stories: Narrative thinking with dynamic geometry. ZDM - The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41(4), 441–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, N., & Moss, J. (2012). The more it changes, the more it becomes the same: The development of the routine of shape identification in dynamic geometry environment. International Journal of Educational Research, 51–52, 28–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T., & MacDonald, A. (2009). Time for talk: The drawing-telling process. Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 14(3), 21–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., Lee, S. A., & Izard, V. (2010). Beyond core knowledge: Natural geometry. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 863–884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunteler, E., Pronk, C. M. E., & Resing, W. C. M. (2008). Inter- and intra-individual variability in the process of change in the use of analogical strategies to solve geometric tasks in children: A microgenetic analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(1), 44–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, D. H. (1996). Angles and distances: Children’s and adults’ reconstruction and scaling of spatial configurations. Child Development, 67(6), 2763–2779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, D. H. (2000). Seeing the big picture: Map use and the development of spatial cognition. Developmental Science, 3(3), 286–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., et al. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: A meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 352–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasilyeva, M., & Huttenlocher, J. (2004). Early development of scaling ability. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 682–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woleck, K. R. (2001). Listen to their pictures: An investigation of children’s mathematical drawings. In A. A. Cuoco & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), The roles of representation in school mathematics (pp. 215–227). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the school participants and Ms. Laaraib Khattak, our research assistant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Donna Kotsopoulos.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kotsopoulos, D., Cordy, M. & Langemeyer, M. Children’s understanding of large-scale mapping tasks: an analysis of talk, drawings, and gesture. ZDM Mathematics Education 47, 451–463 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-014-0661-4

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-014-0661-4

Keywords

Navigation