Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Priority-Setting and Personality: Effects of Dispositional Optimism on Preferences for Allocating Healthcare Resources

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In a publicly financed health system, it is important that priority-setting reflects social values. Many studies investigate public preferences through surveys taken from samples, but to be representative, these samples must reflect value judgments of all relevant population subgroups. In this study, we explore whether, next to better-understood sources of heterogeneity such as age, education or gender, also differences in personality play a role in how people want to set limits to health care. We investigate the influence of dispositional optimism: whether someone anticipates a good or bad future. This is an important personality dimension that has been shown to widely reverberate into people’s lives and that can also be expected to influence people’s views on health care. To test our hypothesis, we asked a representative sample of the Belgian population (N = 750) to complete both the revised life orientation test and a discrete choice experiment about allocating healthcare resources, and we investigated the relationships between both measurements. We found that more pessimistic individuals were less supportive of using patients’ age as a selection criterion and more hesitant to invest in prevention. Since individual dispositions are usually not part of the criteria for selecting representative samples, our findings point at a potential non-response bias in studies that elicit social values.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andersson, M. A. (2012). Dispositional optimism and the emergence of social network diversity. Sociological Quarterly, 53(1), 92–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asaria, M., Griffin, S., & Cookson, R. (2016). Distributional cost-effectiveness analysis: A tutorial. Medical Decision Making, 36(1), 8–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berndsen, M., & van der Pligt, J. (2001). Time is on my side: Optimism in intertemporal choice. Acta Psychologica, 108(2), 173–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brissette, I., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2002). The role of optimism in social network development, coping, and psychological adjustment during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 102–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryan, S., & Dolan, P. (2004). Discrete choice experiments in health economics. For better or for worse? Eur J Health Econ, 5(3), 199–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Lehman, J. M., & Antoni, M. H. (2003). Dispositional pessimism predicts illness-related disruption of social and recreational activities among breast cancer patients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 813–821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2014). Dispositional optimism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(6), 293–299.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2010). Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 879–889.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chiesi, F., Galli, S., Primi, C., Borgi, P. I., & Bonacchi, A. (2013). The accuracy of the life orientation test-revised (LOT-R) in measuring dispositional optimism: Evidence from item response theory analyses. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(5), 523–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cleemput, I. (2014). Incorporating societal preferences in reimbursement decisions: Relative importance of decision criteria according to Belgian citizens. KCE Reports 234.

  • Cookson, R., Ali, S., Tsuchiya, A., & Asaria, M. (2018). E-learning and health inequality aversion: A questionnaire experiment. Health Economics, 27(11), 1754–1771.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Corr, P., & Matthews, G. (2009). The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, N., & Sabin, J. (2008). Setting limits fairly: Learning to share resources for health. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, P., Shaw, R., Tsuchiya, A., & Williams, A. (2005). QALY maximisation and people’s preferences: A methodological review of the literature. Health Economics, 14(2), 197–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, H., & Menzel, P. (2012). Prevention vs treatment: What’s the right balance?. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A. L., Wellman, J. A., & Lassiter, G. D. (2009). Dispositional optimism and engagement: The moderating influence of goal prioritization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 913–932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A. L., Wellman, J. A., Seligman, L. D., Wuyek, L. A., & Neff, L. A. (2010). Dispositional optimism, goals, and engagement in health treatment programs. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 33(2), 123–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, B., & Sanbonmatsu, D. M. (2004). Optimism, pessimism, and gambling: The downside of optimism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2), 149–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giles, L. C., Glonek, G. F., Luszcz, M. A., & Andrews, G. R. (2005). Effect of social networks on 10 year survival in very old Australians: The Australian longitudinal study of aging. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 59(7), 574–579.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Glaesmer, H., Rief, W., Martin, A., Mewes, R., Brahler, E., Zenger, M., et al. (2012). Psychometric properties and population-based norms of the Life Orientation Test Revised (LOT-R). British Journal of Health Psychology, 17, 432–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gu, Y., Lancsar, E., Ghijben, P., Butler, J. R., & Donaldson, C. (2015). Attributes and weights in health care priority setting: A systematic review of what counts and to what extent. Social Science and Medicine, 146, 41–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, J. B. (2002). Managerial optimism and corporate finance. Financial Management, 31(2), 33–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hecht, D. (2013). The neural basis of optimism and pessimism. Exp Neurobiol, 22(3), 173–199.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heinonen, K., Raikkonen, K., Matthews, K. A., Scheier, M. F., Raitakari, O. T., Pulkki, L., et al. (2006). Socioeconomic status in childhood and adulthood: Associations with dispositional optimism and pessimism over a 21-year follow-up. Journal of Personality, 74(4), 1111–1126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herdman, M., Gudex, C., Lloyd, A., Janssen, M., Kind, P., Parkin, D., et al. (2011). Development and preliminary testing of the new five-level version of EQ-5D (EQ-5D-5L). Quality of Life Research, 20(10), 1727–1736.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kam, C., & Meyer, J. P. (2012). Do optimism and pessimism have different relationships with personality dimensions? A re-examination. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(2), 123–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, E. S., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2011). Dispositional optimism protects older adults from stroke: The Health and Retirement Study. Stroke, 42(10), 2855–2859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lancsar, E., Wildman, J., Donaldson, C., Ryan, M., & Baker, R. (2011). Deriving distributional weights for QALYs through discrete choice experiments. J Health Econ, 30(2), 466–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luyten, J., Kessels, R., Goos, P., & Beutels, P. (2015). Public preferences for prioritizing preventive and curative health care interventions: A discrete choice experiment. Value in Health, 18(2), 224–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macleod, A. K., & Conway, C. (2005). Well-being and the anticipation of future positive experiences: The role of income, social networks, and planning ability. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3), 357–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, B., & Schutz, A. (2005). Who are the people reluctant to participate in research? Personality correlates of four different types of nonresponse as inferred from self- and observer ratings. Journal of Personality, 73(4), 959–984.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, K. A., Raikkonen, K., Sutton-Tyrrell, K., & Kuller, L. H. (2004). Optimistic attitudes protect against progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(5), 640–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, D. (1974). Conditional logit analysis of qualitative choice behavior. In P. Zarembka (Ed.), Frontiers in econometrics (pp. 105–142). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McPherson, J., & Mohr, P. (2005). The role of item extremity in the emergence of keying-related factors: An exploration with the life orientation test. Psychological Methods, 10(1), 120–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mooney, G. H., & Blackwell, S. H. (2004). Whose health service is it anyway? Community values in healthcare. Medical Journal of Australia, 180(2), 76–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mosing, M. A., Gordon, S. D., Medland, S. E., Statham, D. J., Nelson, E. C., Heath, A. C., et al. (2009). Genetic and environmental influences on the co-morbidity between depression, panic disorder, agoraphobia, and social phobia: A twin study. Depression and Anxiety, 26(11), 1004–1011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nes, L. S., Evans, D. R., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2009). Optimism and college retention: Mediation by motivation, performance, and adjustment(1). Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(8), 1887–1912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICE. (2008). Social value judgements: Principles for the development of NICE guidance. Retrieved April 4, 2017 from https://www.nice.org.uk/proxy/?sourceUrl=http%3a%2f%2fwww.nice.org.uk%2fmedia%2fC18%2f30%2fSVJ2PUBLICATION2008.pdf.

  • Olsen, J. A., Richardson, J., Dolan, P., & Menzel, P. (2003). The moral relevance of personal characteristics in setting health care priorities. Social Science and Medicine, 57(7), 1163–1172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • OMHLTC. (2013). A report on the ontario citizen’s council QALYs and drug funding decisions in Ontario. O. m. o. h. a. l.-t. care.

  • Pavlova, M. K., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2013). Dispositional optimism fosters opportunity-congruent coping with occupational uncertainty. Journal of Personality, 81(1), 76–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puri, M., & Robinson, D. T. (2007). Optimism and economic choice. Journal of Financial Economics, 86(1), 71–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radcliffe, N. M., & Klein, W. M. P. (2002). Dispositional, unrealistic, and comparative optimism: Differential relations with the knowledge and processing of risk information and beliefs about personal risk. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(6), 836–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rauch, W. A., Schweizer, K., & Moosbrugger, H. (2007). Method effects due to social desirability as a parsimonious explanation of the deviation from unidimensionality in LOT-R scores. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(8), 1597–1607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rius-Ottenheim, N., Kromhout, D., de Craen, A. J., Geleijnse, J. M., van der Mast, R. C., Zitman, F. G., et al. (2012a). Parental longevity correlates with offspring’s optimism in two cohorts of community-dwelling older subjects. Age (Dordr), 34(2), 461–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rius-Ottenheim, N., Kromhout, D., van der Mast, R. C., Zitman, F. G., Geleijnse, J. M., & Giltay, E. J. (2012b). Dispositional optimism and loneliness in older men. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27(2), 151–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality the comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 313–345.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • RobinsonWhelen, S., Kim, C. T., MacCallum, R. C., & KiecoltGlaser, J. K. (1997). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: Is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 1345–1353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, M., Gerard, K., & Amaya-Amaya, M. (2008). Using discrete choice experiments to value health and health care preface. Using Discrete Choice Experiments to Value Health and Health Care, 11(Xiii), Xiv.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health—assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4(3), 219–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063–1078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., Matthews, K. A., Owens, J. F., Schulz, R., Bridges, M. W., Magovern, G. J., et al. (1999). Optimism and rehospitalization after coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Archives of Internal Medicine, 159(8), 829–835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwappach, D. L. (2002). Resource allocation, social values and the QALY: A review of the debate and empirical evidence. Health Expectations, 5(3), 210–222.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S. C. (2007). Optimism and resources: Effects on each other and on health over 10 years. J Res Pers, 41(4), 772–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S. C., Evans, D. R., & Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A. (2011). Optimism and pessimism dimensions in the life orientation test-revised: Method and meaning. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(1), 126–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, K. K. (2009). Severity of illness and priority setting in healthcare: A review of the literature. Health Policy, 93(2–3), 77–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sirven, N., & Debrand, T. (2012). Social capital and health of older Europeans: Causal pathways and health inequalities. Social Science and Medicine, 75(7), 1288–1295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. T., Edens, J. F., Epstein, M., Stiles, P. G., & Poythress, N. G., Jr. (2012). Do the psychopathic personality traits of fearless dominance and self-centered impulsivity predict attitudes about and influences on research participation? Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 30(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srivastava, S., McGonigal, K. M., Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2006). Optimism in close relationships: How seeing things in a positive light makes them so. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 143–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Standaard, D. (2014). G20 000 voor medicijnen. Retrieved April 4, 2017 from http://www.standaard.be/cnt/dmf20140302_01006486.

  • Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Aspinwall, L. G., Schneider, S. G., Rodriguez, R., & Herbert, M. (1992). Optimism, coping, psychological distress, and high-risk sexual behavior among men at risk for acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 460–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tindle, H. A., Chang, Y. F., Kuller, L. H., Manson, J. E., Robinson, J. G., Rosal, M. C., et al. (2009). Optimism, cynical hostility, and incident coronary heart disease and mortality in the Women’s Health Initiative. Circulation, 120(8), 656–662.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr. Michael Shiner and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Funding

The authors acknowledge funding from the Research Foundation—Flanders (FWO, Project Numbers G098911N and G043815N, and Roselinde Kessels’ postdoctoral fellowship) and Pfizer’s European HTAcademy prize competition (2011) and the Antwerp Study Centre for Infectious Diseases (ASCID) at the University of Antwerp. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JL, PD and PB framed the research question and set up the experiment. RK and PG designed the experiment. RK and JL analysed the data. All authors were involved in the writing of the text.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeroen Luyten.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Ethical Standards

The Committee for Medical Ethics of the University of Antwerp reviewed the study protocol, the questionnaire and the information letter for participants and approved them on 16 March 2015. The market research company Ipsos conducted the survey and provided the responses for analysis in anonymous form only.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix A: Example of a Choice Set

Medical interventions A and B are exactly equally expensive, and they apply to a similar number of patients. If you were forced to make a choice, which of both interventions should be reimbursed by the government? To make it easier for you, we have highlighted in yellow the characteristics that differ between both interventions. There are no right or wrong answers; we are interested in your opinion.

 

A

B

What type of intervention is it?

Curative (meant to cure patients who are ill)

Preventive (meant to prevent healthy persons from becoming ill)

How big is the probability of success of the intervention?

2 in 3 is successful

Always successful

How often do adverse effects occur?

Often

Often

How severe is the illness for which the intervention is developed?

Not lethal, but everyone who gets the disease will experience a severe and lasting reduction in quality of life

Lethal, everyone who gets the disease will die from it

Does the patient cause the disease through his or her own lifestyle?

Not at all

Not at all

How long does it take before the patient becomes ill/shows signs/symptoms of illness?

Within a year

Within a year

At what age does the patient become ill?

0–10 years

40–50 years

Your preference

Appendix B: MNL Model

Formally, the MNL model employs random utility theory which describes the utility that a respondent attaches to intervention j (j = 1, 2) in choice set s (s = 1, …, 14) as the sum of a systematic and a stochastic component:

$$ U_{js} = {\mathbf{x^{\prime}}}_{js} {\varvec{\upbeta}} + \varepsilon_{js} . $$

In the systematic component, \( {\mathbf{x^{\prime}}}_{js} {\varvec{\upbeta}},{\mathbf{x}}_{js} \) is a vector containing the attribute levels of intervention j in choice set s. Additionally, in our analysis, this vector includes the interactions between the attribute levels and the LOT-R score or any other respondent variable under investigation. The vector β is the vector of parameter values indicating the importance respondents attach to the different attribute levels and interactions. The stochastic component \( \varepsilon_{js} \) is the error term capturing the unobserved sources of utility. Under the assumption that the error terms are independently and identically Gumbel distributed, the MNL probability that a respondent chooses intervention j in choice set s is

$$ p_{js} = \frac{{\exp \left( {{\mathbf{x^{\prime}}}_{js} {\varvec{\upbeta}}} \right)}}{{\exp \left( {{\mathbf{x^{\prime}}}_{1s} {\varvec{\upbeta}}} \right) + \exp \left( {{\mathbf{x^{\prime}}}_{2s} {\varvec{\upbeta}}} \right)}}. $$

To estimate the parameter vector β, we used a maximum likelihood estimation approach, which maximizes the probability of obtaining the responses from the selected data sample. A positive estimate has a positive effect on the total utility, whereas a negative estimate has a negative effect. We computed the overall significance of the attributes and interactions by means of likelihood ratio (LR) tests. Such tests evaluate the difference in goodness of fit between nested models. More specifically, they compare the goodness of fit of an unrestricted or full model to a restricted model in which one or more parameters have been set to zero.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Luyten, J., Kessels, R., Desmet, P. et al. Priority-Setting and Personality: Effects of Dispositional Optimism on Preferences for Allocating Healthcare Resources. Soc Just Res 32, 186–207 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-019-00329-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-019-00329-5

Keywords

Navigation