Skip to main content
Log in

The origin of centrality deficit in text memory and comprehension by poor comprehenders: a think-aloud study

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study employed a think-aloud method to explore the origin of a centrality deficit (i.e., poor recall of central ideas) found in poor comprehenders (PC). Moreover, utilizing the diverse think-aloud responses, we examined the overall quality of text processing employed by PC during reading, in order to shed more light on the cognitive underpinnings underlying their poor comprehension and memory after reading. To address these goals, adolescents with good and poor comprehension, matched on reading (decoding) skills, were asked to state aloud whatever comes to their mind during the reading of two expository texts. After reading, the participants freely recalled text ideas and answered multiple-choice questions on the texts. Results indicated that PC exhibited lower performance than good comprehenders (GC) on the recall and comprehension tasks. The think-aloud protocols indicated that PC generated fewer responses than GC that reflect high-level, deep text processing, and more responses that reflect low-level, surface text processing. Furthermore, compared to GC, PC reinstated fewer prior text ideas, with this reduction being significantly greater for central than for peripheral ideas. Finally, the proportions of deep processing responses in general were positively associated with participants’ performance on recall and comprehension tasks. These findings suggest that PC exhibit poor text comprehension and memory, particularly of central ideas, because they construct a low-quality, poorly-connected text representation during reading, and produce fewer, less-elaborated retrieval cues for subsequent text comprehension and memory. This explanation is further illuminated in the context of previous findings and theoretical accounts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Responses were also scored as valid-plausible or invalid-implausible ones. However, we analyzed only the valid responses, because the proportions of invalid responses were low (> 3%) and similar in the two groups.

  2. The number of central and peripheral units was not equal, because an unequal number of lowest central and highest peripheral units received identical scores and were included or excluded together in the analyses.

  3. Information units presented in the think-aloud setting were occasionally larger than the original units, in order to reduce the amount of interruptions (reduced to 36, 31, and 27 units in Self-starvation, the Coca plant, and the Prophetess from Delphi, respectively) and avoid units which were too short for formulating a proper response. Nevertheless, all statistical analyses were conducted based on the original parsing of information units.

  4. Responses which included a repetition or a paraphrase of one text word were classified as ‘no response’, because they appeared as artificial attempts to provide some response by force rather than authentic, spontaneous attempts to process the text.

  5. Means in this ANOVA reflected aggregated proportions of four categories of think-aloud responses.

  6. Similar sets of regressions were also conducted with each individual category of think-aloud responses. However, the effects with most response categories did not reach significance, possibly due to an insufficient amount of data obtained for regression analyses in each individual category. We did find that the amount of connective inferences positively predicted participants’ performance in the recall task (p < .001), as was captured by the intra-text connections construct (see Table 4), and the amount of irrelevant associations negatively predicted participants’ accuracy on the comprehension questions (p = .01).

References

Download references

Funding

This research was supported by a Grant Number 485/15 from the Israel Science Foundation to Menahem Yeari.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Menahem Yeari.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1

The following table specifies in more detail the coding schema developed to classify participants’ responses into different think-aloud categories. Following the table, there are several more general coding rules.

Category

Definition

Coding highlights

Comments

Connecting inferences

Explaining an idea by stating a different idea or several ideas that appeared previously in the text

Information that appeared in previous ideas, even if the response includes just a few words

Ambiguous referential responses that do not include a clarification of the references (e.g., “This”, “He”, “Because of that”), are not coded as Connecting inferences

When a response refers to several ideas, the connection is determined to the idea that includes the explicit words stated in the response. If there is no such idea, the connection is determined to the last idea that represents the information in the response

Indicate the number(s) of the idea(s) to which the connection is made

An Evaluative comment that uses a connection to previous ideas is coded as Connecting inference rather than as Evaluative comment (e.g., “a strange principle but applicable to modern society”, expressing an evaluative comment about the principle, but connecting the explanation to a previous idea)

A response that includes Connecting inference and Evaluative comment that do not share the same content is coded separately in both categories

A response that uses prior knowledge to clarify the connection to previous ideas is coded as Connecting inference, rather than as Elaborative inference or as both categories

Elaborative inferences

Explaining an idea by using information from prior knowledge that did not appear explicitly in the text

Relevant explanatory or elaborativeinformation that did not appear previously in the text

If the information is not relevant to the text content, the response is coded as Association

An association which is related to the text content (e.g., “anorexia is an illness” in Self-starving, “drugs” in Coca, “tall plant” in Coca)

A knowledge-based expansion which is mentioned to clarify the connection with previous ideas is coded as Connecting inference

A response that includes knowledge-based information, which is beyond the connection to previous ideas, is coded as Elaborative inference as well

An evaluative comment that uses previous knowledge is coded as Elaborative inference

Predictive Inferences

Stating what will happen later in the text

The participant predicts information that will appear later in the text

The use of predictive words like “probably” or “apparently” are not necessarily coded as Predictive inference, unless it refers to what will come later in the text

Metacognitive comments

Reflecting the degree of understanding or other cognitive processes

Responses that include comments about comprehension like “I don’t understand”, “the text is hard for me”, “the idea is unclear”, “don’t understand the word”, “that’s more sensible”, “ah, that’s what I thought”

Reflections about other cognitive processes (e.g., “I was not concentrating”, “I’m trying to remember what was mentioned before”)

Retrospective comments (“I knew this would happen”) are coded as Metacognitive comments, rather than as Predictive inferences

Reflections about rhetorical or structural functions at text level (e.g., “they give an example for the use of coca plant”) or idea level (e.g., “they explain about the plant”) are coded as Metacognitive comments

Reflections at decoding level (e.g., “How do you read this word?”) are not coded as Metacognitive comment, but rather as No response

Text repetitions

Repeating the idea using the exact same words

A response should repeat at least 2 words (independent of the number of words in the idea)

A repetition of one word is coded as No response

A minimal change in word inflection or structure is still coded as Text repetition (“they made a pilgrimage” instead of “pilgrimage”)

A response is coded as Text repetition only if it lacks connecting, elaborative, and/or predictive inferences

A text repetition which is used as part of an associative description or expression of opinion, is coded as Association (e.g., “It reminds me that I learned in literature about the city of Delphi”) or Evaluative comment

If a response includes a text repetition which is not part of an association or opinion, then it is coded as both categories

Paraphrases

Repeating the gist of an idea using different words

A response should include a paraphrase of at least 2 words (independent of the number of words in the idea)

A paraphrase of one word is coded as No response

Use of synonymous words (lots instead of a large quantity)

A response that includes both paraphrasing (of at least two words) and text repetition is coded as Paraphrase

A response is coded as Paraphrase only if it lacks connecting, elaborative, and/or predictive inferences

Limited use of elementary prior knowledge for paraphrasing (“done in America and Europe” paraphrased as “done all over the world”) is coded as Paraphrase, rather than Elaborative inference

Associations

Expanding an idea using information from prior knowledge which is irrelevant to the text content or to the understanding of the text

Stating information that doesn’t appear in the text, but is not relevant to the text content or does not contribute to the understanding of the text

Referring to personal matters or to matters that digress from the text content

A repeated use of an association should be carefully checked whether it advances the participant’s understanding of the text

Evaluative comments

An affective or any subjective opinion on the text

Expressing subjective impression of the text (e.g., “boring”, “hard”, “long”) that doesn’t relate to specific knowledge or content

Expanding the text based on beliefs or emotions, which are not justified by specific prior knowledge (“She must have really made it up”, “I don’t believe it’s true”)

Expressing curiosity or criticism

Agreeing or disagreeing with the text content

If an evaluative comment includes use of prior knowledge to explain it, then the knowledge-based information is coded as Elaborative inference if it is relevant, or Association if it is not relevant to the text content

An evaluative comment which includes a connection to previous idea(s) is coded as Connecting inference

An evaluative comment which includes text repetition or paraphrases is coded as Evaluative comment

Supplementary general comments

  • An information unit in a response that matches both the first four, high-level response categories and the last four, low-level response categories is coded according to the high-level response category.

  • Questions that appear in responses (“Which messages?”, “What do the mountains look like?”) are considered as statements and coded accordingly. A response that includes only question words (What, Why, Where) is coded as No response. Critical questions (e.g., “why… would men do this?”, “To call the city?”) are coded as Evaluative comments.

  • Visualizing responses (“I see a picture of…”, “I imagine…”, “I thought about the words…”) are considered as statements and coded accordingly (e.g., “I imagined someone shooting up drugs” in Coca is coded as Association; “I imagined a prophetess sitting on a chair” in Prophetess of Delphi is coded as text repetition).

  • When a participant refers to his or her preceding response using general words such as “the same” or “like I just said”, the preceding response is coded again in relation to the current idea.

  • Do not ascribe intentions to the participants regarding information that have not actually been stated, unless it is totally clear and implied by his/her words. Avoid using your own knowledge or understanding of the text to interpret participant responses.

Appendix 2

An example of one of the experimental texts divided into information units. Underlined are the central units and the peripheral units are in italics. The remaining units are those that received intermediate centrality scores and were not included in the analyses.

The coca plant

  • The coca plant is a common bush in South America.

  • It is a domesticated bush, which reaches a height of 2–3 m.

  • This plant has a variety of uses, some of which were widespread in the past and some that are common today.

  • In ancient times, coca leaves were used mainly as a pain medication.

  • Chewing the coca leaves increased the sensitivity threshold of the sufferer.

  • However, the sufferer did not reach a state of unconsciousness or lose the sense of touch.

  • Also, coca leaves alleviate headaches and dizziness.

  • These pains were caused by living in the oxygen-low environment of the high Andes peaks, since the peaks of the Andes mountains can reach up to 7000 m.

  • The Andean population also used coca leaves as a nutritional supplement.

  • Coca leaves contain a large amount of proteins and vitamins, and therefore the Andean people often consumed them.

  • In addition to the coca leaves, the Andean population also consumed other food products, such as quinoa.

  • These plants are able to grow in the harsh conditions of the Andes.

  • The coca plant also had symbolic and religious meanings among the Andean population.

  • Researchers believe that Andeans believed they had to chew coca leaves when performing religious ceremonies, so that their prayers would be answered.

  • These ceremonies were aimed at local gods, whose identity was different from region to region.

  • Coca leaves also have uses today.

  • It was found that they contain chemicals that have a strong effect on the human brain.

  • These substances include alkaloids and cocaine.

  • This substance is produced from the dangerous cocaine drug.

  • In 1884, cocaine was isolated from the coca plant.

  • Since then it has been associated with the euphoria it evokes.

  • Dependence on cocaine also causes the user to give up food, drink and other necessary activities.

  • Other materials found in Coca are used, among other things, for the production of the Coca-Cola beverage.

  • The drink was invented by a pharmacist named John S. Pemberton in 1886.

  • Today it is the best-selling drink in the United States.

  • Coca-Cola has a lot of political power, and has a great impact on the American economy.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yeari, M., Lantin, S. The origin of centrality deficit in text memory and comprehension by poor comprehenders: a think-aloud study. Read Writ 34, 595–625 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10083-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10083-9

Keywords

Navigation