Skip to main content
Log in

Does rhetorical competence moderate the effect of rhetorical devices on the comprehension of expository texts beyond general comprehension skills?

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many studies have demonstrated the facilitating role of rhetorical devices in text comprehension, but there are also studies where rhetorical devices have not shown such effect. The present study sets out to explore whether readers’ knowledge of rhetorical devices (that is, rhetorical competence) moderates their effectiveness beyond general comprehension skills and, consequently, whether rhetorical competence may be considered a component skill of reading comprehension. 192 sixth- to seventh-grade students were assessed on rhetorical competence and were required to read a difficult marked text with specific rhetorical devices (a refutation, an objective, and four organizational signals) or the same text without them. After reading, students produced a summary in order to obtain three dependent variables: main ideas (as a measure of participants’ ability to select relevant information from the text), causal links between them (as an indicator of participants’ ability to grasp the logical structure of the text and to organize its ideas), and the combination of main ideas plus causal links (as an indicator of participants’ global comprehension of the text). Analyses controlling for general comprehension skills and other important variables (working memory, prior knowledge, decoding) demonstrated that: (a) readers of the marked text scored higher in terms of all dependent variables, and (b) rhetorical competence level moderated the effect of rhetorical devices on the composite measure (main ideas plus causal links) and on the organization of the summary by means of causal links.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Acuña, S. R., García-Rodicio, H., & Sánchez, E. (2011). Fostering active processing of instructional explanations of learners with high and low prior knowledge. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 26, 435–452. doi:10.1007/s10212-010-0049-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, S., & West, G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barreyro, J. P., Burin, D. I., & Duarte, D. A. (2009). Capacidad de la memoria de trabajo verbal. Validez y fiabilidad de unatarea de amplitud de lectura [Verbal working memory capacity. Validity and reliability of a reading span task]. Interdisciplinaria, 26, 207–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L., & Dole, A. (1992). Reading and thinking with history and science text. In C. Collins & J. M. Mangieri (Eds.), Teaching thinking: An agenda for the twenty-first century (pp. 1–22). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britton, B. K. (1994). Understanding expository text: Building mental structure to induce insights. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 641–674). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britton, B. K., & Gülgöz, S. (1991). Using Kintsch’s model to improve instructional text: Effects of inference calls on recall and cognitive structures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 329–345. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.3.329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. W., Spurlin, J. E., Dansereau, D. F., & Holley, C. D. (1983). Effects of headings on text-processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 292–302. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.75.2.292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (2003). Text comprehension and its relation to coherence and cohesion in children’s fictional narratives. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 335–351. doi:10.1348/026151003322277739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Nash, H. M. (2011). The influence of connectives on young readers’ processing and comprehension of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 429–441. doi:10.1037/a0022824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Patson, N., & Andrews, L. (2005). Age- and ability-related differences in young readers use of conjunctions. Journal of Child Language, 32, 877–892. doi:10.1017/S0305000905007014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple correlation-regression analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crosson, A. C., & Lesaux, N. K. (2013). Does knowledge of connectives play a unique role in the reading comprehension of English learners and English-only students? Journal of Research in Reading, 36, 241–260. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2011.01501.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual-differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Vega, M. (2005). El procesamiento de oraciones con conectoresadversativos y causales [Processing of sentences with causal or adversative connectives]. Cognitiva, 17, 85–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Degand, L., & Sanders, T. J. M. (2002). The impact of relational markers on expository text comprehension in L1 and L2. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 739–757. doi:10.1023/A:1020932715838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diakidoy, I. A. N., Kendeou, P., & Ioannides, C. (2003). Reading about energy: The effects of text structure in science learning and conceptual change. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 335–356. doi:10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00039-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, M. F., & Remond, M. (1997). Skilled and less skilled comprehenders: French children’s processing of anaphoric devices in written texts. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 291–308. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1997.tb00522.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, M. F., Remond, M., & Tardieu, H. (1999). Processing of anaphoric devices in young skilled and less skilled comprehenders: Differences in metacognitive monitoring. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 29–63. doi:10.1023/A:1007996502372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elosúa, M. R., Gutiérrez, F., García-Madruga, J. A., Luque, J. L., & Gárate, M. (1996). Adaptaciónespañoladel “Reading span test” de Daneman y Carpenter [Spanish adaptation of “Reading span test” from Daneman and Carpenter]. Psicothema, 8, 383–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelen, J. A. A., Bouwmeester, S., de Bruin, A. B. H., & Zwaan, R. A. (2014). Eye movements reveal differences in children’s referential processing during narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 118, 57–77. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fajardo, I., Tavares, G., Ávila, V., & Ferrer, A. (2013). Towards text simplification for poor readers with intellectual disability: When do connectives enhance text cohesion? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 1267–1279. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • García, J. R., Bustos, A., & Sánchez, E. (2015). The contribution of knowledge about anaphors, organisational signals and refutations to reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 38, 405–427. doi:10.1111/1467-9817.12021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1996). Coherence cues mapping during comprehension. In J. Costermans & M. Fayol (Eds.), Processing interclausal relationship in the production and comprehension of text (pp. 3–21). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilabert, R., Martínez, G., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2005). Some good texts are always better: Text revision to foster inferences of readers with high and low prior background knowledge. Learning and Instruction, 15, 45–68. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Givón, T. (1992). The grammar of referential coherence as mental processing instructions. Linguistics, 30, 5–55. doi:10.1515/ling.1992.30.1.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S. R., & Rakestraw, J. A. (2000). Structural aspects of constructing meaning from text. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 311–335). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A., & Britton, B. (1996). Five metaphors for text understanding. In B. Britton & A. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 341–352). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A., & Goodman, S. H. (1985). How to construct conceptual graph structures. In B. K. Britton & J. B. Black (Eds.), Understanding expository text (pp. 363–383). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A., Millis, K. K., & Zwaan, R. A. (1997). Discourse comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 163–189. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. Essex, England: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hynd, C. R. (2001). Refutational texts and the change process. International Journal of Educational Research, 35, 699–714. doi:10.1016/S0883-0355(02)00010-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2007). The effects of prior knowledge and text structure on comprehension processes during reading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1567–1577. doi:10.3758/BF03193491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension. A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, E., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Strategies to promote active learning from text: Individual differences in background knowledge. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 54, 141–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W., & Yarbrough, J. C. (1982). Role of rhetorical structure in text comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 828–834. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.74.6.828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemarie, J., Lorch, R. F., Eyrolle, H., & Virbel, J. (2008). SARA: A text-based and reader-based theory of signaling. Educational Psychologist, 43, 27–48. doi:10.1080/00461520701756321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linderholm, T., Everson, M. G., van den Broek, P., Mischinski, M., Crittenden, A., & Samuels, J. (2000). Effects of causal text revisions on more- and less-skilled readers’ comprehension of easy and difficult texts. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 525–556. doi:10.1207/S1532690XCI1804_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linderholm, T., Virtue, S., Tzeng, Y., & van den Broek, P. W. (2004). Fluctuations in the availability of information during reading: Capturing cognitive processes using the landscape model. Discourse Processes, 37, 165–186. doi:10.1207/s15326950dp3702_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loman, N. L., & Mayer, R. E. (1983). Signaling techniques that increase the understandability of expository prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 402–412. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.75.3.402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., & Lorch, E. P. (1985). Topic structure representation and text recall. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 137–148. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.77.2.137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L., Gava, M., & Boldrin, A. (2008). On warm conceptual change: The interplay of text, epistemological beliefs, and topic interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 291–309. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (1996). Learning strategies for making sense out of expository text: The SOI model for guiding three cognitive processes in knowledge construction. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 357–371. doi:10.1007/BF01463939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrudden, M. T., & Schraw, G. (2007). Relevance and goal-focusing in text processing. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 113–139. doi:10.1007/s10648-006-9010-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrudden, M. T., & Schraw, G. (2010). The effects of relevance instructions and verbal ability on text processing. Journal of Experimental Education, 78, 96–117. doi:10.1080/00220970903224529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from texts: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes, 22, 247–288. doi:10.1080/01638539609544975.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 1–43. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1401_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J. P. (2009). Towards a comprehensive model of comprehension. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 51, pp. 297–384). New York, NY, US: Elsevier Science. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(09)51009-2.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Megherbi, H., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2005). Language impairment in less skilled comprehenders: The on-line processing of anaphoric pronouns in a listening situation. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 715–753. doi:10.1007/s11145-005-8131-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F. (1985). Prose analysis: Purposes, procedures and problems. In B. K. Britton & J. B. Black (Eds.), Understanding expository text. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F., Brandt, D. M., & Bluth, G. J. (1980). Use of top-level structure in text: Key for reading comprehension of 9th-grade students. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 72–103. doi:10.2307/747349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F., & Poon, L. W. (2001). Effects of structure strategy training and signaling on recall of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 141–159. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millis, K. K., Graesser, A. C., & Haberlandt, K. (1993). The impact of connectives on the memory for expository texts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 317–339. doi:10.1002/acp.2350070406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millis, K. K., & Just, M. A. (1994). The influence of connectives on sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 128–147. doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millis, K. K., & Magliano, J. P. (1999). The co-influence of grammatical markers and comprehender goals on the memory for short discourse. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 183–198. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, J. D. (1995). Logical connectives and local coherence. In R. Lorch & E. O’Brien (Eds.), Sources of coherence in reading (pp. 107–126). NJ Laurence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noordman, L. G. M., Vonk, W., & Kempff, H. J. (1992). Causal inferences during the reading of expository texts. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 573–590. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(92)90029-W.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443–468. doi:10.1080/01690960344000008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., & Yuill, N. (1986). Pronoun resolution in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders: Effects of memory load and inferential complexity. Language and Speech, 29, 25–37. doi:10.1177/002383098602900104.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Reilly, T., & McNamara, D. S. (2007). Reversing the reverse cohesion effect: Good texts can be better for strategic, high-knowledge readers. Discourse Processes, 43, 121–152. doi:10.1080/01638530709336895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramos, J. L., & Cuetos, F. (1999). Batería de evaluación de losprocesoslectoresen el alumnadodeltercerciclo de educaciónprimaria y educaciónsecundariaobligatoria (PROLEC-SE) [Battery assessment of reading processes in secondary education]. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, R. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1982). Influence of questions on the allocation of attention during reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 623–632. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.74.5.623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richgels, D. J., McGee, L. M., Lomax, R. G., & Sheard, C. (1987). Awareness of 4 text structures: Effects on recall of expository text. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 177–196. doi:10.2307/747664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roller, C. M. (1990). The interaction between knowlege and structure variables in the processing of expository prose. Reading Research Quarterly, 25, 78–89. doi:10.2307/747595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothkopf, E. Z., & Kaplan, R. (1972). Exploration of the effect of density and specifity of instructional objectives on learning from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 295–302. doi:10.1037/h0033586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, E. (1998). Comprensión y redacción de textos [Comprehension and writing of texts]. Barcelona: Edebé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, E., & García, J. R. (2009). The relation of knowledge of textual integration devices to expository text comprehension under different assessment conditions. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 1081–1108. doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9145-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, E., García, J. R., & Gonzalez, A. J. (2007). Can differences in the ability to recognize words cease to have and effect under certain reading conditions? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 290–306. doi:10.1177/00222194070400040101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, E., García-Rodicio, H., & Acuña, S. R. (2009). Are instructional explanations more effective in the context of an impasse? Instructional Science, 37, 537–563. doi:10.1007/s11251-008-9074-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez, R. P., Lorch, E. P., & Lorch, R. F. (2001). Effects of headings on text processing strategies. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 418–428. doi:10.1006/ceps.2000.1056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, E., Rosales, J., & Cañedo, I. (1999). Understanding and communication in expositive discourse: An analysis of the strategies used by expert and preservice teachers. Teaching and Teacher Education, 15, 37–58. doi:10.1016/S0742-051X(98)00033-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, T. J. M., & Noordman, L. G. M. (2000). The role of coherence relations and their linguistic markers in text processing. Discourse Processes, 29, 37–60. doi:10.1207/S15326950dp2901_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, T. J. M., Spooren, W. P. M., & Noordman, L. G. M. (1992). Toward a taxonomy of coherence relations. Discourse Processes, 15, 1–35. doi:10.1080/01638539209544800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spyridakis, J. H., & Standal, T. C. (1987). Signals in expository prose effects on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 285–298. doi:10.2307/747969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, B. M. (1982). Text structure and children’s comprehension and memory for expository material. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 323–340. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.74.3.323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, B. M., & Beach, R. W. (1984). The effects of text structure instruction on middle-grade student’s comprehension and production of expository text. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 134–146. doi:10.2307/747358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P., & Kendeou, P. (2008). Cognitive processes in comprehension of science texts: The role of co-activation in confronting misconceptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 335–351. doi:10.1002/acp.1418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Schoot, M., Vasbinder, A. L., Horsley, T. M., & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2008). The role of two reading strategies in text comprehension: An eye fixation study in primary school children. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 203–223. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00354.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wessa P. (2015). Box–cox normality plot (Version 1.1.11) in free statistics software (v1.1.23-r7). Office for Research Development and Education. http://www.wessa.net/rwasp_boxcoxnorm.wasp/.

  • Williams, J. P., Nubla-Kung, A. M., Pollini, S., Stafford, K. B., Garcia, A., & Snyder, A. E. (2007). Teaching cause-effect text structure through social studies content to at-risk second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 111–120. doi:10.1177/00222194070400020201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. (1988). Understanding of anaphoric relations in skilled and less skilled comprehenders. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 173–186. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02282.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. (1991). Children’s problems in text comprehension. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1177/026565909200800207.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Support for this research and the writing of this paper was provided by a research project financed by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (EDU2012-33593) and a third author’s grant funded by the Comisión Nacional de Investigación Científica y Tecnológica (Conicyt) from Chile. We wish to thank the heads, teachers and students of the eight primary schools from Valparaíso (Chile) that participated in the study reported here: San Luis, San Ignacio de Loyola, Barros Luco, Jorge Williams, Seminario San Rafael, San Judas Tadeo, Internacional and Colegio Rubén Castro.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Ricardo García.

Appendix: Translation of “The Mediterranean Is Dying”

Appendix: Translation of “The Mediterranean Is Dying”

The state of the waters of the Mediterranean is described by experts as alarming, and some have even mentioned the imminent death of this historic sea.

With a surface area five times greater than Spain and an average depth of 1400 m (which makes it a relatively small sea), its waters lap the shores of 18 countries, continually receiving the urban and industrial waste of more than 150 million people. If we also consider the refuse generated by the 100 million tourists who visit its coasts every summer—a number that could double over the next 25 years—it is clear that these waters are irredeemably condemned to become a foul sewer.

Given that the Mediterranean’s waters are only renewed through the narrow Strait of Gibraltar, they are unable to support all this waste, which includes the millions and millions of tons of highly contaminating, extraordinarily dangerous toxic waste that are pumped into the water—petrol, mercury, lead, and so on.

Furthermore, the dumping of organic materials and fertilizers is leading to the growth of massive algae colonies, which cause problems for many of the living things that inhabit the sea.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sánchez, E., García, J.R. & Bustos, A. Does rhetorical competence moderate the effect of rhetorical devices on the comprehension of expository texts beyond general comprehension skills?. Read Writ 30, 439–462 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9684-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9684-2

Keywords

Navigation