Skip to main content
Log in

The contribution of executive functions to naming digits, objects, and words

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although it is established that reading fluency is more strongly related to serial naming of symbols than to naming of isolated items (serial superiority effect), the reason for the difference remains unclear. The purpose of this study was to examine the role of executive functions in explaining the serial superiority effect. One hundred seven Grade 6 Greek children were assessed on serial and discrete naming (digits, objects, and words), executive (inhibition, shifting, and updating) and non-executive tasks (simple choice reaction), and on a serial Rapid Alternating Stimuli task. Reading fluency correlated more strongly with serial naming than with discrete naming, consistent with the serial superiority effect. In hierarchical regression analyses, executive measures failed to account for variance shared between serial naming and reading fluency. In confirmatory factor analyses, including a discrete and a serial factor for the naming tasks, variance in the executive tasks not shared with simple choice reaction was not associated with the serial factor. Thus, the executive tasks failed to account for the serial superiority effect. The high correlation between the simple choice factor and the discrete naming factor suggests that method variance partially underlies the observed relationship between executive function tasks and word reading. We argue that the distinction between serial and discrete dimensions indicates that internally regulated cognitive control is crucial for the serial superiority in naming symbols and words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Both the distinction among EF sub-components in children and the timing of their emergence have been controversial. Although a multidimensional structure of EF components is apparent in adulthood (Miyake et al., 2000), a more unified construct has been identified earlier in development, with preschool EF attributed to a single global factor of executive control (Fuhs & Day, 2011; Wiebe et al., 2008). Recent studies suggest that EF is a global ability that gradually becomes differentiated from middle childhood through adulthood (De Luca et al., 2003; van der Ven, Kroesbergen, Boom, & Leseman, 2013).

  2. The serial and discrete naming data were derived from tasks previously reported in Protopapas et al. (2013a). Previous analyses were conducted with discrete naming response times including articulation (i.e., offset latency, from the onset of item presentation to the offset of the spoken response). In contrast, analyses reported here used onset latencies for the discrete tasks, which is the measure more frequently reported in the literature.

  3. Phantom factors are “latent variables with no observed indicators” (Rindskopf, 1984, p. 38). These latent variables allow estimation of model parameters in alternative forms or metrics. In the present analysis, phantom factors were used to convert the residual variances of the observed variables (inhibition, shifting, and updating) to latent variables.

References

  • Altemeier, L., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2008). Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 588–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amtmann, D., Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (2007). Mixture growth models of RAN and RAS row by row: Insight into the reading system at work over time. Reading and Writing, 20, 785–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arrington, C. N., Kulesz, P. A., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., & Barnes, M. A. (2014). The contribution of attentional control and working memory to reading comprehension and decoding. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 325–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bental, B., & Tirosh, E. (2007). The relationship between attention, executive function and reading domain abilities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading disorder: A comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 455–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bexkens, A., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Tijms, J. (2015). Rapid automatized naming in children with dyslexia: Is inhibitory control involved? Dyslexia, 21, 212–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P., & Weeninck, D. (2012). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.3.17) [Computer program]. Retrieved June 13, 2012, from http://www.praat.org/

  • Booth, J. N., Boyle, J. M. E., & Kelly, S. W. (2014). The relationship between inhibition and working memory in predicting children’s reading difficulties. Journal of Research in Reading, 37, 84–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M., Demetre, J., Hamill, S., Robson, K., Shepherd, H., & Cody, G. (2002). Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2144–2155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christopher, M. E., Miyake, A., Keenan, J. M., Pennington, B., DeFries, J. D., Wadsworth, S. J., et al. (2012). Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 141, 470–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F. (2011). What discrete and serial rapid automatized naming can reveal about reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 314–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, C. R., Wood, S. J., Anderson, V., Buchanan, J.-A., Proffitt, T. M., Mahony, K., et al. (2003). Normative data from the Cantab. I: Development of executive function over the lifespan. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 242–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M., & Rudel, R. (1976). Naming of object drawings by dyslexic and other learning disabled children. Brain and Language, 3, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., Blitsas, P., & Carreiras, M. (2009). A standardized set of 260 pictures for Modern Greek: Norms for name agreement, age of acquisition and visual complexity. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 584–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Everatt, J., Warner, J., Miles, T. R., & Thomson, M. E. (1997). The incident of Stroop interference in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 3, 222–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Everatt, J., Weeks, S., & Brooks, P. (2008). Profiles of strengths and weaknesses in dyslexia and other reading difficulties. Dyslexia, 14, 16–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faccioli, C., Peru, A., Rubini, E., & Tassinari, G. (2008). Poor readers but compelled to read: Stroop effects in developmental dyslexia. Child Neuropsychology, 14, 277–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J., Nie, Z., & Byrnes, J. (2014). sem: Structural equation models. R package version 3.1.2, retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sem

  • Fuhs, M. W., & Day, J. D. (2011). Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at head start. Developmental Psychology, 47, 404–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., & Adams, A.-M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 218–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. C., & Hoedemaker, R. S. (2015). Effective scheduling of looking and talking during rapid automatized naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi:10.1037/xhp0000171

  • Hatzigeorgiu, N., Gavrilidou, M., Piperidis, S., Carayannis, G., Papakostopoulou, A., Spiliotopoulou, A., et al. (2000). Design and implementation of the online ILSP corpus. In Proceedings of the second international conference of language recourses and evaluation (LREC) (Vol. 3, pp. 1737–1740). Athens, Greece.

  • Helland, T., & Asbjørnsen, A. (2000). Executive function in dyslexia. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 37–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T. C., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). The contribution of naming speed to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 19, 339–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. W., Branigan, H. P., Hatzidaki, A., & Obregón, M. (2010). Is the “naming” deficit in dyslexia a misnomer? Cognition, 116, 56–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. W., Branigan, H. P., & Kelly, M. L. (2009). Dyslexic and nondyslexic reading fluency: Rapid automatized naming and the importance of continuous lists. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 567–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., Helder, A., & Karlsson, J. (2014). A cognitive view of reading comprehension: Implications for reading difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 29, 10–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M. (2002). Observations on the temporal correlates of reading failure. Reading and Writing, 15, 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latzman, R. D., Elkovitch, N., Young, J., & Clark, L. A. (2010). The contribution of executive functioning to academic achievement among male adolescents. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 455–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laubrock, J., & Kliegl, R. (2015). The eye-voice span during reading aloud. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lervåg, A., Bråten, I., & Hulme, C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45, 764–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, J. A. R., Schatschneider, C., & Wagner, R. K. (2011). Rapid serial naming and reading ability: The role of lexical access. Reading and Writing, 24, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, J. L., & Meyer, A. S. (2005). Processing of extrafoveal objects during multiple-object naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 428–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Närhi, V., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Leppasaari, T., Korhone, T., Tolvanen, A., et al. (2005). Rapid serial naming: Relations between different stimuli and neuropsychological factors. Brain and Language, 92, 45–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navarro, J. I., Aguilar, M., Alcalde, C., Ruiz, G., Marchena, E., & Menacho, I. (2011). Inhibitory processes, working memory, phonological awareness, naming speed, and early arithmetic achievement. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 580–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pham, A. V., Fine, J. G., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2011). The influence of inattention and rapid automatized naming on reading performance. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26, 214–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A. (2007). Check Vocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 859–862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Altani, A., & Georgiou, G. K. (2013a). Development of serial processing in reading and rapid naming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 914–929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Altani, A., & Georgiou, G. K. (2013b). RAN backward: A test of the visual scanning hypothesis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 453–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Archonti, A., & Skaloumpakas, C. (2007a). Reading ability is negatively related to Stroop interference. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 251–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Sideridis, G. D., Mouzaki, A., & Simos, P. G. (2007b). Development of lexical mediation in the relation between reading comprehension and word reading skills in Greek. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 165–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rindskopf, D. (1984). Using phantom and imaginary latent variables to parameterize constraints in linear structural models. Psychometrika, 49, 37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2011). Modeling a cascade of effects: The role of speed and executive functioning in preterm/full-term differences in academic achievement. Developmental Science, 14, 1161–1175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R., Pillay, V., & Melidona, S. (2007). Deconstructing rapid automatized naming: Component processes and the prediction of reading difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 129–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M., Guy, K., Griffin, J. D., & Hynd, G. W. (2000). Rapid naming deficits in children and adolescents with reading disabilities and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain and Language, 74, 70–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sesma, H. W., Mahone, E. M., Levine, T., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. E. (2009). The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 232–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan, M. A., Pennington, B. F., Yerys, B. E., Scott, A., Boada, R., Willcutt, E. G., et al. (2006). Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperactive disorder and reading disability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 584–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shao, Z., Meyer, A. S., & Roelofs, A. (2013). Selective and nonselective inhibition of competitors in picture naming. Memory & Cognition, 41, 1200–1211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Sources of individual differences in the speed of naming objects and actions: The contribution of executive control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1927–1944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6, 174–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stringer, R. W., Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2004). Differential relationships between RAN performance, behaviour ratings, and executive function measures: Searching for a double dissociation. Reading and Writing, 17, 891–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2004). Inhibition and shifting in children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 239–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Sluis, S., de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., Boom, J., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). The structure of executive functions in children: A closer examination of inhibition, shifting and updating. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 70–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44, 575–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 436–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 35–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1986). Rapid alternating stimulus naming in the developmental dyslexias. Brain and Language, 27, 360–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68. Serial No. 274.

  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Lami, L., Pizzoli, C., Pontillo, M., & Spinelli, D. (2013). Multiple stimulus presentation yields larger deficits in children with developmental dyslexia: A study with reading and RAN-type tasks. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 639–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., & Spinelli, D. (2015). Discrete versus multiple word displays: A re-analysis of studies comparing dyslexic and typically developing children. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Angeliki Altani.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Altani, A., Protopapas, A. & Georgiou, G.K. The contribution of executive functions to naming digits, objects, and words. Read Writ 30, 121–141 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9666-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9666-4

Keywords

Navigation