Skip to main content
Log in

Large grain instruction and phonological awareness skill influence rime sensitivity, processing speed, and early decoding skill in adult L2 learners

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Linguistic knowledge, cognitive ability, and instruction influence how adults acquire a second orthography yet it remains unclear how different forms of instruction influence grain size sensitivity and subsequent decoding skill and speed. Thirty-seven monolingual, literate English-speaking adults were trained on a novel artificial orthography given initial instruction that directed attention to either large or small grain size units (i.e., words or letters). We examined how initial instruction influenced processing speed (i.e., reaction time (RT)) and sensitivity to different orthographic grain sizes (i.e., rimes and letters). Directing attention to large grain size units during initial instruction resulted in higher accuracy for rimes, whereas directing attention to smaller grain size units resulted in slower RTs across all measures. Additionally, phonological awareness skill modulated early learning effects, compensating for the limitations of the initial instruction provided. Collectively, these findings suggest that when adults are learning to read a second orthography, consideration should be given to how initial instruction directs attention to different grain sizes and to inherent phonological awareness ability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H. R. (1993). The {CELEX} lexical data base on {CD-ROM}.

  • Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, C. H. (1964). Transfer effects of word and letter training in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 215–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bitan, T., & Karni, A. (2003). Alphabetical knowledge from whole words training: Effects of explicit instruction and implicit experience on learning script segmentation. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 323–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bitan, T., & Karni, A. (2004). Procedural and declarative knowledge of word recognition and letter decoding in reading an artificial script. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 229–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bitan, T., Manor, D., Morocz, I. A., & Karni, A. (2005). Effects of alphabeticality, practice and type of instruction on reading an artificial script: an fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 90–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read—A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L., & Miller, A. (Eds.). (1979). A comparison of explicit and implicit knowledge of an alphabet (Vol. 1). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Detey, S., & Nespoulous, J. L. (2008). Can orthography influence second language syllabic segmentation? Japanese epenthetic vowels and French consonantal clusters. Lingua, 118(1), 66–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlam, R. (2005). Language aptitude and its relationship to instructional effectiveness in second language acquisition. Language Teaching Research, 9, 147–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrington-Flint, L., Wood, C., Canobi, K. H., & Faulkner, D. (2004). Patterns of analogical reasoning among beginning readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 226–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998). The role of instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1999). Causal connections in beginning reading: The importance of rhyme. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 217–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J. (1990). Word-frequency and neighborhood frequency-effects in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(2), 228–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamada, M., & Koda, K. (2008). Influence of first language orthographic experience on second language decoding and word learning. Language Learning, 58, 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamada, M., & Koda, K. (2011). Similarity and difference in learning L2 word-form. System, 39(4), 500–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M. (1992). L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 25–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knafle, J. D., & Legenza, A. (1978). External generalizability of inquiry involving artificial orthography. American Educational Research Journal, 15, 331–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (1997). Orthographic knowledge in L2 lexical processing: a cross-linguistic perspective. In J. Coady & T. Huchin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary acquisition (pp. 35–52). England: Cambridge University Press.

  • Koda, K. (1999). Development of L2 intraword orthographic sensitivity and decoding skills. Modern Language Journal, 83, 51–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laufer, B. (1997). What’s in a word that makes it hard or easy: some intralexical factors that affect the learning of words. In N. Schmitt & M. McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: Description, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 140–155). England: Cambridge University Press.

  • Medler, D. A. & Binder, J. R. (2005). MCWord: An on-line orthographic database of the English language. http://www.neuro.mcw.edu/mcword/.

  • Muljani, D., Koda, K., & Moates, D. R. (1998). The development of word recognition in a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 99–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nokes, T. J., & Ash, I. K. (2010). Investigating the role of instructional focus in incidental pattern learning. Journal of General Psychology, 137, 84–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, M. E., & Haines, L. P. (1992). Orthographic analogy training with kindergarten children: Effects on analogy use, phonemic segmentation, and letter-sound knowledge. Journal of Reading Behavior, 24, 109–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skehan, P. (1991). Individual differences in second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 275–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R. L., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., & Humbach, N. (2009). Long-term crosslinguistic transfer of skills from L1 to L2. Language Learning, 59, 203–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, R. L., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., & Humbach, N. (2012). Do L1 reading achievement and L1 print exposure contribute to the prediction of L2 proficiency? Language Learning, 62, 473–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Tong, F. H., Irby, B. J., Lara-Alecio, R., & Mathes, P. G. (2010). Hispanic english learners’ responses to longitudinal english instructional intervention and the effect of gender: A multilevel analysis. Elementary School Journal, 110, 542–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency (TOWRE). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Stone, G. O., & Pennington, B. F. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. Psychological Review, 97, 488–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (2004). A web-based interface to calculate phonotactic probability for words and nonwords in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 481–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing: CTOPP. Pro-Ed.

  • Walton, P. D. (1995). Rhyming ability, phoneme identity, letter-sound knowledge, and the use of orthographic analogy by prereaders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 587–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, P. D., Walton, L. M., & Felton, K. (2001). Teaching rime analogy or letter recoding reading strategies to prereaders: Effects on prereading skills and word reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 160–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, B. W., Olson, D. K., & Treiman, R. (1990). Subsyllabic units as aids in beginning reader’s word learning: Onset-rime versus post-vowel segmentation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities. Riverside Pub.

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The first author was partially funded by the Ruth L. Kirschstein NRSA Institutional Research T32 Training Grant from the NIDCD (T32 DC009399-01A10) to The Roxelyn and Richard Pepper Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christine Brennan.

Appendix

Appendix

All pseudowords used in training and generalization testing including English spelling (not trained, only used for creation of stimuli), pronunciation in IPA, and artificial orthographic pattern.

figure a

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brennan, C., Booth, J.R. Large grain instruction and phonological awareness skill influence rime sensitivity, processing speed, and early decoding skill in adult L2 learners. Read Writ 28, 917–938 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9555-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9555-2

Keywords

Navigation