Skip to main content
Log in

What exactly is “social” about social networks?: Accounting for socio-cultural context in networks of human interaction

  • Published:
Quality & Quantity Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In recent years the study of networks has grown across the social, natural, and computer sciences. As numerous fields and disciplines have taken up formal network methodology, however, distinctions between human interactional and other types of networks have become blurred. This article argues for the utility of stepping back from the current state of affairs and asking “what exactly is social about social network analysis?” Answering this question requires drawing on current work in social cognition and cultural evolution to better account for the unique reflexive and adaptive nature of human experience and behavior within socio-cognitive niches. The study of diffusion and homophily are reexamined in light of this perspective on culture and cognition. The article concludes with a brief discussion of the broader implications of taking the human component of social networks more seriously.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Not applicable.

Code availability

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. While graph theory is the dominant approach to formal network analysis, other methods, like multidimensional scaling, can also be employed (e.g., Barrett and Rice 1985).

  2. Brandes et al. skeptically call this an effort to find a GrUNT, or Grand Unified Network Theory, in the premier issue of Network Science (2013).

  3. In many ways the core issue in the comparative study of social cognition remains Darwin’s claim “that the difference in mind between man and the higher animals... is certainly one of degree and not of kind” (1974, p. 122). This question of whether human cognition is fundamentally different from animal cognition or only a variation of it is ultimately not fundamental for the argument of this paper, however. Whether other animals have, for example, rudimentary Theory of Mind capabilities does not change the fact that for many types of empirical problems analysts studying human social networks should take higher order social cognition into account.

  4. The social intelligence hypothesis is also referred to as the Machiavellian hypothesis (Byrne and Whiten 1988). The cultural intelligence hypothesis can be seen as a special case of the social intelligence hypothesis that focuses especially on the transmission of culture. Another complementary, and specifically human focused, perspective is the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis that emphasizes the central role of social cooperation in human evolution (Moll and Tomasello 2007).

  5. One of the significant pieces of evidence for the social intelligence hypothesis is the strong correlation between increase in neocortex and group size, something true not only for primates, but for mammals more generally (Dunbar 1992). Moreover, the human neocortex is larger in other primates in exactly these areas that support social competencies that seem to be unique to humans like Theory of Mind and sense of self (Flinn et al. 2005).

  6. As will be discussed later, the ability to reason about higher-order relationships is facilitated by language and the ability to name relationships (Penn et al. 2008).

  7. Another way to think about the distinction between physical and socio-cognitive niches is Hacking’s notion of “transient” and “permanent” kinds (1999: chapter 4). As Machery and Faucher write, “[a] kind is transient if its reality depends on a cultural niche, that is, on the ideas and social practices that are prevalent in a given society at a given time. The dependence is such that were the niche of a transient kind to disappear, the kind would disappear as well…By contrast, the reality of permanent kinds basically natural kinds…is not dependent on any culture, even if the concepts that refer to them may vary across cultures (2005, pp. 1013–1014).” For a similar argument, see Searle (1995).

  8. As Mesoudi notes, there is a double-disassociation between behavior and ecology in that “two societies living the same environment can have totally different behavioral practices, conversely two societies with similar behavioral practices may live very different environments (2011, pp. 12–13).” Moreover, similar stimuli and activities can lead to different brain responses depending on a person’s cultural and social background (Roepstoroff et al. 2010). The relationship between brain and world is complex and culturally mediated.

  9. See Hacking (2004) for discussion of the difference between discourse in the abstract and in face-to-face interaction.

  10. We may of course also be interested in a fuller account of disease spread that includes understanding why the actor engaged in the practice that led to transmission, but that is a separate issue.

  11. All of these categories refer to different types of 'social learning biases’, the innate or learned rules people use when copying others.

  12. A key issue in the research literature is differentiating social learning from either individual learning (learning with no influence from conspecifics) or genetic inheritance (acquisition of information via genetic inheritance). Teasing these apart empirically is a difficult process.

  13. As Mesoudi (2011, p. 67) notes, the characteristics of successful innovation identified by Rogers (1995) in the network diffusion literature are very similar to the various content biases identified by cultural evolutionists.

  14. Experimental work has found that fidelity of transmission of an arbitrary norm is increased when it is associated with membership in an organization or institution (Jacobs and Campbell 1961; Zucker 1977).

  15. This process seems to work in two directions. Through diffusion, we unintentionally change information to make it easier to remember and learn (Kirby et al. 2008). We also take advantage of this process and purposefully shape information to make it ‘stickier.’

  16. One example of a TRIM from the network literature given by Mesoudi (2011, p. 68) is Roger’s study of health workers’ attempts to convince housewives in Puruvian villages to boil water to reduce spread of waterborne disease. The effort failed because it was inconsistent with existing folk beliefs about food and illness.

References

  • Adolphs, R.: Social cognition and the human brain. Trends Cognit. Sci. 3(12), 469–479 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G.: The Nature of Prejudice. Perseus Books, Cambridge (1954)

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R.: Ecological Psychology: Concepts and Methods for Studying the Environment of Human Behavior. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, G.A., Rice, R.E.: Longitudinal non-euclidean networks: applying Galileo. Soc. Netw. 7(4), 287–322 (1985)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L.W.: Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 364(1521), 1281–1289 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barth, F.: Ethnic groups and Boundaries: The Social Organization of Culture Difference. Waveland Press, Long Grove (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • Basov, N., De Nooy, W., Nenko, A.: Local meaning structures: mixed-method sociosemantic network analysis. Am. J. Cult. Sociol. 1–42 (2019)

  • Baumeister, R.F., Masicampo, E.J.: Conscious thought is for facilitating social and cultural interactions: how mental simulations serve the animal–culture interface. Psychol. Rev. 117(3), 945–971 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P.L., Luckmann, T.: The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise in the Sociology of Knowledge. Doubleday, Garden City (1966)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, H.R., Killworth, P.D.: Informant accuracy in social network data II. Hum. Commun. Res. 4(1), 3–18 (1977)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, H.R., Killworth, P.D., Sailer, L.: Informant accuracy in social network data IV: a comparison of clique-level structure in behavioral and cognitive network data. Social Networks 2(3), 191–218 (1979)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogdan, R.J.: Interpreting Minds: The Evolution of a Practice. The MIT Press, Cambridge (1997)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Borgatti, S.P., Lopez-Kidwell, V.: Network theory. In: Scott, J., Carrington, P.J. (eds.) The Sage Handbook of Social Network Analysis, pp. 40–54. SAGE Publications Inc, Thousand Oaks (2011)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bottero, W., Crossley, N.: Worlds, fields and networks: Becker, Bourdieu and the structures of social relations. Cult. Sociol. 5(1), 99–119 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P.: In Other Words: Essays Towards a Reflexive Sociology. Stanford University Press, Stanford (1990)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P., Wacquant, L.J.D.: An Invitation to Reflexive Sociology. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1992)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandes, U., Lerner, J., Snijders, T.A.B.: Networks evolving step by step: statistical analysis of dyadic event data. In: 2009 International Conference Advances in Social Network Analysis and Mining, pp. 200–205. IEEE Computer Society (2009)

  • Brandes, U., Robins, G., McCranie, A., Wasserman, S.: What is network science? Netw. Sci. 1(1), 1–15 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brands, R.A.: Cognitive social structures in social network research: a review. J. Organ. Behav. 34(S1), S82–S103 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brashears, M.E., Quintane, E.: The microstructures of network recall: how social networks are encoded and represented in human memory. Soc. Netw. 41, 113–126 (2015)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brubaker, R., Loveman, M., Stamatov, P.: Ethnicity as cognition. Theory Soc. 33, 31–64 (2004)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, R.S.: Social contagion and innovation: cohesion versus structural equivalence. Am. J. Sociol. 92(6), 1287–1335 (1987)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Tomasello, M.: Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Dev. Sci. 10(4), F31–F38 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butts, C.: Revisiting the foundations of network analysis. Science 325, 414–416 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R.W., Whiten, A.: Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes and Humans. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • Call, J., Tomasello, M.: Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. In: Human Nature and Self Design, pp. 83–96 (2011)

  • Carley, K.: Coding choices for textual analysis: a comparison of content analysis and map analysis. Sociol. Methodol. 23, 75–126 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., Feldman, M.W.: Cultural Transmission and Evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton (1981)

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D.L., Seyfarth, R.M.: Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. Linguist. Rev. 22(2–4), 135–159 (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  • Chudek, M., Henrich, J.: Culture-gene coevolution, norm psychology, and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends Cognit. Sci. 15(5), 218–226 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A.: Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends Cognit. Sci. 10(8), 370–374 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A.: Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind. Oxford University Press, New York (2015)

    Google Scholar 

  • Collard, M., Shennan, S.J., Tehrani, J.J.: Branching, blending, and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences among human populations. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27(3), 169–184 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, R.: On the microfoundations of macrosociology. Am. J. Sociol. 86(5), 984–1014 (1981)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Contractor, N.S., DeChurch, L.A.: Integrating social networks and human social motives to achieve social influence at scale. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111(4), 13650–13657 (2014)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crossley, N.: The social world of the network. Combining qualitative and quantitative elements in social network analysis. Sociologica 4(1), 1–33 (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., Gergely, G.: Natural pedagogy. Trends Cognit. Sci. 13(4), 148–153 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C.: The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. Gale Research, Detroit (1974)

    Google Scholar 

  • De Nooy, W.: Fields and networks: correspondence analysis and social network analysis in the framework of field theory. Poetics 31(5–6), 305–327 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E., Gallagher, S.: Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends Cognit. Sci. 14(10), 441–447 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., Peräkylä, A., Stevanovic, M.: The co-creation of meaningful action: bridging enaction and interactional sociology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 371(1693), 20150378 (2016)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T., Cashman, T.: The role of symbolic capacity in the origin of religion. J. Study Relig. Nat. Cult. 3, 490–517 (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dean, L.G., Vale, G.L., Laland, K.N., Flynn, E., Kendal, R.L.: Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective. Biol. Rev. 89(2), 284–301 (2014)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, D.K.: Language and interaction: applying sociolinguistics to social network analysis. Qual. Quant. 53(2), 757–774 (2019)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, D., McFarland, D.A.: Classroom ordering and the situational imperatives of routine and ritual. Sociol. Educ. 85(4), 326–349 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diesner, J., Carley, K.M.: Revealing social structure from texts: meta-matrix text analysis as a novel method for network text analysis. In: Causal Mapping for Research in Information Technology, pp. 81–108. IGI Global (2005)

  • Doerfel, M.L.: What constitutes semantic network analysis? A comparison of research and methodologies. Connections 21(2), 16–26 (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • Doerfel, M.L., Barnett, G.A.: A semantic network analysis of the International Communication Association. Hum. Commun. Res. 25(4), 589–603 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R.: Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. J. Hum. Evol. 22(6), 469–493 (1992)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durham, W.H.: Coevolution: Genes, Culture, and Human Diversity. Stanford University Press, Stanford (1991)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Emirbayer, M.: Manifesto for a relational sociology. Am. J. Sociol. 103(2), 281–317 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emirbayer, M., Goodwin, J.: Network analysis, culture, and the problem of agency. Am. J. Sociol. 99(6), 1411–1454 (1994)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, F.: Talk and Social Theory: Ecologies of Speaking and Listening in Everyday Life. Polity Press, Malden (2004)

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E.: Formalist and relationalist theory in social network analysis. Sociol Theory 31(3), 219–242 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E.: Relationalism and social networks. In: Dépelteau, F. (ed.) The Palgrave Handbook of Relational Sociology, pp. 271–287. Palgrave Macmillan, New York (2018)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Feld, S.L.: The focused organization of social ties. Am. J. Sociol. 86(5), 1015–1035 (1981)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, T., Huber, L., Bugnyar, T.: Social cognition and evolution of language: constructing cognitive phylogenies. Neuron 65, 1–21 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flinn, M.V., Geary, D.C., Ward, C.V.: Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26(1), 10–46 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, L.C.: Filling in the blanks: a theory of cognitive categories and the structure of social affiliation. Soc. Psychol. Q. 55, 118–127 (1992)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, L.C., Webster, C.M.: Interpersonal proximity in social and cognitive space. Soc. Cogn. 12(3), 223–247 (1994)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhse, J.A.: The meaning structure of social networks. Sociol Theory 27(1), 51–73 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhse, J.A.: Theorizing social networks: the relational sociology of and around Harrison White. Int. Rev. Sociol. 25(1), 15–44 (2015)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallotti, M., Frith, C.D.: Social cognition in the we-mode. Trends Cognit. Sci. 17(4), 160–165 (2013)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J.J.: The ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Erlbaum, Hillsdale (1979)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, D.R.: Participation shifts: order and differentiation in group conversation. Soc. Forces 81(4), 1335–1380 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, D.R.: Taking turns and talking ties: networks and conversational interaction. Am. J. Sociol. 110(6), 1561–1597 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E.: Forms of Talk. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia (1981)

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A.I.: Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford University Press on Demand, Oxford (2006)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Guinote, A.: Power and affordances: when the situation has more power over powerful than powerless individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95(2), 237–252 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hacking, I.: Between Michel Foucault and Erving Goffman: between discourse in the abstract and face-to-face interaction. Econ. Soc. 33(3), 277–302 (2004)

  • Hacking, I.: The Social Construction of What? Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B.: Survival of the friendliest: homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 155–186 (2017)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M.V., Hare, B., Tomasello, M.: Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis”. Science 317(5843), 1360–1366 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, R.C., Campbell, D.T.: The perpetuation of an arbitrary tradition through several generations of a laboratory microculture. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 62(3), 649 (1961)

  • Jang, H.Y., Barnett, G.A.: Cultural differences in organizational communication: a semantic network analysis 1. Bull Sociol Methodol 44(1), 31–59 (1994)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, E.: Theorizing diffusion: tarde and Sorokin revisited. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 566, 144–155 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, M., Mesoudi, A.: Experimental and theoretical models of human cultural evolution. Wires Cognit. Sci. 5, 317–326 (2014)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilduff, M., Lee, J.W.: The integration of people and networks. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psych. Organ. Behav. 7, 155–179 (2020)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilduff, M., Crossland, C., Tsai, W., Krackhardt, D.: Organizational network perceptions versus reality: a small world after all? Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 107(1), 15–28 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killworth, P., Bernard, H.: Informant accuracy in social network data. Hum. Organ. 35(3), 269–286 (1976)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S., Cornish, H., Smith, K.: Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: an experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105(31), 10681–10686 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., Piiroinen, T.: Toward pragmatist methodological relationalism: from philosophizing sociology to sociologizing philosophy. Philos. Soc. Sci. 36(3), 303–329 (2006)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., Piiroinen, T.: Pragmatist methodological relationalism in sociological understanding of evolving human culture. In: Dépelteau, F. (ed.) The Palgrave Handbook of Relational Sociology, pp. 119–142. Palgrave Macmillan, New York (2018)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Krackhardt, D.: Cognitive social structures. Soc. Netw. 9(2), 109–134 (1987)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahire, B.: The Plural Actor. Polity Press, Malden (2011)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K.: Field Theory in Social Science. Harper, New York (1951)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M.D.: Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 259–289 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lizardo, O., Strand, M.: Skills, toolkits, contexts and institutions: clarifying the relationship between different approaches to cognition in cultural sociology. Poetics 38(2), 204–227 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E.E.: The Two Sexes: Growing Up Apart Coming Together. Belknap Press, Cambridge (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  • Machery, E., Luc Faucher, L.: Why do we think racially? Culture, evolution, and cognition. In: Cohen, H., Lefebvre, C. (eds.) Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science, pp. 1009–1033. Elsevier Press, Amsterdam (2005)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mallon, R., Kelly, D.: Making race out of nothing: psychologically constrained social roles. In: Kincaid, H. (ed.) Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Social Science. Oxford University Press, New York (2012)

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J.L.: What is field theory? Am. J. Sociol. 109(1), 1–49 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J.L.: The Explanation of Social Action. Oxford University Press, New York (2011)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J.L., Murphy, J.P.: Networks, status, and inequality. In: Moody, J., Light, R. (eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Social Network Analysis, pp. 98–118. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2020)

    Google Scholar 

  • McFarland, D.A., Moody, J., Diehl, D., Smith, J.A., Thomas, R.J.: Network ecology and adolescent social structure. Am. Sociol. Rev. 79(6), 1088–1121 (2014)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., Cook, J.M.: Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Ann. Rev. Sociol. 27, 415–444 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G.H.: Mind, Self and Society. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1934)

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A.: Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory Can Explain Human Culture and Synthesize the Social Sciences. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (2011)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A.: The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 363, 3489–3501 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, H., Tomasello, M.: Cooperation and human cognition: The Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 639–648 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monge, P.R., Eisenberg, E.M.: Emergent communication networks. In: Jablin, F.M., Putnam, L.L., Roberts, K.H., Porter, L.W. (eds.) Handbook of Organizational Communication: An Interdisciplinary Perspective, pp. 303–342. Sage, Newbury Park (1987)

    Google Scholar 

  • Moody, J.: Race, school integration, and friendship segregation in America. Am. J. Sociol. 107(3), 679–716 (2001)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newen, A., De Bruin, L., Gallagher, S. (eds.): The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2018)

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., Keseber, S., Snyder, B.H.: Sociology: a lost connection in social psychology. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 13(4), 334–353 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Padgett, J.F., Powell, W.W.: The Emergence of Organizations and Markets. Princeton University Press, Princeton (2012)

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, C., Wheatley, T., Kleinbaum, A.M.: The Neuroscience of social networks. In: Moody, J., Light, R. (eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Social Network Analysis, pp. 496–515. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2020)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pattison, P., Robins, G.: Neighborhood-based models for social networks. Sociol. Methodol. 32(1), 301–337 (2002)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D.C., Holyoak, K.J., Povinelli, D.J.: Darwin’s mistake: explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behav. Brain Sci. 31(2), 109–130 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J.: Understanding the Representational Mind. The MIT Press, Cambridge (1991)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, T.F., Tropp, L.R., Wagner, U., Christ, O.: Recent advances in intergroup contact theory. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 35(3), 271–280 (2011)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S.: The cognitive niche: coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107(Supplement 2), 8993–8999 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roepstroff, A., Niewohner, J., Beck, S.: Enculturating brains through patterned practices. Neural Netw. 23(8–9), 1051–1059 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roerig, S., van Wesel, F., Evers, S., Krabbendam, L.: Researching children’s individual empathic abilities in the context of their daily lives: the importance of mixed methods. Front. Neurosci. 9, 261 (2015)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, E.M.: Diffusion of Innovations, 4th edn. Free Press, New York (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, D.: Naturalism: The place of society in nature. In: Jarvie, I.C., Zamora-Bonilla, J. (eds.) The Sage Handbook of Philosophy of Social Science, pp. 121–136. SAGE Publications Inc, Thousand Oaks (2011)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, L., D’Angelo, A.: Changing times: migrants’ social network analysis and the challenges of longitudinal research. Soc. Netw. 53, 148–158 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salvini, A.: Symbolic interactionism and social network analysis: an uncertain encounter. Symb. Interact. 33(3), 364–388 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatzki, T.R.: Social practices: A Wittgensteinian approach to human activity and the social. Cambridge University Press. (1996)

  • Schmälzle, R., O’Donnell, M.B., Garcia, J.O., Cascio, C.N., Bayer, J., Bassett, D.S., et al.: Brain connectivity dynamics during social interaction reflect social network structure. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114(20), 5153–5158 (2017)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schotter, A., Sopher, B.: Social learning and coordination conventions in intergenerational games: an experimental study. J. Polit. Econ. 111(3), 498–529 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A., Luckmann, T.: The Structures of the Life-World, vol. one. Northwestern Press, Evanston (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J.R.: The Construction of Social Reality. The Free Press, New York (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  • Seed, A., Bryne, R.: Animal tool use. Curr. Biol. 20(23), 1032–1039 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R.M., Cheney, D.L.: Social cognition. Anim. Behav. 103, 191–202 (2015)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S.J.: Modularity, comparative cognition and human uniqueness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 367(1599), 2794–2802 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, M.L.: Unanticipated Gains: Origins of Network Inequality in Everyday Life. Oxford University Press, New York (2009)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J.A.: A social space approach to testing complex hypotheses: the case of Hispanic marriage patterns in the United States. Socius 3, 2378023117739176 (2017)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S., Van Tubergen, F., Maas, I., McFarland, D.: Ethnic composition and friendship segregation: differential effects for adolescent natives and immigrants. Am. J. Sociol. 121(4), 1223–1272 (2016)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E.B., Brands, R.A., Brashears, M.E., Kleinbaum, A.M.: Social networks and cognition. Ann. Rev. Sociol. 46, 159–174 (2020)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternley, K.: Thought in a Hostile World: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Blackwell Publishing, Malden (2003)

    Google Scholar 

  • Strang, D., Soule, S.: Diffusion in organizations and social movements. Ann. Rev. Sociol. 24, 265–290 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S., Calero, C.I., Sigman, M.: Teaching, naturally. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 3(2), 38–43 (2014)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M.: The human adaptation for culture. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 28(1), 509–529 (1999)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., Moll, H.: Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5), 675–691 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valente, T.: Network Models of the Diffusion of Innovation. Hampton Press, Cresskill (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, B.: Structural analysis: from method and metaphor to theory and substance. In: Wellman, B., Berkowitz, S.D. (eds.) Social Structures: A Network Approach, pp. 19–61. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1988)

    Google Scholar 

  • White, H.: Identity and Control: A Structural Theory of Social Action. Princeton University Press, Princeton (1992)

    Google Scholar 

  • White, H.: Network switchings and bayesian forks: reconstructing the social and behavioral sciences. Soc. Res. 62, 1035–1063 (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • White, H.: Identity and Control: How Social Formations Emerge. Princeton University Press, Princeton (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  • White, H.C., Godart, F.: Stories from identity and control. Sociologica 3, 1–17 (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A.: The second inheritance of chimpanzees and humans. Nature 437, 52–55 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Erdal, D.: The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 367(1599), 2119–2129 (2012)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., van Schaik, C.P.: The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 362(1480), 603–620 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Spiteri, A., Horner, V., Bonnie, K.E., Lambeth, S.P., Schapiro, S.J., de Waal, F.B.M.: Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. Curr. Biol. 17(2), 1038–1043 (2007)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windzio, M.: The “social brain”, reciprocity, and social network segregation along ethnic boundaries. Hum. Nat. 31(4), 443–461 (2020)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zerubavel, E.: Lumping and splitting: notes on social classification. Sociol. Forum 11(3), 421–433 (1996)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, L.G.: The role of institutionalization in cultural persistence. Am. Soc. Re. 726–743 (1977)

Download references

Funding

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David K. Diehl.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Diehl, D.K. What exactly is “social” about social networks?: Accounting for socio-cultural context in networks of human interaction. Qual Quant 57, 1369–1392 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-022-01410-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-022-01410-z

Keywords

Navigation