Skip to main content
Log in

Addiction and embodiment

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recent experiments have shown that when individuals with a substance use disorder are confronted with drug-related cues, they exhibit an automatically activated tendency to approach these cues (i.e., drug approach bias). The strength of the drug approach bias has been associated with clinically relevant measures, such as increased drug craving and relapse, and activations in brain reward areas. Retraining the approach bias by means of cognitive bias modification has been demonstrated to decrease relapse rates in patients with an alcohol use disorder and to reduce alcohol cue-evoked limbic activity. Here, we review empirical and theoretical literature on the drug approach bias and explore two distinct models of how the drug approach bias may be embodied. First, we consider the “biological meaning” hypothesis, which grounds the automatic approach bias in the natural meaning of the body. Second, we consider the “sensorimotor hypothesis,” which appeals to the specific sensorimotor loops involved in the instantiation of addictive behaviors as the basis of the automatic approach bias. In order to differentiate between the adequacies of these competing explanations, we present specific, predictions that each model should make.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This is not to claim, of course, that all addicts are unwilling to stop using drugs.

  2. To be clear, we do not hold it to be conceptually necessary that embodied theories of cognition are anti-representationalist or anti-information-processing. In fact, most embodied theorists are firmly rooted in functionalist, representationalist, information-processing paradigms (e.g., Barsalou 1999, 2002, 2008, 2009, 2010; Damasio 1994, 1999).

  3. We do not claim that these biases cannot be intentional, conscious, controlled or not mandatory (Bargh 1994), and do not think the concept of automaticity refers to a unified set of features (Fridland 2015).

  4. We are not claiming that all cognitivist theories are computational (just like we are not claiming that all embodied theories are non-representational) but merely appealing to the computational theory of mind to draw a clear conceptual contrast with embodied theories.

  5. To be clear, we do not hold it to be conceptually necessary that embodied theories of cognition are anti-representationalist or anti-information-processing. In fact, most embodied theorists are firmly rooted in functionalist, representationalist, information-processing paradigms (e.g., Barsalou 1999, 2002, 2008, 2009, 2010; Damasio 1994, 1999).

  6. It’s important to note that philosophers often distinguish between the cognitive system and both sensory and motor systems. The former are thought to involve states, which can enter into reasoning (often construed as semantic, conceptual, intelligent, personal-level processes) and the latter, which are construed as subpersonal or bodily, and which require significant argumentation when conceived of as, e.g., responsive to reasons, or composed of conceptual content. Importantly, this is different from the way in which psychologists or cognitive scientists categorize cognitive systems where, e.g., perception is clearly a cognitive process. This is why, in philosophy, the question about the relationship between cognition and perception constitutes a classic issue in both the metaphysics of mind and epistemology. And, relatedly, the question of cognitive penetrability, i.e., “is the qualitative character of visual perception impacted by cognition (i.e., what we know/believe)?” is a question that makes sense to philosophers.

  7. The movements we have in mind are arbitrary but not random or awkward. Obviously, if entirely counterintuitive movements or symbols were used to test the approach/avoidance bias we would see a decrease in accuracy detecting automatic biases simply in virtue of the fact that subjects would have difficulty learning the connection between the arbitrary task and its assigned meaning.

  8. Thanks to an anonymous referee for making this point.

  9. For more on the spatial organization of human cognition, see Tversky (2009). As she explains, “spatial thinking forms the foundation for other thought” (p.202) and spatial thinking is necessarily tied to the boundaries, shape, and size and function of our bodies, the space of possible action, and our access to the distal layout of the environment which surrounds us.

  10. Examples of the way the body informs the meaning of concepts is illustrated in the following way:

    “HAPPY IS UP; SAD IS DOWN; HEALTH AND LIFE ARE UP; SICKNESS AND DEATH ARE DOWN (1980a, p.16).

  11. Obviously, this applies less to the organs that are protected by ribs.

  12. As Damasio writes, “emotions allow organisms to cope successfully with objects and situations that are potentially dangerous or advantageous…[they are] the most visible part of a huge edifice of undeliberated biological regulation that includes homeostatic reaction that maintain metabolism, pain signaling, and drives such as hunger and thirst (2001, p. 781).

  13. Noë writes, “Perceptual experience acquires content thanks to our possession of bodily skills. What we perceive is determined by what we do (or what we know how to do); it is determined by what we are ready to do…To be a perceiver is to understand, implicitly, the effects of movement on sensory stimulation…the enactive approach is that our ability to perceive not only depends on, but is constituted by, our possession of this sort of sensorimotor knowledge” (2005, p. 1). Emphasis in original.

  14. As O’Regan and Noë write. “For two systems to have the same knowledge of sensorimotor contingencies all the way down they will have to have bodies that are identical all the way down (at least in relevant respects). For only bodies that are alike in low-level detail can be functionally alike in the relevant ways” (2001, p. 1015).

  15. By proprioceptive experiences, Barsalou (1999) has in mind the internal bodily sensations involved in, e.g., lifting or running. Introspective experiences also include the experience of emotions or moods such as, happiness or hunger.

  16. Thanks to an anonymous referee for pointing out this possibility.

  17. Of course, a cognitivist could appeal to particular representations and memories as the basis for associative links with affective states. Still, we should notice that if a high degree of match between a particular stimulus and the actual perceptual experience of the agent were relevant for explaining AAT and retraining, then this would eliminate at least one form of cognitive explanation from adequately accounting for the above results.

References

  • Attwood, A. S., O'Sullivan, H., Leonards, U., Mackintosh, B., & Munafo, M. R. (2008). Attentional bias training and cue reactivity in cigarette smokers. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Addiction, 103(11), 1875–1882. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02335.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baler, R. D., & Volkow, N. D. (2006). Drug addiction: The neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 12(12), 559–566. doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2006.10.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. S. Jr (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Basic processes; applications, Vols. 1–2 (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkby, H., Dickson, J. M., Roper, L., & Field, M. (2012). To approach or avoid alcohol? Automatic and self-reported motivational tendencies in alcohol dependence. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 36(2), 361–368. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01620.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2002). Being there conceptually: Simulating categories in preparation for situated action. In N. L. Stein, P. J. Bauer, & M. Rabinowitz (Eds.), Representation, memory, and development: Essays in honor of Jean Mandler (pp. 1–19). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 364, 1281–1289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. [research support, N.I.H., Extramural review]. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1458–1463. doi:10.1038/nn1584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 295–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2003). Role of the amygdala in decisionmaking. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 356–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein, H., & Schardt, S. (2009). Utility of radiography in suspected ventricular shunt malfunction. The Journal of Emergency Medicine, 36(1), 50–54. doi:10.1016/j.jemermed.2007.06.044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Field, M., Healy, H., & Mogg, K. (2008). Do the affective properties of smoking-related cues influence attentional and approach biases in cigarette smokers? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22(7), 737–745. doi:10.1177/0269881107083844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, E. C., Tas, C., Kuzu, D., Esen-Danaci, A., Roelofs, K., & Brune, M. (2014). Social approach and avoidance behaviour for negative emotions is modulated by endogenous oxytocin and paranoia in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.038.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 215–224. doi:10.1177/0146167299025002007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cousijn, J., Goudriaan, A. E., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). Reaching out towards cannabis: Approach-bias in heavy cannabis users predicts changes in cannabis use. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Addiction, 106(9), 1667–1674. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03475.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, W. M., Fadardi, J. S., & Pothos, E. M. (2006). The addiction-stroop test: Theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. [meta-analysis research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 443–476. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. Putnam.

  • Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt Brace and Co.

  • Damasio, A. R. (2001). Fundamental feelings. Nature, 413, 781. doi:10.1038/35101669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013a). Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? [randomized controlled Trial research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 38–51. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013b). Implementation of approach bias re-training in Alcoholism-how many sessions are needed? Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research. doi:10.1111/acer.12281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, L. H., Plichta, M. M., Dresler, T., Zesewitz, A. K., Tupak, S. V., Haeussinger, F. B., et al. (2014). Prefrontal correlates of approach preferences for alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependence. Addiction Biology, 19(3), 497–508. doi:10.1111/adb.12005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fadardi, J. S., & Cox, W. M. (2009). Reversing the sequence: Reducing alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 101(3), 137–145. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.11.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, M., Duka, T., Tyler, E., & Schoenmakers, T. (2009). Attentional bias modification in tobacco smokers. [randomized controlled Trial research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 11(7), 812–822. doi:10.1093/ntr/ntp067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, M., Mogg, K., Mann, B., Bennett, G. A., & Bradley, B. P. (2013). Attentional biases in abstinent alcoholics and their association with craving. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(1), 71–80. doi:10.1037/A0029626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. New York: Thomas Crowell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1981). Representations. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1987). Psychosemantics. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fridland, E. (2015). Automatically minded. [article]. SYNTHESE, doi:10.1007/s11229-014-0617-9.

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gladwin, T. E., Figner, B., Crone, E. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). Addiction, adolescence, and the integration of control and motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 364–376. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gladwin, T. E., Wiers, C. E., & Wiers, R. W. (2016). Cognitive neuroscience of cognitive retraining for addiction medicine: From mediating mechanisms to questions of efficacy. Progress in Brain Research, 224, 323–344. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grubb, J. D., & Reed, C. L. (2002). Trunk orientation induces neglect-like lateral biases in covert attention. Psychological Science, 13(6), 553–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. (1978). The nature and plausibility of cognitivism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 215–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. (1981). Mind design. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi, T., Ko, J. H., Strafella, A. P., & Dagher, A. (2013). Dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex interactions during self-control of cigarette craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. doi:10.1073/pnas.1212185110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinz, A., Beck, A., Grusser, S. M., Grace, A. A., & Wrase, J. (2009). Identifying the neural circuitry of alcohol craving and relapse vulnerability. [review]. Addiction Biology, 14(1), 108–118. doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00136.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, K., Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Avoidance of emotional facial expressions in social anxiety: The approach-avoidance task. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(12), 2990–3001. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.08.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. L. (1998). Consciousness in action. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. L. (2006). Active perception and perceiving action: The shared circuits model. In T. Szabo Gendler & J. Hawthorne (Eds.), Perceptual experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jentsch, J. D., & Taylor, J. R. (1999). Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. [research support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S. Review]. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 373–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalivas, P. W. (2004). Glutamate systems in cocaine addiction. [research support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S. Review]. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 4(1), 23–29. doi:10.1016/j.coph.2003.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1975). The mechanism of hemispheric control of the lateral gradient of attention. In PMA Rabbitt and S. Dornic (Eds), Attention and Performance V (pp. 81–97). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (2010). Neurocircuitry of addiction. [research support, N.I.H., Extramural review]. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 217–238. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krieglmeyer, R., & Deutsch, R. (2010). Comparing measures of approach-avoidance behaviour: The manikin task vs. two versions of the joystick task. Cogn Emot, 24(5), 810–828. doi:10.1080/02699930903047298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krieglmeyer, R., Deutsch, R., De Houwer, J., & De Raedt, R. (2010). Being moved: Valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independently of evaluation and approach-avoidance intentions. Psychological Science, 21(4), 607–613. doi:10.1177/0956797610365131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980a). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavender, T., & Hommel, B. (2007). Affect and action: Towards an event-coding account. Cogn Emot, 21(6), 1270–1296. doi:10.1080/02699930701438152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lempert, H., & Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Effect of laterality of orientation on verbal memory. Neuropsychologia, 20, 211–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loeber, S., Grosshans, M., Korucuoglu, O., Vollmert, C., Vollstadt-Klein, S., Schneider, S., et al. (2011). Impairment of inhibitory control in response to food-associated cues and attentional bias of obese participants and normal-weight controls. International Journal of Obesity. doi:10.1038/ijo.2011.184.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahler, S. V., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Which cue to “want?” central amygdala opioid activation enhances and focuses incentive salience on a prepotent reward cue. [research support, N.I.H., Extramural]. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(20), 6500–6513. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3875-08.2009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A. B., & Brendl, C. M. (2005). Constraining theories of embodied cognition. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Psychological Science, 16(1), 6–10. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00772.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1983). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Field, M., & De Houwer, J. (2003). Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: Relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. [research support, non-U.S. Gov’t]. Addiction, 98(6), 825–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Field, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional and approach biases for smoking cues in smokers: An investigation of competing theoretical views of addiction. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Psychopharmacology, 180(2), 333–341. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-2158-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsson, L.-G., & Craik, F. I. M. (1990). Additive and interactive effects in memory for subject-performed tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2(4), 305–324. doi:10.1080/09541449008406210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2005). Action in perception. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2009). Out of our heads: Why you are not your brain, and other lessons from the biology of consciousness. Hill and Wang.

  • O'Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 883–917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, S. Q., Kahnt, T., Beck, A., Cohen, M. X., Dolan, R. J., Wrase, J., et al. (2010). Prefrontal cortex fails to learn from reward prediction errors in alcohol dependence. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7749–7753. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-09.2010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulus, M. P., & Stewart, J. L. (2014). Interoception and drug addiction. [research support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Neuropharmacology, 76 Pt B, 342-350, doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.002.

  • Peeters, M., Wiers, R. W., Monshouwer, K., van de Schoot, R., Janssen, T., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2012). Automatic processes in at-risk adolescents: The role of alcohol-approach tendencies and response inhibition in drinking behavior. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Addiction, 107(11), 1939–1946. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03948.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phaf, R. H., Mohr, S. E., Rotteveel, M., & Wicherts, J. M. (2014). Approach, avoidance, and affect: A meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 378. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J., & Barsalou, L. W. (2000). Steering a course for embodied representation. In E. Dietrich & A. Markman (Eds.), Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual change in humans and machines (pp. 51–77). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(2), 253–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F. (2008). Grounding language in the brain. In M. de Vega, A. Glenberg, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols and embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. (1980). Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundation of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 111–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. (1984). Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radke, S., Guths, F., Andre, J. A., Muller, B. W., & de Bruijn, E. R. (2014). In action or inaction? Social approach-avoidance tendencies in major depression. Psychiatry Res. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.07.011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(2), 105–120. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (1999). Drug addiction: Bad habits add up. [news]. Nature, 398(6728), 567–570. doi:10.1038/19208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. [research support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S. Review]. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews, 18(3), 247–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2003). Addiction. [research support, U.S. Gov’t, non-P.H.S. Research support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S. Review]. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 25–53. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, K., Minelli, A., Mars, R. B., van Peer, J., & Toni, I. (2009). On the neural control of social emotional behavior. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 50–58. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenmakers, T., Wiers, R. W., Jones, B. T., Bruce, G., & Jansen, A. T. (2007). Attentional re-training decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. [randomized controlled Trial research support, non-U.S. Gov’t]. Addiction, 102(3), 399–405. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01718.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenmakers, T. M., de Bruin, M., Lux, I. F., Goertz, A. G., Van Kerkhof, D. H., & Wiers, R. W. (2010). Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. [multicenter study randomized controlled Trial research support, non-U.S. Gov’t]. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 109(1–3), 30–36. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidhu, D. M., Kwan, R., Pexman, P. M., & Siakaluk, P. D. (2014). Effects of relative embodiment in lexical and semantic processing of verbs. Acta Psychologica, 149, 32–39. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidhu, D. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2016). Is moving more memorable than proving? Effects of embodiment and imagined enactment on verb memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1010. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S. (1999). Drug anticipation and drug addiction. The 1998 H. David Archibald lecture. [lectures research support, non-U.S. Gov’t research support, U.S. Gov’t, P.H.S.] Addiction, 94(8), 1113–1124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2011). Implicit race attitudes predict trustworthiness judgments and economic trust decisions. [research support, non-U.S. Gov’t]. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(19), 7710–7715. doi:10.1073/pnas.1014345108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. [research support, non-U.S. Gov’t research support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S.] Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 768–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. [review]. Psychological Review, 97(2), 147–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turing, A. (1936). On computable numbers. Proceedings of the London Mathematical Society, 24, 230–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (2009). Spatial cognition: Embodied and situated. In M. Aydede & P. Robbins (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 201–217). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Dantzig, S., Zeelenberg, R., & Pecher, D. (2009). Unconstraining theories of embodied cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(2), 345–351. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.11.001.

  • Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenstra, E. M., & de Jong, P. J. (2011). Reduced automatic motivational orientation towards food in restricting anorexia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(3), 708–718. doi:10.1037/a0023926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veling, H., & Aarts, H. (2009). Putting behavior on hold decreases reward value of need-instrumental objects outside of awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 1020–1023. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S., Tomasi, D., Telang, F., & Baler, R. (2010). Addiction: Decreased reward sensitivity and increased expectation sensitivity conspire to overwhelm the brain's control circuit. [review]. BioEssays, 32(9), 748–755. doi:10.1002/bies.201000042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Volman, I., Toni, I., Verhagen, L., & Roelofs, K. (2011). Endogenous testosterone modulates prefrontal-amygdala connectivity during social emotional behavior. [comparative study research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 2282–2290. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, P., de Wit, S., Cousijn, J., Hommel, B., & Wiers, R. W. (2013). Motivational mechanisms underlying the approach bias to cigarettes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 250–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, P., de Wit, S., Hommel, B., & Wiers, R. W. (2012). Motivational mechanisms and outcome expectancies underlying the approach bias toward addictive substances. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 440. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, C. E., & Heinz, A. (2015). Neurobiology of alcohol craving and relapse prediction: Implications for diagnosis and treatment. In S. J. Wilson (Ed.), The Wiley handbook on the cognitive neuroscience of addiction (Vol. 219). Chichester: Wiley Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, C. E., Ludwig, V. U., Gladwin, T. E., Park, S. Q., Heinz, A., Wiers, R. W., et al. (2015a). Effects of cognitive bias modification training on neural signatures of alcohol approach tendencies in male alcohol-dependent patients. Addiction Biology, 20(5), 990–999. doi:10.1111/adb.12221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, C. E., Stelzel, C., Gladwin, T. E., Park, S. Q., Pawelczack, S., Gawron, C. K., et al. (2015b). Effects of cognitive bias modification training on neural alcohol cue reactivity in alcohol dependence. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(4), 335–343. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13111495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, C. E., Stelzel, C., Park, S. Q., Gawron, C. K., Ludwig, V. U., Gutwinski, S., et al. (2014). Neural correlates of alcohol-approach bias in alcohol addiction: The spirit is willing but the flesh is weak for spirits. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(3), 688–697. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, C. E., & Wiers, R. W. (2016). Imaging the neural effects of cognitive bias modification training. NeuroImage. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.041.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Bartholow, B. D., van den Wildenberg, E., Thush, C., Engels, R. C., Sher, K. J., et al. (2007). Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. [research support, N.I.H., Extramural research support, non-U.S. Gov’t review]. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 86(2), 263–283. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients' approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. [randomized controlled Trial research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Psychological Science, 22(4), 490–497. doi:10.1177/0956797611400615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, C. E., Kuhn, S., Javadi, A. H., Korucuoglu, O., Wiers, R. W., Walter, H., et al. (2013a). Automatic approach bias towards smoking cues is present in smokers but not in ex-smokers. [research support, non-U.S. Gov’t]. Psychopharmacology, 229(1), 187–197. doi:10.1007/s00213-013-3098-5.

  • Wiers, R. W., Gladwin, T. E., Hofmann, W., Salemink, E., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2013b). Cognitive bias modification and cognitive control training in addiction and related psychopathology: Mechanisms, clinical perspectives, and ways forward. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(2), 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Gladwin, T. E., & Rinck, M. (2013c). Should we train alcohol-dependent patients to avoid alcohol? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 33. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Rinck, M., Dictus, M., & van den Wildenberg, E. (2009). Relatively strong automatic appetitive action-tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 G-allele. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 8(1), 101–106. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00454.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Rinck, M., Kordts, R., Houben, K., & Strack, F. (2010). Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. [randomized controlled Trial research support, non-U.S. Gov’t]. Addiction, 105(2), 279–287. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02775.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, Y., Li, X., Zhang, M., Zhang, F., Zhu, C., & Shen, M. (2012). Behavioural approach tendencies to heroin-related stimuli in abstinent heroin abusers. [research support, non-U.S. Gov't]. Psychopharmacology, 221(1), 171–176. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2557-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmer, H. D., & Engelkamp, J. (1999). Levels-of-processing effects in subject-performed tasks. Memory & Cognition, 27(5), 907–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

CEW was funded by the Berlin School of Mind and Brain and Humboldt Graduate School.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Ellen Fridland or Corinde E. Wiers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fridland, E., Wiers, C.E. Addiction and embodiment. Phenom Cogn Sci 17, 15–42 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9508-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9508-0

Keywords

Navigation