Skip to main content
Log in

The dynamics of avoidance goal regulation

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

An avoidance goal is an undesired state from which a person seeks to distance themselves. Though important for understanding behavior, avoidance goals have received less attention than approach goals. In this paper, we present a dynamic, formal model that provides a framework for describing and predicting the dynamics of avoidance goal regulation. We conduct a series of simulations to examine the dynamic pattern of behavior that emerges from the model when an avoidance goal is pursued in isolation and when an approach goal is also present. Two versions of the model were examined. In the first, the avoidance goal is regulated by a positive feedback loop. In the second, the avoidance goal is regulated by a negative feedback loop. We find that the positive feedback model produces a pattern of runaway behavior, even in a scenario where an approach goal is also present. By contrast, the negative feedback loop model produces a stable pattern of behavior that is more consistent with existing theory. The findings provide an important step toward theoretical parsimony by demonstrating that avoidance goal regulation, like approach goal regulation, can be understood using a negative feedback control system framework. We discuss new insights provided by this model and its potential to spark empirical research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Formally, a feedback loop is negative if there is an odd number of links between the variables in the loop. Otherwise, the loop is positive (Richardson 1991).

  2. Some models (e.g., Vancouver et al. 2005, 2008) have included a condition in the comparator function that restricts the value of the discrepancy from taking on negative values. We omit that condition for simplicity. In the simulations presented below, adding this condition makes no difference to the results.

References

  • Ballard, T., Farrell, S., & Neal, A. (in press). Quantifying the psychological value of goal achievement. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

  • Ballard, T., Yeo, G., Loft, S., Vancouver, J. B., & Neal, A. (2016a). An integrative, formal model of motivation and decision making: The MGPM*. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 1240–1265. doi:10.1037/apl0000121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ballard, T., Yeo, G., Neal, A., & Farrell, S. (2016b). Departures from optimality when pursuing multiple approach or avoidance goals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 1056–1066. doi:10.1037/apl0000082.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Castro de la Guardia, L., Hu, X., & Myers, P. G. (2015). Greenland Ice Sheet melt increases Baffin Bay heat content on the west Greenland shelf. Geophysical Research Letters, 42, 4922–4930. doi:10.1002/2015GL064626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eddington, K. M., Majestic, C., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Contrasting regulatory focus and reinforcement sensitivity: A daily diary study of goal pursuit and emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 335–340. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Sheldon, K. M. (1997). Avoidance achievement motivation: A personal goals analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 171–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1932). The goal gradient hypothesis and maze learning. Psychological Review, 39, 25–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyland, M. E. (1987). Control theory interpretation of psychological mechanisms of depression: Comparison and integration of several theories. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 109–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kernan, M. C., & Lord, R. G. (1990). Effects of valence, expectancies, and goal-performance discrepancies in single and multiple goal environments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 194–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewandowsky, S., Ballard, T., & Pancost, R. D. (2015). Uncertainty as knowledge. Philosophical Transactions A, 373, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, R. G., & Levy, P. E. (1994). Moving from cognition to action: A control theory perspective. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 43, 335–398. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.1994.tb00828.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louro, M. J., Pieters, R., & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 174–193. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neal, A., Ballard, T., & Vancouver, J. B. (2017). Dynamic self-regulation and multiple-goal pursuit. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 4, 410–423. doi:10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032516-113156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, G. P. (1991). Feedback thought in social science and systems theory. Philadelpha: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Righetti, F., Rusbult, C., & Finkenauer, C. (2010). Regulatory focus and the Michelangelo Phenomenon: How close partners promote one another’s ideal selves. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 972–985. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.06.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roskes, M., Elliot, A. J., & De Drue, C. K. W. (2014). Why is avoidance motivation problematic, and what can be done about it? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 133–138. doi:10.1177/0963721414524224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherbaum, C. A., & Vancouver, J. B. (2010). If we produce discrepancies, then how? Testing a computational process model of positive goal revision. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 2201–2231. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00656.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2007). What to do? The effects of discrepancies, incentives, and time on dynamic goal prioritization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 928–941. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, A. M., & Dolis, C. M. (2009). Something’s got to give: The effects of dual-goal difficulty, goal progress, and expectancies on resource allocation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 678–691. doi:10.1037/a0014945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, A. M., Dolis, C. M., & Tolli, A. P. (2009). A matter of time: Individual differences, contextual dynamics, and goal progress effects on multiple-goal self-regulation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 692–709. doi:10.1037/a0015012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholer, A. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2013). Dodging monsters and dancing with dreams: Success and failure at different levels of approach and avoidance. Emotion Review, 5, 254–258. doi:10.1177/1754073913477506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Busemeyer, J. R. (1989). Approach-avoidance: Return to dynamic decision behavior. In Cognitive Processes: Tulane Flowerree Symposium On Cognition.

  • Vancouver, J. B. (2008). Integrating self-regulation theories of work motivation into a dynamic process theory. Human Resource Management Review, 18, 1–18. doi:10.1016/j.hrmr.2008.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vancouver, J. B., & Purl, J. D. (2017). A computational model of self-efficacy’s various effects on performance: Moving the debate forward. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102, 599–616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vancouver, J. B., Putka, D. J., & Scherbaum, C. A. (2005). Testing a computational model of the goal-level effect: An example of a neglected methodology. Organizational Research Methods, 8, 100–127. doi:10.1177/1094428104271998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vancouver, J. B., & Scherbaum, C. A. (2008). Do we self-regulate actions or perceptions? A test of two computational models. Computational and Mathematical Organization Theory, 14, 1–22. doi:10.1007/s10588-008-9021-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vancouver, J. B., Tamanini, K. B., & Yoder, R. J. (2008). Using dynamic computational models to reconnect theory and research: Socialization by the proactive newcomer as example. Journal of Management, 36, 764–793. doi:10.1177/0149206308321550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vancouver, J. B., Weinhardt, J. M., & Schmidt, A. M. (2010). A formal, computational theory of multiple-goal pursuit: Integrating goal-choice and goal-striving processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 985–1008. doi:10.1037/a0020628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vancouver, J. B., Weinhardt, J. M., & Vigo, R. (2014). Change one can believe in: Adding learning to computational models of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 124, 56–74. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by an Australian Research Council Grant DP120100852, awarded to Neal (CI), Yeo (CI), Zacher (CI), Vancouver, & Schmidt.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timothy Ballard.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ballard, T., Yeo, G., B. Vancouver, J. et al. The dynamics of avoidance goal regulation. Motiv Emot 41, 698–707 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9640-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9640-8

Keywords

Navigation