Skip to main content
Log in

Phonological and Visuospatial Working Memory in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We evaluated phonological and visuospatial working memory (WM) in autism spectrum disorders. Autistic children and typically developing children were compared. We used WM tasks that measured phonological and visuospatial WM up to the capacity limit of each children. Overall measures of WM did not show differences between autistic children and control children. However, when the recall of children was examined in detail, autistic children showed reduced phonological WM compared with control children. Moreover, phonological and visuospatial WM did not increase with the age of autistic children while a development of phonological and visuospatial WM with age was found in control children. The pattern of results is discussed in terms of previous studies about WM and autism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Digit span as an overall measure of WM. The digit span task is an omnibus measure that indexes working memory amplitude (verbal WM) and many other aspects such as word knowledge (e.g., magnitude information associated to the digits) and executive functioning (e.g., updating information in the backward version). For example, digit span shows correlation with many other executive and working memory tasks such as sentence and word reading tasks (Lehto 1996, for a systematic examination). Moreover, the recall of digits in the digit span task shows an advantage due to the increased frequency with which digits, compared to other verbal material, appear as random sequences in natural language (see Jones & Macken 2015, for a critical review). In addition, the recall of digits or words is better than the recall of nonwords (Hulme et al. 1997). This observation seems to indicate that linguistic familiar material enhances integrity of the items to be recalled probably due to the use of long-term lexico-phonological representations (e.g., Schweickert 1993). All these aspects guided us to consider the digit span task as an overall control measure of WM while we performed a detailed examination of the visuospatial and phonological WM with tasks that controlled for some factors such as word knowledge, familiarity of the material and frequency of items to be recalled (e.g., in the phonological span task meaningless CVC trigrams were used).

References

  • Alloway, T. P., Bibile, V., & Lau, G. (2013). Computerized working memory training: Can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Computer in Human Behavior, 29, 632–638. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloway, T. P., & Copello, E. (2013). Working memory: The what, the why, and the how. The Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 30, 105–118. doi:10.1017/edp.2013.13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloway, T. P., Rajendran, G., & Archibald, L. M. D. (2009). Working memory in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 372–382. doi:10.1177/0022219409335214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (Ed.). (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, P. N., Hovik, K. T., Skogli, E. W., Egeland, J., & Øie, M. (2013). Symptoms of ADHD in children with high-functioning autism are related to impaired verbal working memory and verbal delayed recall. PLoS One, 8, e64842. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064842.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., & Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory: The multiple component model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltruschat, L., Hasselhorn, M., Tarbox, J., Dixon, D. R., Najdowski, A. C., Mullins, R. D., & Gould, E. R. (2011). Addressing working memory in children with autism through behavioral intervention. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 267–276. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bankó, E. M., Gál, V., & Vidnyánszky, Z. (2009). Flawless visual short-term memory for facial emotional expressions. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–13. doi:10.1167/9.1.12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barendse, E. M., Hendriks, M. P. H., Jansen, J. F. A., Backes, W. H., Hofman, P. A. M., Thoonen, G., et al. (2013). Working memory deficits in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Neuropsychological and neuroimaging correlates. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 5, 1–14. doi:10.1186/1866-1955-5-14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01830.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berch, D. B., Krikorian, R., & Huha, E. M. (1998). The Corsi block-tapping task: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain and Cognition, 38, 317–338. doi:10.1006/brcg.1998.1039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451. doi:10.1192/bjp.175.5.444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73–87. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1976.tb01499.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, J., & Stokes, M. A. (2013). Removal of negative feedback enhances WCST performance for individuals with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 785–792. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2013.03.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bull, R., Phillips, L. H., & Conway, C. A. (2008). The role of control functions in mentalizing: Dual-task studies of theory of mind and executive function. Cognition, 107, 663–672. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corsi, P. M. (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, 891B. (UMI No. AA105-77717).

  • Cowan, N., Day, L., Saults, J. S., Keller, T. A., Johnson, T., & Flores, L. (1992). The role of verbal output time in the effects of word length on immediate memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 1–17. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(92)90002-F.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cui, J., Gao, D., Chen, Y., Zou, X., & Wang, Y. (2010). Working memory in early-school-age children with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 958–967. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0943-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, M. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives in Neurology, 35, 777–786. doi:10.1001/archneur.1978.00500360001001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: An update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 365–382. doi:10.1023/A:1025054610557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: beyond ‘‘theory of mind’’. Cognition, 50, 115–132. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90024-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gabig, C. S. (2008). Verbal working memory and story retelling in school-age children with autism. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 498–511. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2008/07-0023).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (2014). Working memory and language. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, M. C., Mostofsky, S. H., Cutting, L. E., Mahone, E. M., Astor, B. C., Denckla, M. B., & Landa, R. J. (2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 279–293. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1461–1471. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01287.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (2000). Parts and wholes, meanings and minds: central coherence and its relation to theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 203–221). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Ronald, A. (2008). The ‘fractionable autism triad’: A review of evidence from behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychological Review, 18, 287–304. doi:10.1007/s11065-008-9076-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189–233. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Schweickert, R., Brown, G. D., Martin, S., & Stuart, G. (1997). Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: Evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1217. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.5.1217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, P., & Goetz, L. (1997). Research on inclusive educational programs, practices, and outcomes for students with severe disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 31, 3–29. doi:10.1177/002246699703100102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G., & Macken, B. (2015). Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition, 144, 1–13. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Freeman, S. F., Bauminger, N., & Alkin, M. C. (1999). Parental perspectives on inclusion: Effects of autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 297–305. doi:10.1023/A:1022159302571.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenworthy, L., Black, D. O., Harrison, B., della Rosa, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2011). Are executive control functions related to autism symptoms in high-functioning children? Child Neuropsychology, 15, 425–440. doi:10.1080/09297040802646983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kercood, S., Grskovic, J. A., Banda, D., & Begeske, J. (2014). Working memory and autism: A review of literature. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 1316–1332. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.06.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koshino, H., Carpenter, P. A., Minshew, N. J., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Just, M. A. (2005). Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. NeuroImage, 24, 810–821. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koshino, H., Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2008). fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: Visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 289–300. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landa, R. J., & Goldberg, M. C. (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: A continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 557–573. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0001-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehto, J. (1996). Are executive function tests dependent on working memory capacity? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 49, 29–50. doi:10.1080/713755616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H., & Pearson, D. G. (1997). The inner ear and the inner scribe of visuo-spatial working memory: Evidence from developmental fractionation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 241–257. doi:10.1080/713752559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., & Prior, M. (2003). The reach-to-grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. Philosophical Transactions Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358, 393–403. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (Eds.). (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R. G., Rowe, A., Fox, N., Feigenbaum, J. D., Miotto, E. C., & Howlin, P. (1999). Spatial working memory in Asperger’s syndrome and in patients with focal frontal and temporal lobe lesions. Brain and Cognition, 41, 9–26. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1093.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Stauder, J. E. A., & Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 203–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177. doi:10.1023/A:1023052913110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 257–263. doi:10.1023/A:1010794902139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, S. J. (2001). The development of visuo-spatial working memory. Memory, 9, 423–432. doi:10.1080/09658210143000182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, S. J., Gathercole, S. E., & Peaker, S. M. (1998). Verbal and visuospatial short-term memory in children: Evidence for common and distinct mechanisms. Memory and Cognition, 26, 1117–1130. doi:10.3758/BF03201189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, P., & Gifford, K. (2016). Working memory goes to school. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 5, 194–201. doi:10.1080/21622965.2016.1167502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I. (1996). Preschool children with inadequate communication: Developmental language disorder, autism, low IQ. London: Mac Keith Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, K. E. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders in childhood: a clinical update. Community Practitioner, 84, 36–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roodenrys, S., & Hinton, M. (2002). Sublexical or lexical effects on serial recall of nonwords? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 29–33. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.28.1.29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65, 77–86. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salmanian, M., Tehrani-Doost, M., Ghanbari-Motlagh, M., & Shahrivar, Z. (2012). Visual memory of meaningless shapes in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry, 7, 104–108.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, J., Johnson, K. A., Garavan, H., Gill, M., & Gallagher, L. (2008). A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research in autistic spectrum disorders: Attention, inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 1–16. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.03.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweickert, R. (1993). A multinomial processing tree model for degradation and redintegration in immediate recall. Memory and Cognition, 21, 168–175. doi:10.3758/BF03202729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, T., Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Tonge, B. J., Egan, G., O’Boyle, M. W., & Cunnington, R. (2006). Visuospatial processing and the function of prefrontal-parietal networks in autism spectrum disorder: A functional MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1440–1443. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.8.1440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Holmes, J. (2008). Improving short-term and working memory: Methods of memory training. In N. B. Johansen (Ed.), New research on short-term memory (pp. 125–154). New York, NY: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • St Clair-Thompson, H., Stevens, R., Hunt, A., & Bolder, E. (2010). Improving children’s working memory and classroom performance. Educational Psychology, 30, 203–219. doi:10.1080/01443410903509259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, S. D., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Spatial working memory deficits in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 605–612. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0202-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence of a cognitive deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00886.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2003). WISC-IV technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., Carpenter, P. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2005). Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 747–756. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0021-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 20, 21–29. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (Research Project PSI2012–32287 to P. Macizo) and Junta de Andalucía (Biomedical and Heath Science Research Project PI-0410-2014 to M. F. Soriano).

Author Contributions

Every author of the manuscript has made substantial contributions to all of the aspects of this work and approval of the final submitted version of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to P. Macizo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Pedro Macizo, María Felipa Soriano and Natalia Paredes declared no potential conflict of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Macizo, P., Soriano, M.F. & Paredes, N. Phonological and Visuospatial Working Memory in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 2956–2967 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2835-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2835-0

Keywords

Navigation