Introduction

Seasonal migration, the round-trip movement between a breeding location and one or more non-breeding locations, is a strategy that allows organisms to exploit the fitness benefits of being at a breeding location at certain times of the year, while avoiding the costs of staying there continually (Alerstam et al. 2003; Winger et al. 2019). Seasonal migration occurs in numerous shapes and forms, and varies among species and populations, as well as among and within individuals (Newton 2008). Consider, for example, the apparent dichotomy between obligate and facultative migrants; in some species, all individuals migrate each year (obligate), while in other species, only some individuals migrate in some years (facultative; Newton 2008). Such differences are thought to be largely context specific, reflecting differences in the ecological circumstances experienced by species, populations, or individuals (Alerstam et al. 2003).

The fact that populations differ in migratory behaviour is fascinating from a life-history perspective—but also essential to the conservation of migratory species. It means that to conserve a population, researchers must first learn about the migratory routes, stopover sites, and non-breeding locations on which the population relies (Piersma and Baker 2000; Webster and Marra 2005). This knowledge about a population’s distribution is necessary to develop, in turn, an ecological understanding of why the population relies on certain geographic areas but not others, and what effect the conditions at such sites might have on the population’s demography (Alves et al. 2013; Rakhimberdiev et al. 2018). Finally, this ecological understanding can be used to further conservation efforts directly—for example, by assessing which sites are of the highest priority to conserve, what can be done to improve conditions at those sites in the event of degradation, and whether there are similar alternative sites available elsewhere (Reynolds et al. 2017; Chan et al. 2019a; Rushing et al. 2020).

It is also becoming increasingly clear that populations vary with respect to the similarity or dissimilarity among the migratory routines of individuals (Sergio et al. 2014; Lok et al. 2015; Eichhorn et al. 2017; Ruthrauff et al. 2019). Determining that individuals have different migratory routines and testing whether the causes of such individual variation are the same among populations are exciting avenues of research in their own right (Mellone et al. 2012; Trierweiler et al. 2014). However, acquiring this knowledge is also important for effective conservation, because it is intimately connected to both the vulnerabilities and potential flexibility of a given population (Gill et al. 2019; Senner et al. 2020).

Whether all individuals rely on the same routine, or whether they have multiple routines, could mean a population is more or less vulnerable to changing conditions, such as the degradation or loss of certain sites (Piersma et al. 2016; Studds et al. 2017). Understanding the causes of variation in migratory routine among individuals—i.e., whether a population possesses the plasticity to develop multiple routines, or whether it faces environmental or organismal constraints such as a lack of sites, lack of genetic variation, or lack of suitable information (Winkler et al. 2014)—is potentially even more important (Senner et al. 2020). Gaining such an understanding will help to identify the mechanism(s) by which a population can adjust to altered surroundings and thus inform effective conservation measures (Piersma 2011; Gill et al. 2019).

Our goal in this study is to describe the spatial distribution of Continental Black-tailed Godwits (Limosa limosa limosa, hereafter “godwits”) throughout their annual cycle and to assess whether individuals have consistently different migratory routes. This information will provide a foundation on which the management and conservation of godwits can be based and contribute to the understanding of the ecological basis of intraspecific variation in migratory routines. Godwits are a ground-nesting, relatively long-lived shorebird that breed across Europe (Gill et al. 2007). Currently, we know that godwits breeding in The Netherlands either spend the entire non-breeding period in the Mediterranean or cross the Sahara to spend at least part of the non-breeding period in the Sahel zone of West Africa (Hooijmeijer et al. 2013; Kentie et al. 2017; Verhoeven et al. 2019), where the non-breeding period lasts from the termination of southward migration until the departure on northward migration. Individual adults are consistent in whether they cross the Sahara at all, and although among-individual differences in the timing of the Sahara crossings are large, individual godwits are relatively consistent in their timing of this crossing (Verhoeven et al. 2019). Adult godwits are also highly consistent in their use of “stopping” sites (a term used to collectively refer to stopover and staging sites—see “Methods”; Warnock 2010; Chan et al. 2019b) on the Iberian Peninsula during northward migration (Verhoeven et al. 2018), but nonetheless may vary the number of stops they make over the course of their migration between years (Senner et al. 2019).

The picture that emerges from past godwit tracking efforts is an interplay between individual consistency and environmental contingency: individuals have a routine, but they may be forced to adjust it in response to the ecological conditions they encounter (Senner et al. 2015, 2019). In this context, it is necessary to map as fully as possible the entire network of stopping sites used by godwits, to understand the full range of alternatives that may (or may not) be available to an individual given their routine and the conditions they encounter en route (sensu Taylor and Norris 2007). Furthermore, this information will aid in the maintenance and restoration of stopping and non-breeding sites—a goal which has been classified as “high priority” by the International Single Species Action Plan for the Conservation of the Black-tailed Godwit (Jensen et al. 2008). Such information can only be gained through the repeated tracking of individuals, which remains difficult because of both the cost and limitations of tracking technologies (McKinnon and Love 2018). In addition, we lack even basic information about the flight orientation of migrating godwits. Knowing the direction an individual flies could help shed light on the degree to which deviations from its routine may be in response to conditions on the ground (e.g., fuel reserves) or encountered mid-flight (e.g., wind).

To extend our understanding of the natural variation of godwit migratory routes, we collected tracking data from 2015 to 2018 from satellite tags deployed on 36 individual adult godwits breeding within a kilometre of each other in The Netherlands. Because we tracked 19 individuals on multiple southward migrations and 12 individuals on multiple northward migrations, we were able to quantify the spatial consistency of individuals during their migratory and stationary periods, as well as their flight orientation during each segment of their migration. These results provide a foundation for future research into inter- and intraspecific variation in migratory patterns by extending our knowledge of the individual routines of godwits and offering one of the most complete overviews of migration yet assembled for any shorebird species.

Methods

Deploying satellite tags

During the breeding seasons of 2015–2017, we used walk-in and automated drop-cages to capture adult godwits on their nests in the 220-ha Haanmeer polder, The Netherlands (52.9226° N, 5.4336° E). After catching an adult, we marked it with a unique colour-ring combination and measured its tarsus-toe (without nail) and bill length (exposed culmen), and weighed it to the nearest gram. In 2015 and 2016, we deployed 32 solar-powered PTT-100s of 9.5 g from Microwave Technology Inc. using a leg-loop harness of 2-mm Dyneema rope (Lankhorst Ropes, Sneek, The Netherlands). Because of the substantial weight of this attachment (~ 10.5 g), we deployed these transmitters only on birds that we classified as “large”. As a rule of thumb, we selected birds with at least one, but usually two of the following characteristics: tarsus-toe length > 120 mm, bill length > 100 mm and body mass > 300 g (see Schroeder et al. 2008 for the range of these measures in godwits). In 2017, we deployed four 5-g solar-powered PTT-100s from Microwave Technology Inc., again using a leg-loop harness made of 2-mm Dyneema rope. The total weight of this attachment was considerably lower (~ 6 g), so we deployed these tags on godwits without selecting for size.

Based on a combination of morphological characteristics (n = 10 individuals, see Schroeder et al. 2008) and molecular sexing (n = 26 individuals), we determined that our sample of transmitter-carrying birds consisted of 34 females and 2 males. Although we mostly tracked females, we believe our results are representative of the entire adult population. The small number of satellite tracks obtained for males (Senner et al. 2019, this study) and the much larger sample of geolocator tracks of males (Hooijmeijer et al. 2013; Verhoeven et al. 2019) indicate that there are no sex-specific differences in the large-scale patterns of movement in godwits. This is further supported by more recent satellite-tracking efforts that include more males (T. Piersma, R. Howison, J. Hooijmeijer, A.H.J. Loonstra and M.A. Verhoeven unpubl. data). In 2015 and 2016, the loading factor of the transmitters was 3.4% ± 0.2 (range 3.0–4.0%) of a female’s body mass at capture; in 2017, the loading factor was 1.9% for each of the two females and 2.2% for each of the two males.

Tracking data

Thirty-four of the 36 transmitters were programmed to turn on for 8 h and turn off for 24 h. One transmitter was programmed to turn on for 8 h and off for 25 h, and one was programmed to turn on for 10 h and off for 48 h; these two transmitters had been deployed previously in two other tracking studies that used these different duty cycles. The birds outfitted with a 9.5-g transmitter were considered dead when their transmitter’s built-in activity sensor remained constant. The 5-g transmitters did not have such an activity sensor, but they did have a temperature sensor; we considered these birds dead when the measured temperature dropped and began following a day–night rhythm. For both the 9.5-g and 5-g transmitters, we considered a bird to be dead when its transmitter suddenly stopped transmitting and never turned on again; we feel that this is reasonable, as we never subsequently observed any of these 36 individuals alive during our resighting efforts of marked birds at the breeding grounds, stopping sites, or non-breeding sites (Verhoeven et al. 2018; Loonstra et al. 2019a).

All tracking data were extracted from the CLS tracking system (http://www.argos-system.org) and stored at Movebank (http://www.movebank.org). In Movebank, we selected all data up to 1 October 2018 and used the “Best Hybrid” Douglas Argos-Filter Algorithm (Douglas et al. 2012) to remove implausible locations by setting the threshold for maximum movement rate to 120 km h−1. This resulted in an average of 3.00 ± 0.24 locations per individual duty cycle. On the breeding grounds, we have been able to simultaneously compare the locations received through the CLS tracking system with actual observations of these godwits. The error rarely exceeds 5 km and, therefore, is unlikely to affect the large-scale patterns of movement presented here (M.A. Verhoeven and A.H.J Loonstra pers. obs.).

Annotating and plotting migratory tracks

We plotted every individual’s tracking data in Google Earth and, from the resulting tracks, determined the spatial organisation of each individual’s annual cycle. To do this, we divided the individual’s locations into four groups: (1) at the breeding grounds, (2) in flight, (3) at stopping sites during migration, and (4) at non-breeding sites during the period between termination of southward migration and departure on northward migration. We considered all points north of 52° N to be on the breeding grounds. All points that were not in the same location, but rather made up a route toward a new location, were considered to be in flight. Points at the same location during migration were considered to be at stopping sites and points after the termination of southward migration were considered to be at non-breeding sites. The termination of southward migration occurred either when an individual first arrived at a site in West Africa (for those individuals that crossed the Sahara) or when an individual arrived at its most southern site (for those individuals that spent their non-breeding period north of the Sahara, i.e., on the west coast of Portugal, or in Doñana or Morocco).

Warnock (2010) states that all sites used by birds to stop during migration are stopover sites, but that any stopover sites used for longer periods to fuel for longer flights should be further classified as staging sites. Because godwits stop at certain sites for both long and short periods of time (Table 1), we cannot differentiate between stopover and staging sites. For this reason, we refer to all sites used during migration as “stopping” sites (following Chan et al. 2019b). We used the annotated data to combine and plot all of our individual tracking data, and created two plots that show: (1) in-flight locations and stopping sites during southward migration, plus the non-breeding sites (Fig. 1a), and (2) in-flight locations and stopping sites during northward migration (Fig. 1b). These maps were made following the example of Vansteelant et al. (2017); we downloaded a relief map from Natural Earth (http://www.naturalearthdata.com) and masked the seas by overlaying a high-resolution shoreline map obtained from the National Center for Environmental Information (http://www.ngdc.noaa.gov/mgg/shorelines). We then plotted the locations on the resulting map using “ggplot2” in R (Wickham 2016; R Core Team 2018).

Table 1 Summary of passage dates and lengths of stay for every stopping site used on migration north of the Sahara (> 28° N)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Presentation of the data collected and analysed in this study. Red dots in a and blue dots in b show the in-flight locations. The other dots show the stationary locations, and are coloured by geographic region. On northward migration (b), there is a single red dot in the province of Léon in northern Spain and a single khaki dot just north of the French Pyrenees

Analysis

We quantified three different aspects of the variation in migratory routes: (1) variation in orientation, (2) longitudinal distribution of tracks at each latitudinal boundary, and (3) variation in the use of stopping and non-breeding sites. Since our goal was to make comparisons between different routes, and because godwits do not migrate in a straight line (Senner et al. 2018; Loonstra et al. 2019b), we did not assume a straight migration during the programmed “off” cycle of the transmitters (which is often done in tracking studies). Rather, in our analyses we used only those routes that were actually observed during the “on” cycle of the transmitters.

In-flight orientation and distribution

We quantified an individual’s orientation during south- and northward migration by calculating its longitudinal (i.e., east–west) movement, measured in kilometres, between latitudinal boundaries (Fig. 2). Latitudinal boundaries were spaced 1° of latitude apart along the migratory route (from 52° N to 18° N, Fig. 3a). To calculate longitudinal movement, an individual’s tag had to have been transmitting live data when crossing at least two consecutive latitudinal boundaries (Fig. 2). We also quantified the longitudinal distribution of tracks at each latitudinal boundary. To account for the fact that degrees of longitude differ in width at different latitudes, we expressed the longitudinal distribution as the number of kilometres between individual tracks, measured from west to east, at each latitudinal boundary (Fig. 2). The most westerly track at each latitudinal boundary was considered to be at zero; since godwits do not migrate directly north to south, this allowed us to compare the longitudinal distributions with each other and examine the spatial distributions of tracks along the entire migratory corridor (Fig. 3b). To calculate the longitudinal distribution, more than one tag had to have been transmitting live data at a given latitudinal boundary (Fig. 2). It was more common to have multiple tags transmitting at a single latitudinal boundary than to have a single tag transmitting for two consecutive latitudinal boundaries, but both of these circumstances occurred relatively frequently in our dataset (see dots in Fig. 3 for sample sizes). When possible, we also calculated the difference between the routes of the same individual in different years for both measurements. However, this sample size is small because it requires a tag to have been transmitting live data at the same place in different years.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Schematic overview showing how longitudinal distribution and longitudinal movement were calculated. The longitudinal distribution of tracks was calculated at each latitudinal boundary. To account for the fact that degrees of longitude differ in width at different latitudes, we expressed the longitudinal distribution as the number of kilometres between individual tracks, measured from west to east, at each latitudinal boundary. The most westerly track at each latitudinal boundary was considered to be at zero; since godwits do not migrate directly north to south, this scaling toward the most westerly track allowed us to compare the longitudinal distributions among different latitudinal boundaries. The longitudinal movement was calculated as the east–west movement of an individual between latitudinal boundaries (in kilometres). Calculating the longitudinal movement of individuals at different boundaries allowed us to evaluate whether the flight orientation of godwits differs along their migratory route

Fig. 3
figure 3

A summary of the observed variation in longitudinal movement and longitudinal distribution. Boxes indicate the 25th, 50th and 75th percentile and whiskers extend to the largest value no further than 1.5 times the interquartile range away from the boxes. a Shows the longitudinal movement for each latitudinal segment on southward (red) and northward (blue) migration. The green line illustrates a route that is directly north–south. b Shows the longitudinal distribution at each latitudinal boundary, i.e., the spatial distribution of tracks within the migratory corridor, on southward (red) and northward (blue) migration. The colours correspond to the in-flight locations in Fig. 1

To calculate the distances between points, we used the function “distHaversine” in the R package “geosphere” (Hijmans 2017). We then used Levene’s test, which is part of the R package “car” (Fox and Weisberg 2019), to compare the variances of the longitudinal movements and longitudinal distributions across latitudes. Only in the case of longitudinal distribution on northward migration did this Levene’s test indicate that the variances were not homogeneous across latitudes. There are no post hoc tests, such as pairwise comparisons, to further assess between which specific latitudes the variances are not equal to each other. For this reason, we relied on visually inspecting Figs. 1b and 3b and directly comparing variances between latitudes; though this is by no means a formal statistical test, it could nonetheless be informative. However, we were able to formally test whether the mean longitudinal distribution on northward migration varied between latitudinal boundaries. For this, we used the non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test, which indicated that longitudinal distribution differed at different latitudinal boundaries. We made pairwise comparisons of all means using a Dunn test, which is part of the R package “dunn.test” (Dinno 2017), while applying a Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment to adjust the p values for multiple comparisons.

In all other cases—southward longitudinal distribution and both southward and northward longitudinal movement—the variances were homogeneous across latitudes, so we further analysed each case using one-way ANOVAs. When an ANOVA indicated that means varied significantly by latitude (p < 0.05), we made pairwise comparisons of estimated marginal means using the R package “emmeans” (Lenth 2019); we first used the function “emmeans” to compute estimated marginal means and then performed pairwise comparisons of these means while applying Tukey adjustments using the “pairs” call.

Site use

We quantified the variability in site use for two regions: (1) stopping sites north of the Sahara and (2) non-breeding sites south of the Sahara. We did this by calculating the nearest-neighbour distances between all the stationary points in a focal track and all the stationary points in a comparison track (which was also done in Guilford et al. 2011; van Bemmelen et al. 2017; Oudman et al. 2018). In brief, this method takes a point in a focal track and finds the nearest point in the comparison track. Doing this for every point in the focal track results in a list of nearest-neighbour or “minimum intertrack” distances (one for every point in the focal track). This method enabled us to compare nearest-neighbour distances among all individuals across all years, among all individuals within the same year, and within the same individuals across different years.

We used the mean of all the nearest-neighbour distances within a pairwise combination as a measure of the similarity of the compared tracks. To assess whether the mean nearest-neighbour distances of between-individual track comparisons within years and within-individual track comparisons between years were smaller than the mean nearest-neighbour distances between all individual tracks—i.e., to show aggregation within years and site fidelity between years (see Fig. 2 in Oudman et al. 2018)—we randomly re-assigned all individual IDs to a track 10,000 times (Guilford et al. 2011; Oudman et al. 2018). This created a randomised estimate of mean nearest-neighbour distances for every type of track comparison. The proportion of times that a randomised estimate of track similarity is larger or smaller than the observed mean nearest-neighbour distances can then be interpreted as a two-tailed p value. For this analysis, we selected the best location—based on Argos data quality—per day per individual. If there were multiple best locations for a single day, we used the first one.

We also qualitatively assessed the use of stopping and non-breeding sites by assigning location names to a geographic region (for example, the west coast of Portugal; see Fig. 1). This allowed us to discuss different stopping sites instead of just differences in kilometres between tracks. We also used this qualitative assessment to calculate the annual return rates of individuals to stopping sites. For this, we identified the sites an individual visited in a given year (t) and compared these to the sites the same individual visited in the next year (t + 1). We then calculated, for each site, the percentage of individuals that returned in the second year (t + 1). We were able to calculate these return rates for three years for autumn migration (2016–2018) and two years for spring migration (2017 and 2018).

Results

Southward migration

All 36 individuals left the breeding grounds after being tagged. In both 2015 and 2017, one individual died at a post-breeding stopping site in The Netherlands (< 150 km from the breeding grounds) within two months of being tagged. The other 34 individuals all left The Netherlands heading toward the southwest (Figs. 1a and 3a). In 2015, one of these individuals died during migration in Normandy, France. The other 33 individuals all reached their non-breeding sites in the year of tagging: West Africa (28 individuals, 85%), Doñana, Spain (3 individuals), the west coast of Portugal (1 individual) and Morocco (1 individual). The other two occasions of mortality during southward migration happened in the years after tagging and consisted of an individual that died in Normandy, France in 2016—its tag was later discovered on the side of a cliff, which might suggest that the bird was killed by a Peregrine Falcon (Falco peregrinus)—and an individual that died in 2017 on the coast of northern Spain after crossing the Bay of Biscay. In total, we observed 62 successful southward migrations from 2015 to 2018 (13 individuals with 1 track, 13 with 2 tracks, 5 with 3 tracks and 2 with 4 tracks). In 2015, one individual stopped for a night that coincided with its tag’s “off” cycle. For this reason, we missed its exact stopping location on its way to West Africa and excluded this individual from the southward migration site-use analysis.

Southward migration—Site use

Godwits migrated to a variety of locations after their initial departure from The Netherlands. Two locations were used as a first destination in all four years: Doñana (22/61 southward migrations, 36%) and the French side of the Bay of Biscay (31%). Godwits also flew non-stop to coastal northwest Europe (7%), the west coast of Portugal (10%), the Extremadura region of Spain (5%) and West Africa (5%). Other first destinations after leaving The Netherlands were: the Balearic coast of mainland Spain (3%), the Côte du Soleil, France (1%) and Morocco (1%). As a result, the distance of initial flights from The Netherlands varied considerably—from 80 to 4477 km (\(\overline{x}\) = 1596 ± 877 km, n = 61). Non-breeding sites were reached directly from The Netherlands on 11% of southward migrations, reached after one stop on 38% of southward migrations, and reached after multiple stops on 52% of southward migrations. Migration routes that included an initial stop close to the breeding grounds in coastal northwest Europe and on the French side of the Bay of Biscay were especially likely to include additional stops north of the Sahara (96%, n = 23 flights); eventually these individuals also ended up in Doñana (65%), the west coast of Portugal (17%) and Morocco (13%) before either settling for the non-breeding period or flying on to West Africa.

Doñana and the French side of the Bay of Biscay were visited by most of the tracked individuals (Table 1). Of the 19 individuals that were followed for multiple southward migrations, one flew non-stop to West Africa in both years that it was tracked. Another individual stopped in Doñana for 5 days in one year, but flew non-stop to West Africa the next year. The remaining 17 individuals revisited stopping sites with varying consistency. Three sites were revisited in all years (2016–2018), and also had the highest average annual return rates: Doñana (0.79, 0.67–1), the French side of the Bay of Biscay (0.67 in all three years) and Portugal (0.75, 0.25–1). Morocco was visited in 2015 by one individual which returned in 2016, and in 2017 by one individual which did not return in 2018. Northwest Europe (in 2015) and Extremadura (in 2016) were visited by two individuals, and both sites were revisited by one individual the following year. The Balearic coast of mainland Spain (2 individuals in 2015) and the Côte du Soleil (1 individual in 2016) were not revisited in the following year. The mean nearest-neighbour distance between stopping locations during southward migration of the same individual across years was 217 km (95% CI 122–312), which was smaller than the distance between all tracks (306 km, 95% CI 249–364, p = 0.002). This indicates that across years, stopping site use during southward migration is clearly more site-faithful than it is nomadic.

Southward migration—Flights

Throughout their southward migration, godwits oriented to the southwest; the absolute amount of longitudinal movement per degree of latitude was u = 42 km ± 44 (n = 204, Fig. 3a). The variance of these longitudinal movements did not clearly differ between latitudinal segments (Levene’s test: F33,170 = 0.883, p = 0.654). The average longitudinal movement did differ somewhat between latitudinal segments (F33,170 = 1.706, p = 0.016): the average longitudinal movements from 52 to 51° N (1 km ± 36) and from 29 to 28° N (− 47 km ± 95) were more to the east than the average longitudinal movements from 22 to 21° N (82 km ± 45; p < 0.05 for both pairwise comparisons), while the comparisons of average longitudinal movements for all other pairs were not different (Fig. 3a). The variation in longitudinal movement between tracks of the same individual across years was 41 km ± 30 (range 8–120, n = 15 repeated movements by 8 individuals).

The maximum longitudinal distribution at a latitudinal boundary ranged from 128 km (51° N) to 836 km (18° N, Fig. 3b). The average longitudinal distribution across all latitudinal boundaries was 249 km ± 243 (n = 570, Fig. 3b). The variance of these longitudinal distributions did not clearly differ across latitudes (Levene’s test: F34,291 = 1.175, p = 0.239). The average longitudinal distribution, however, did differ by latitude (F34,291 = 2.378, p < 0.001). A pairwise post hoc comparison of all means indicated that the longitudinal distribution was on average smaller at 51° N (44 km ± 43) than it was at 21° N, 40° N and 41° N, and also that it was larger at 41°N (325 km ± 156) than at 31° N, 51° N and 52° N (see also Fig. 3b); for all other pairs of latitudinal boundaries, comparisons of the mean longitudinal distributions were not clearly different. That the longitudinal distribution was smaller at 51° N was likely the result of multiple godwits leaving from a single or several nearby post-breeding sites in The Netherlands (~ 52° N), whereas 21° N, 40° N and 41° N are all near the end of non-stop flights to stopping and non-breeding sites, when differences in longitudinal movement may have accumulated en route. The largest longitudinal distribution, with 836 km between individuals at 18° N, was measured during the southward migration of the only tracked godwit that spent the non-breeding period in the Inner Niger Delta, Mali; this area lies approximately 1000 km inland from coastal West Africa, where all the other tracked godwits spent the non-breeding season (Fig. 1A). The average longitudinal distribution between repeated tracks of the same individuals was 129 km ± 88 (range 1–299 km, n = 36 boundary crossings by 11 individuals).

Non-breeding period south of the Sahara

Twenty-eight of 33 individuals crossed the Sahara to spend part of their non-breeding period in West Africa (Fig. 1a). They visited eight wetland complexes, including (1) the Inner Niger Delta in Mali (1 individual; see above) and a network of sites in coastal West Africa: (2) the west and (3) east Senegal River, in Senegal and Mauritania, (4) Technopole, in Senegal, (5) the Saloum River, in Senegal, (6) the Gambia River, in The Gambia, (7) the Casamance River, in Senegal, and (8) multiple rivers on the coast of Guinea-Bissau and Guinea, including the Cacheu, Mansoa and Geba (see Fig. 1a). Two individuals visited up to five of the seven coastal sites within the non-breeding period, whereas five individuals stayed at one site during their stay in West Africa. (Fig. 4 and Suppl. Fig. 1). The average number of sites used per individual per year was 2.36 ± 1.00.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Repeated tracks of three individuals, “Kollum”, “Lollum” and “Abbegea”, in West Africa. These tracks illustrate the high site fidelity and individuality shown in the use of non-breeding sites south of the Sahara (< 28° N). Colours represent different years: green = winter 2015/16; pink = 2016/17; red = 2017/18. All tracks south of the Sahara (< 28° N) are presented in Suppl. Fig. 1

In total, we collected data for 45 southward Sahara crossings by 28 individuals. Three of these tracks are from one individual that flew to the Inner Niger Delta in all years it was tracked. The other 42 tracks are from individuals that all went to the network of sites in coastal West Africa and arrived at the west Senegal River (23 out of 42 tracks), the east Senegal River (9 times), the Casamance River (7 times), or the Saloum River (3 times). No individual flew directly to the rivers on the coast of Guinea-Bissau and Guinea, which is the most southerly site. All individuals were consistent across years in whether they crossed the Sahara or not (n = 19 individuals with repeated southward tracks). Individual site-use consistency is best illustrated by the quantitative analysis in the paragraph below, but on a more qualitative scale, the average individual site-use consistency was 98% (30 tracks from 14 individuals that crossed the Sahara). Due to the behaviour of two individuals, site-use consistency was not 100%: one bird used four sites in 2015, but in 2016 added a stop on the Saloum River for a total of five sites; another bird used four sites in 2015, arriving at the east Senegal River after crossing the Sahara, but in 2016 skipped this site and flew directly to the Casamance River for a total of only three sites. The other 12 individuals were entirely consistent in their use of the wetland complexes during the non-breeding period and they visited the same locations in every year they were tracked.

The mean nearest-neighbour distance between all individuals south of the Sahara across all years was 232 km (95% CI 165–299) and did not differ from the mean distance between individuals within years (228 km, 95% CI 158–289, p = 0.792, n = 45 non-breeding tracks). However, the mean nearest-neighbour distance between all locations of the same individual across years was 16 km (95% CI 0–32) and differed from the mean nearest-neighbour distance between all locations of all individuals in these years (p < 0.001, n = 14 repeated individuals). This illustrates the site fidelity and idiosyncratic use of the different non-breeding sites by godwits in West Africa. When the individual that went to the Inner Niger Delta is excluded, the mean distance between all individuals across all years decreases to 156 km (95% CI 113–201), and the mean distance between individuals within years decreases to 157 km (95% CI 113–199). These distances are not different (p = 0.942, n = 42 non-breeding tracks), which again shows that there are no clear annual differences in site use. With the Inner Niger Delta individual excluded, the mean distance between all locations of the same individual across all years decreases to 11 km (95% CI 0–23) and remains different from the mean distance between all locations (p < 0.001, 13 repeated individuals), thus suggesting strong site fidelity across years.

Northward migration—Site use

From 2016 to 2018, we observed 30 successful northward Sahara crossings by 21 individuals. Most birds that crossed the Sahara arrived in Morocco (37%) and Doñana (33%), while others arrived at the west coast of Portugal (20%), the Balearic coast of mainland Spain (7%) and the Extremadura region (3%; Fig. 1b). At these sites, other tagged godwits were already present (Morocco (1), Doñana (1), and the west coast of Portugal (3)), having spent the non-breeding period north of the Sahara. However, no godwit, including those individuals that spent the non-breeding period north of the Sahara, flew directly to the Netherlands. This means that the minimum number of stopping sites was 1, while the average was 3.02 ± 1.42 and the maximum was 6 (n = 2 individuals). All 11 godwits that arrived in Morocco continued on to Doñana.

As was the case for southward migration, Doñana and the French side of the Bay of Biscay were visited by most of the tracked individuals (Table 1). The 12 individuals that were followed for multiple northward migrations revisited their stopping sites with varying consistency. The annual return rate was 1 for the west coast of Portugal and the Balearic coast of mainland Spain in both 2017 and 2018. Doñana (0.5 in 2017, 0.75 in 2018), the French side of the Bay of Biscay (0.83, 1), northwest Europe (1, 0.5) and the Extremadura Region (1, 0) were less likely to be revisited. Morocco (0, 0) was never revisited by the same individual either in the following year or in any other year. The mean nearest-neighbour distance between stopping locations of the same individual across years was 139 km (95% CI 64–214), which was smaller than the distance between all tracks (282 km, 95% CI 241–324, p = 0.001). This indicates that across years, stopping site use during northward migration is more site-faithful than it is nomadic.

Northward migration—flights

Throughout their northward migration, godwits oriented to the northeast. The absolute amount of longitudinal movement per degree of latitude averaged − 59 km ± 62 (n = 113, Figs. 1b and 3a). The variance of these longitudinal movements did not clearly vary by latitudinal segment (Levene’s test: F32,80 = 1.194, p = 0.259), nor did the average longitudinal movements (F32,80 = 0.891, p = 0.634). The difference in longitudinal movement between tracks of the same individual across years was 69 km ± 54 (range 7–145, n = 6 repeated movements by 5 individuals).

The maximum longitudinal distribution at a given latitudinal boundary ranged from 3 km (51° N) to 2140 km (25° N). The variances of these longitudinal distributions differed between latitudes (Levene’s test: F34,173 = 2.065, p = 0.001). A visual inspection of Figs. 1b and 3b and the variance at each latitudinal boundary suggested that the variance was large at latitudes 25° N, 27–28° N, and 33–34° N (SD > 450 km). We believe the tracks at these latitudes were more widely distributed because some individuals flew over the Atlantic Ocean, some followed the coast, and others flew inland (Figs. 1b and 3b). At latitudes 51° N and 52° N, the variance was especially small (SD < 50 km), probably because individuals were converging on the same breeding site (~ 52° N). Those latitudes that appear to have an especially large or small variance are likely the latitudes that cause the Levene’s test to show that variances differ across latitudinal segments; this notion is supported by the fact that when these latitudes are excluded from the Levene’s test, the variances are homogeneous among the remaining latitudinal segments (F27,140 = 1.380, p = 0.118). Nevertheless, these visual comparisons of the variance at different latitudes are not based on a formal test and should be interpreted with care.

The average longitudinal distributions across all latitudes was 325 km ± 330 (n = 208, Fig. 3b) and varied by latitude (H(34) = 57.874, p = 0.007). A post hoc Dunn pairwise comparison that adjusted p-values for multiple comparisons indicated that no latitudes were different from each other (p > 0.05 for all pairwise comparisons). The longitudinal distribution between tracks of the same individual was on average 195 km ± 227 (range 1–895 km, n = 16 boundary crossings by 8 individuals). The largest longitudinal distribution within tracks of the same individual was 895 km at 33° N (northern Morocco).

Discussion

Our study describes natural variation throughout the annual cycle in the spatial distribution of adult Black-tailed Godwits that breed within a kilometre of each other in The Netherlands. Our results confirm the previous findings of less extensive tracking studies of godwits breeding in The Netherlands (Hooijmeijer et al. 2013; Senner et al. 2019), showing that godwits: (1) migrate southwest and northeast along the same relatively straight, narrow corridor on southward and northward migration, (2) exhibit large individual variation in the distances and duration of their flights, (3) use a network of sites across Western Europe, the Mediterranean, and West Africa (including the Inner Niger Delta), and (4) spend the non-breeding period in the Mediterranean or West Africa. Our results also confirm that godwits are present in the Doñana wetlands in southern Spain during the entire non-breeding period (Table 1), as was previously documented by Márquez-Ferrando et al. (2014).

However, our tracking study also substantially extends our understanding of godwit migration by identifying additional sites used during migration and the non-breeding period. These include sites along the northwest coast of mainland Europe (the English Channel and North Sea), the Balearic coast of mainland Spain (including the wetlands and rice fields in the Ebro Delta and L’Albufera), and the Saloum and Gambia rivers (in Senegal and The Gambia, respectively). Although it was already known that godwits use a network of sites across Western Europe and the Mediterranean (Lourenço and Piersma 2008), previous studies have focused mostly on Doñana (Márquez-Ferrando et al. 2014), the Extremadura region (Masero et al. 2011), and the west coast of Portugal (Lourenço et al. 2010). Our results clearly illustrate the additional importance of Morocco, the Balearic coast of mainland Spain, and the French side of the Bay of Biscay.

Importantly, our study also illuminates another previously unknown aspect of godwit migration. Just as godwits exhibit significantly more variation in migratory timing at the population level compared to the individual level (Verhoeven et al. 2019), our results show that the godwit population as a whole uses a broad network of sites during both south- and northward migration, but that individual godwits are relatively consistent in their site use north of the Sahara and exhibit high site fidelity and individuality in their use of non-breeding sites in West Africa. The fact that individuals return to the same sites probably also explains why the observed longitudinal distributions are somewhat smaller within tracks of single individuals than at the population level. In contrast, however, Morocco was visited by a large proportion of our tracked individuals during northward migration, but was never revisited by the same individual across years. We speculate that godwits coming from West Africa do not attempt to fly to Morocco as a destination, but only stop there out of necessity after encountering headwinds en route (see Loonstra et al. 2019b). Even though such “emergency” stopping sites (Shamoun-Baranes et al. 2010) may be used intermittently, they could be of critical importance for the survival of long-distance migrants.

Applied considerations

In mapping the distribution of godwits across seasons and years in the mid-2010s, we have: (1) identified previously unknown and possibly new sites, (2) revealed that individual godwits differ in which sites they use and (3) demonstrated that godwits show fidelity to these sites. Some of this information can contribute directly to conservation measures; for example, we now realise that any management of stopping and non-breeding sites used by godwits should be applied not only to the well-studied sites across the Iberian Peninsula, but also to sites in Morocco, the Balearic coast of mainland Spain, and the French side of the Bay of Biscay.

However, further work will be needed to effectively maintain or restore stopping and non-breeding sites—a “high priority” goal according to the International Single Species Action Plan for the Conservation of the Black-tailed Godwit (Jensen et al. 2008). Tracked locations must first be put into context—researchers need to begin developing an understanding of why godwits use certain sites, why consistency in site use differs between stopping sites, and what godwits are doing in each place. This will require ground-based methods that gather a great deal of additional information about the habitat characteristics and godwit use at sites across the migratory route (Schlaich et al. 2016; Chan et al. 2019b). For example, that godwits consistently use different sites in West Africa during different times of year is an opportunity to compare those different habitats at different time points and in this way gain insight into the ecological basis of the godwit distributions we have already been able to observe. Certain insights could potentially be extrapolated over the entire range by using remote sensing (Howison et al. 2020, in revision). These exciting possibilities would have valuable implications for conservation efforts—but it is important to realize they are all wholly reliant on first building a detailed description of godwit spatial distribution. Only with such a foundation can we begin to ask the right questions and measure the right parameters.

Population variation in space use

Our detailed description of the natural variation in the migratory routines of godwits allows us to compare migratory behaviour among different species, populations and individuals. The high site fidelity and individuality of godwits in their use of non-breeding sites is similar to that of other migratory shorebirds such as Northern Lapwing (Vanellus vanellus; Eichhorn et al. 2017), Red-Necked Phalaropes (Phalaropus lobatus; van Bemmelen et al. 2019), Icelandic Black-tailed Godwits (Limosa limosa islandica; Gill et al. 2019), Marbled Godwits (Limosa fedoa; Ruthrauff et al. 2019), and Eurasian Woodcock (Scolopax rusticola; Tedeschi et al. 2019). It is also similar to species such as the Marsh Harrier (Circus aeruginosus; Strandberg et al. 2008), Whinchat (Saxicola rubetra; Blackburn and Cresswell 2015) and Montagu’s Harrier (Circus pygargus; Schlaich et al. 2020), all of which, like godwits, use the East-Atlantic Flyway and the Sahel zone.

With this information, we can begin exploring the ecological basis for the variation in migratory routines among these and other species. For example, the long-distance migratory birds on this non-exhaustive list all have distinct life-history strategies, including considerably different mating systems, diets, densities, flight behaviours, and migration routes. This is consistent with the idea that high site fidelity and individuality in the use of non-breeding sites is relatively common among long-distance migratory birds (see Cresswell 2014). However, among the birds that use the East-Atlantic Flyway to travel to the Sahel zone, there are also species with nomadic non-breeding movements (e.g., White Storks (Ciconia ciconia); Berthold et al. 2002) and low non-breeding site fidelity (e.g., European Hoopoes (Upupa epops); van Wijk et al. 2016). There is therefore a gradient in site-use behaviour among long-distance migrants ranging from nomadic to strongly site-faithful.

A similar gradient exists in the spatial distribution of actively migrating birds. For example, when crossing the Sahara, the longitudinal distribution of godwits is similar to that of Marsh Harriers but narrower than that of Ospreys (Pandion haliaetus) and Egyptian Vultures (Neophron percnopterus) (Lopez-Lopez et al. 2014; Vardanis et al. 2016). Even within the Continental Black-tailed Godwit subspecies, there is variation along this gradient: comparing our results to Loonstra et al. (2019c) shows that Polish godwits exhibit higher within- and between-individual variation in space use during migration than Dutch godwits. Such variation among and within species begs the question: what causes these differences?

Given the large variation in space use of migratory birds among continents, flyways, life-history strategies and migratory behaviours (e.g., Finch et al. 2017), differing space use among species is likely not a simple matter of broad ecological differences (such as between passerines vs. raptors, insectivores vs. carnivorous, or short-lived vs. long-lived species). Instead, space use is likely to be species- or even population specific. In the case of Polish versus Dutch godwits, for example, the ways in which social information is transmitted among individuals may be different between the high-density Dutch population and the low-density Polish population, and this may lead to different levels of canalization of migratory routines (see Loonstra et al. 2019c for a more extensive discussion). The fact that migratory routines are likely not based simply on broad ecological differences supports recent calls to study the full annual cycle of different species, and stresses the need to combine the non-breeding and migration ecology of a given species with its population dynamics to better understand the observed differences among species, and in this way effectively inform conservation measures (Marra et al. 2015; Rushing et al. 2017; Cresswell 2018).

At the same time, however, tracking the same individuals across years (e.g., Eichhorn et al. 2017; Ruthrauff et al. 2019) is making it increasingly clear that individuality in space use is common within species. This means that space use is not only species- or population specific, but also specific to individuals. Because individuals from the same population likely have a generally similar genetic background, the observed individual differences are not likely to be of a purely genetic origin. For example, the godwits we tracked breed within a kilometre of each other in an area where natal dispersal was found to be 915 m (95% CI 550–1515 m, Kentie et al. 2014). This means the most likely cause for the individuality we observed is that migratory birds have the phenotypic plasticity to develop differences in migratory routines (sensu Piersma and Drent 2003). This idea is supported by a growing number of studies that have shown age-dependent strategies among migratory birds by (1) tracking juveniles throughout their development, and (2) making comparisons between adults and juveniles tracked in the same year (e.g., Perdeck 1958; Hake et al. 2003; Chernetsov et al. 2004; Lok et al. 2011; Mueller et al. 2013; Gill et al. 2014; Sergio et al. 2014; Rotics et al. 2016; Meyburg et al. 2017; Vansteelant et al. 2017; Verhoeven et al. 2018). This suggests that, in addition to studying the full annual cycle of a population, researchers need to acquire lifelong tracks of individual birds to help identify the mechanistic processes behind the observed variation in adult migration routes. This information will enable researchers to predict whether and how fast a population can adjust to altered surroundings and, as such, is also essential to developing effective conservation measures (Reynolds et al. 2017; Senner et al. 2020).