Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The effects of expectancy-incongruent feedback and self-affirmation on task performance of secondary school students

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the present study, the assumption was tested that expectancy-incongruent feedback in conjunction with explicit self-affirmation directs attention away from the task and to the self. As a result, performance should decrease in resource-sensitive text/picture comprehension tasks as compared to resource-insensitive tasks. Three hundred and thirty-seven fifth graders first completed either easy or difficult text/picture comprehension tasks and then rated their learning outcome. After that, they received bogus feedback that had been congruent or incongruent with their expectations. In addition, half of the participants were given the opportunity for self-affirmation before they completed additional easy or difficult text/picture tasks. As predicted, feedback better than expected or worse than expected impaired performance in the resource-sensitive tasks but only in the self-affirmation condition. The results are discussed with regard to central assumptions of the feedback intervention theory (Kluger and DeNisi, Psychological Bulletin, 119: 254–284, 1996) and practical implications for the provision of feedback.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aronson, J., Cohen, G., & Nail, P. R. (1999). Self-affirmation theory: an update and appraisal. In E. Harmon-Jones & J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Science conference series (pp. 127–147). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baadte, C., & Dutke, S. (2013). Learning about persons: the effects of text structure and executive capacity on conceptual change. European journal of psychology of education, 28, 1045–1064. doi:10.1007/s10212-012-0153-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baadte, C., & Schnotz, W. (2014). Feedback effects on performance, motivation and mood: are they moderated by the learner’s self-concept? Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 58, 570–591. doi:10.1080/00313831.2013.781059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baadte, C., Rasch, T., & Honstein, H. (2016). Attention switching and multimedia learning: the impact of executive resources on the integrative comprehension of texts and pictures. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. doi:10.1080/00313831.2014.965785.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bangert-Drowns, R. L., Kulik, C.-L. C., Kulik, J. A., & Morgan, M. (1991). The instructional effect of feedback in test-like events. Review of Educational Research, 61, 213–238. doi:10.3102/00346543061002213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckmann, N., Beckmann, J. F., & Elliott, J. G. (2009). Self-confidence and performance goal orientation interactively predict performance in a reasoning test with accuracy feedback. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 277–282. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2008.09.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science, 16, 101–105. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00789.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brünken, R., Steinbacher, S., Plass, J. L., & Leutner, D. (2002). Assessment of cognitive load within multimedia learning by the dual task methodology. Experimental Psychology, 49, 109–119. doi:10.1027//1618-3169.49.2.109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunot, S., Huguet, P., & Monteil, J. M. (1999). Performance feedback and self-focused attention in the classroom: when past and present interact. Social Psychology of Education, 3, 271–293. doi:10.1023/A:1009631930740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, D. L., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self-regulated learning: a theoretical synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 65, 245–281. doi:10.3102/00346543065003245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 180–211. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative effect: a control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2008). Feedback processes in the simultaneous regulation of action and affect (Handbook of motivation science, pp. 308–324).

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbalan, G., Kester, L., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2009). Dynamic task selection: effects of feedback and learner control on efficiency and motivation. Learning and Instruction, 19, 455–465. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, P., & Li, H. (2013). Mind wandering in text comprehension under dual-task conditions. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–14. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutke, S., & Rinck, M. (2006). Multimedia learning: working memory and the learning of word and picture diagrams. Learning and Instruction, 16, 526–537. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.10.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutke, S., Baadte, C., Hähnel, A., von Hecker, U., & Rinck, M. (2010). Using diagnostic text information to constrain situation models. Discourse Processes, 47, 510–544. doi:10.1080/01638530903416257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (pp. 102–134). London: Cambridge Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174909.007.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Friese, M., Messner, C., & Schaffner, Y. (2012). Mindfulness meditation counteracts self-control depletion. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1016–1022. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.01.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friese, M., Binder, J., Luechinger, R., Boesiger, P., & Rasch, B. (2013). Suppressing emotions impairs subsequent Stroop performance and reduces prefrontal brain activation. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e60385. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0060385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A., & Tiffany, C. (2008). Deficits in verbal long-term memory and learning in children with poor phonological short-term memory skills. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 474–490. doi:10.1080/17470210701273443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1986). Persistent high self-focus after failure and low self-focus after success: the depressive self-focusing style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1039–1044. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.1039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of Educational Research, 77, 81–112. doi:10.3102/003465430298487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 962. doi:10.1037/a0012705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Bleckley, K. M., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, S., & Berman, M. G. (2010). Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 43–57. doi:10.1177/1745691609356784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: historical review, a meta-analysis and a preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 254–284. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1998). Feedback interventions: toward the understanding of a double-edged sword. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 67–72. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindenberger, U., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychology and Aging, 15, 417–436. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipnevich, A. A., & Smith, J. K. (2009). Effects of differential feedback on students’ examination performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 319–333. doi:10.1037/a0017841.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S., & Baddeley, A. D. (2004). Is there a specific executive capacity for dual task coordination? Evidence from Alzheimer’s Disease. Neuropsychology, 18, 504–513. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 302. doi:10.1037/a0025250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mory, E. H. (2004). Feedback research revisited. In D. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology (pp. 745–783). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narciss, S. (2006). Informatives tutorielles Feedback. Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narciss, S. (2008a). Feedback strategies for interactive learning tasks. In J. M. Spector, M. D. Merrill, J. J. D. van Merrienboer, & M. P. Driscoll (Eds.), Handbook of research on educational communications and technology, 3 (pp. 125–143). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narciss, S. (2008b). Feedback strategies for interactive learning tasks. Handbook of research on educational communications and technology, 3, 125–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narciss, S., & Huth, K. (2006). Fostering achievement and motivation with bug-related tutoring feedback in a computer-based training for written subtraction. Learning and Instruction, 16, 310–322. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 241. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmeichel, B. J., & Demaree, H. A. (2010). Working memory capacity and spontaneous emotion regulation: high capacity predicts self-enhancement in response to negative feedback. Emotion, 10, 739–744. doi:10.1037/a0019355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., Horz, H., McElvany, N., Schroeder, S., Ullrich, M., Baumert, J., Hachfeld, A., & Richter, T. (2010). Das BITE-Projekt: Integrative Verarbeitung von Texten und Bildern in der Sekundarstufe I (Integrative comprehension of texts and pictures in secondary schools). In E. Klieme, D. Leutner, & M. Kenk (Eds.), Kompetenzmodellierung. Zwischenbilanz des DFG-Schwerpunktprogramms und Perspektiven des Forschungsansatzes. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 56 (pp. 143–153). Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schöne, C., Dickhäuser, O., Spinath, B., & Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (2002). Skalen zur Erfassung des schulischen Selbstkonzeptes SESSKO (Scales to assess the academic self-concept). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2002). Accepting threatening information: self-affirmation and the reduction of defensive biases. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 119–123. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-defense: self-affirmation theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 183. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38004-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, D. K., Cohen, G. L., Nelson, L. D., Nussbaum, A. D., Bunyan, D. P., & Garcia, J. (2009). Affirmed yet unaware: exploring the role of awareness in the process of self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 745. doi:10.1037/a0015451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, D. K., Hartson, K. A., Binning, K. R., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Garcia, J., Taborsky-Barba, S., & Cohen, G. L. (2013). Deflecting the trajectory and changing the narrative: how self-affirmation affects academic performance and motivation under identity threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 591–618. doi:10.1037/a0031495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shute, V. J. (2008). Focus on formative feedback. Review of Educational Research, 78, 153–189. doi:10.3102/0034654307313795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: sustaining the integrity of the self. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 261–302. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60229-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 294–321. doi:10.1006/jecp.2000.2587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., et al. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 17152–17156. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707678104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ullrich, M., Schnotz, W., Horz, H., McElvany, N., Schroeder, S., & Baumert, J. (2012). Kognitionspsychologische Aspekte der Bild-Text-Integration (Cognitive aspects of text and picture integration). Psychologische Rundschau, 63, 11–17. doi:10.1026/0033-3042/a000105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vancouver, J. B., & Tischner, E. C. (2004). The effect of feedback sign on task performance depends on self-concept discrepancies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1092–1098. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.89.6.1092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venables, L., & Fairclough, S. (2009). The influence of performance feedback on goal-setting and mental effort regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 63–74. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9116-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christiane Baadte.

Additional information

Christiane Baadte. Johannes Gutenberg-Universität Mainz, Psychologisches Institut, Binger Straße 14-16, 55122 Mainz, Germany. E-mail: baadte@uni-mainz.de

Current themes of research:

Cognitive processes and mental model construction with texts, illustrations, and multiple representations. Working memory and learning with multimedia. Working memory and the processing of feedback. Effects of feedback on self-regulated text/picture comprehension. Didactic and diagnostic competences of teachers. Attachment and self/emotion regulation.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Baadte, C., & Dutke, S. (2013). Learning about persons: The effects of text structure and executive capacity on conceptual change. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28, 1045–1064. doi: 10.1007/s10212-012-0153-2.

Baadte, C., Rasch, T., & Honstein, H. (2015). Attention switching and multimedia learning: The impact of executive resources on the integrative comprehension of texts and pictures. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 59, 478498. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2014.965785.

Baadte, C., & Schnotz, W. (2014). Feedback effects on performance, motivation and mood: Are they moderated by the learner’s self-concept? Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 58, 570–591. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2013.781059.

Schnotz, W., & Baadte, C. (2015). Surface and deep structures in graphics comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 43, 605–618. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0490-2.

Schnotz, W., Mengelkamp, C., Baadte, C., & Hauck, G. (2014). Focus of attention and choice of text modality in multimedia learning. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29, 483–501. doi: 10.1007/s10212-013-0209-y.

Friedrike Kurenbach. University of Koblenz-Landau, Fortstr. 7, 76829 Landau, Germany. E-mail: fuchs@uni-landau.de

Current themes of research:

Teacher education. School psychology.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

None.

Appendix

Appendix

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baadte, C., Kurenbach, F. The effects of expectancy-incongruent feedback and self-affirmation on task performance of secondary school students. Eur J Psychol Educ 32, 113–131 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-016-0312-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-016-0312-y

Keywords

Navigation