Skip to main content
Log in

Implementation of the diffusion model on dot-probe task performance in children with behavioral inhibition

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Attentional bias to threat, the process of preferentially attending to potentially threatening environmental stimuli over neutral stimuli, is positively associated with behavioral inhibition (BI) and trait anxiety. However, the most used measure of attentional bias to threat, the dot-probe task, has been criticized for demonstrating poor reliability. The present study aimed to assess whether utilizing a sequential sampling model to describe performance could detect adequate test–retest reliability for the dot-probe task, demonstrate stronger cueing effects, and improve the association with neural signals of early attention. One hundred and twenty children aged 9–12 years completed the dot-probe task twice. During the second administration, event-related potentials (ERPs) were obtained as time-sensitive neural markers of attention. BI was not associated with traditional or diffusion model measures of performance. Traditional and diffusion model measures of performance were also not associated with N1, P2, or N2 ERP amplitude. There were main effects of Visit, in which RTs were faster and standard deviation of RT smaller during the second administration due to an increase in drift rate and a decrease in non-decision time. The traditional RT bias score (r = 0.06) and bias scores formed via diffusion model parameters (all r’s < 0.40) all demonstrated poor reliability. Results confirm recommendations to move away from using the dot-probe task as the primary or sole index of attentional bias.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and material

Raw study data used in this publication is available upon request.

Notes

  1. The information used to judge the orientation of the target is also influenced by learned factors that are independent of the perceptual qualities of the target itself (e.g. including learning to suppress distractors, see Sewell et al. (2018)). This latter process, however, is less likely to be observed in a task like the dot-probe, where explicit distractors are not present.

  2. Results did not change when a cutoff of < 300 ms was applied to better approximate cutoffs used for diffusion modeling.

  3. Because the CDF plots suggested the presence of some misfits, 1000 datasets were subsequently simulated. Participants who exhibited a lower model fit (defined as < 10% quantile of the distribution of p values) for any of the four conditions were removed from analysis. This resulted in a reduced N = 57 (22 BI, 34 Controls). CDF plots generated from the remaining participants demonstrated improved model fit, but primary results did not change. See Supplementary Table 2 and Supplementary Fig. 1.

  4. A Cue (3: Neutral, Threat Congruent, Threat Incongruent) Visit (2) BI (2) GLM replicated these effects. However, this GLM identified an additional Visit (2) Cue (3) interaction on Ter (F(2, 236) = 5.17, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.042) in which the Neutral cue trials did not differ between visit 1 and 2 as much as the task condition cue trials did.

References

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., & Glickman, S. (2005). Attentional bias in anxiety: A behavioral and ERP study. Brain and Cognition, 59(1), 11–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibition? Child Development, 74(6), 1899–1917. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00645.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blackford, J. U., & Pine, D. S. (2012). Neural substrates of childhood anxiety disorders: a review of neuroimaging findings. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics, 21(3), 501–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carretié, L., Mercado, F., Tapia, M., & Hinojosa, J. A. (2001). Emotion, attention, and the ‘negativity bias’, studied through event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 41(1), 75–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chronis-Tuscano, A., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Diaz, Y., et al. (2009). Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(9), 928–935. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09df

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. H. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 28–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. H. (1999). Top-down attentional guidance based on implicit learning of visual covariation. Psychological Science, 10(4), 360–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coll, C. G., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Development, 55, 1005–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrenreich, J. T., & Gross, A. M. (2002). Biased attentional behavior in childhood anxiety: A review of theory and current empirical investigation. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(7), 991–1008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Holmes, A. (2007). Event-related brain potential correlates of emotional face processing. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 15–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.022

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Eldar, S., Yankelevitch, R., Lamy, D., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2010). Enhanced neural reactivity and selective attention to threat in anxiety. Biological Psychology, 85(2), 252–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enkavi, A. Z., Eisenberg, I. W., Bissett, P. G., Mazza, G. L., MacKinnon, D. P., Marsch, L. A., & Poldrack, R. A. (2019). Large-scale analysis of test–retest reliabilities of self-regulation measures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(12), 5472–5477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fu, X., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2019). Threat-related attention bias in socioemotional development: A critical review and methodological considerations. Developmental Review, 51, 31–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55(4), 468–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 1166–1186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, H. A. (2010). Electrophysiological correlates of cognitive control and the regulation of shyness in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(2), 177–193. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640903526538

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang-Pollock, C., & Nigg, J. T. (2003). Searching for the attention deficit in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the case of visuospatial orienting. Clinical Psychology Review, 23(6), 801–830. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(03)00073-4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J. L., Dodd, H. F., Lyneham, H. J., & Bovopoulous, N. (2011). Temperament and family environment in the development of anxiety disorder: Two-year follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(12), 1255–1264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.09.009

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Research, 1148, 138–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanske, P., Plitschka, J., & Kotz, S. A. (2011). Attentional orienting towards emotion: P2 and N400 ERP effects. Neuropsychologia, 49(11), 3121–3129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kappenman, E. S., Farrens, J. L., Luck, S. J., & Proudfit, G. H. (2014). Behavioral and ERP measures of attentional bias to threat in the dot-probe task: Poor reliability and lack of correlation with anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1368.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kunar, M. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (2011). Target absent trials in configural contextual cuing. Attention Perception and Psychophysics, 73(7), 2077–2091. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0164-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunar, M. A., Flusberg, S., Horowitz, T. S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Does contextual cuing guide the deployment of attention? Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 816–828. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.816

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lamm, C., Walker, O. L., Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., Pine, D. S., McDermott, J. M., & Fox, N. A. (2014). Cognitive control moderates early childhood temperament in predicting social behavior in 7-year-old children: an ERP study. Developmental Science, 17(5), 667–681.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leite, F. P., & Ratcliff, R. (2011). What cognitive processes drive response biases? A diffusion model analysis. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(7), 651–687.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerche, V., & Voss, A. (2017). Retest reliability of the parameters of the Ratcliff diffusion model. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 81(3), 629–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lisk, S., Vaswani, A., Linetzky, M., Bar-Haim, Y., & Lau, J. Y. F. (2019). Systematic review and meta-analysis: Eye-tracking of attention to threat in child and adolescent anxiety. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.06.006

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., Woodman, G. F., & Vogel, E. K. (2000). Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 432–440. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01545-x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molloy, A., & Anderson, P. L. (2020). Evaluating the reliability of attention bias and attention bias variability measures in the dot-probe task among people with social anxiety disorder. Psychological Assessment, 32(9), 883.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morales, S., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2017). Patterns of attention to threat across tasks in behaviorally inhibited children at risk for anxiety. Developmental Science, 20(2), e12391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, E. M., Hofmann, S. G., Santesso, D. L., Meuret, A. E., Bitran, S., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). Electrophysiological evidence of attentional biases in social anxiety disorder. Psychological Medicine, 39(7), 1141–1152. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291708004820

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mulder, M. J., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Ratcliff, R., Boekel, W., & Forstmann, B. U. (2012). Bias in the brain: A diffusion model analysis of prior probability and potential payoff. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(7), 2335–2343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-Edgar, K., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., McDermott, J. M., White, L. K., Henderson, H. A., Degnan, K. A., et al. (2011). Attention biases to threat link behavioral inhibition to social withdrawal over time in very young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(6), 885–895.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Peter, J. P., Churchill, G. A., & Brown, T. J. (1993). Caution in the use of difference scores in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 19(4), 655–662. https://doi.org/10.1086/209329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, R. B., Rosen, D., Siegle, G. J., Ladouceur, C. D., Tang, K., Allen, K. B., et al. (2016). From anxious youth to depressed adolescents: Prospective prediction of 2-year depression symptoms via attentional bias measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Price, R. B., Brown, V., & Siegle, G. J. (2019). Computational modeling applied to the dot-probe task yields improved reliability and mechanistic insights. Biological Psychiatry, 85(7), 606–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2002). Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(3), 438–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodebaugh, T. L., Scullin, R. B., Langer, J. K., Dixon, D. J., Huppert, J. D., Bernstein, A., et al. (2016). Unreliability as a threat to understanding psychopathology: The cautionary tale of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(6), 840.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rossignol, M., Campanella, S., Bissot, C., & Philippot, P. (2013). Fear of negative evaluation and attentional bias for facial expressions: An event-related study. Brain and Cognition, 82(3), 344–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.008

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, D. A., Richler, J. J., & Gauthier, I. (2015). Reliability of composite-task measurements of holistic face processing. Behavior Research Methods, 47(3), 736–743.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schankin, A., & Schubo, A. (2009). Cognitive processes facilitated by contextual cueing: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 46(3), 668–679. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00807.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schankin, A., & Schubo, A. (2010). Contextual cueing effects despite spatially cued target locations. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 717–727. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00979.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(8), 1008–1015. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199908000-00017

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sewell, D. K., Colagiuri, B., & Livesey, E. J. (2018). Response time modeling reveals multiple contextual cuing mechanisms. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(5), 1644–1665. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1364-y

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. J. (2002). Non-spatial attentional effects on P1. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113(12), 1903–1908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thai, N., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2016). Neural correlates of attention biases, behavioral inhibition, and social anxiety in children: An ERP study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 200–210.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Victeur, Q., Huguet, P., & Silvert, L. (2020). Attentional allocation to task-irrelevant fearful faces is not automatic: Experimental evidence for the conditional hypothesis of emotional selection. Cognition and Emotion, 34(2), 288–301. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1622512

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel, S., Mathys, C., Daunizeau, J., Bauer, M., Driver, J., Friston, K. J., & Stephan, K. E. (2014). Spatial attention, precision, and bayesian inference: A study of saccadic response speed. Cerebral Cortex, 24(6), 1436–1450. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs418

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, A., Nagler, M., & Lerche, V. (2013). Diffusion models in experimental psychology: A practical introduction. Experimental Psychology, 60(6), 385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waechter, S., & Stolz, J. A. (2015). Trait anxiety, state anxiety, and attentional bias to threat: Assessing the psychometric properties of response time measures. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(4), 441–458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9670-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waechter, S., Nelson, A. L., Wright, C., Hyatt, A., & Oakman, J. (2014). Measuring attentional bias to threat: Reliability of dot probe and eye movement indices. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(3), 313–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weigard, A., & Huang-Pollock, C. L. (2014). A diffusion modeling approach to understanding contextual cueing effects in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(12), 1336–1344. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12250

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, C. N., & Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Decomposing bias in different types of simple decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 385.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, C. N., Ratcliff, R., Vasey, M. W., & McKoon, G. (2010). Anxiety enhances threat processing without competition among multiple inputs: A diffusion model analysis. Emotion, 10(5), 662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, C. N., Skokin, K., Carlos, B., & Weaver, A. (2016). Using decision models to decompose anxiety-related bias in threat classification. Emotion, 16(2), 196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, G., Liu, Q., Jiao, J., Zhou, P. L., Li, H., & Sun, H. J. (2012). Dual-state modulation of the contextual cueing effect: Evidence from eye movement recordings. Journal of Vision, 12(6), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1167/12.6.11

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Pennsylvania State University Social, Life, and Engineering Sciences Imaging Center (SLEIC) Human Electrophysiology Facility for supporting data collection, the TAU/NIMH ABMT Initiative for providing the task toolkit, and the many individuals who contributed to data collection and data processing. We would especially like to thank the parents of the children who participated and continue to participate in our studies.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health under Grant [R01MH094633] to Koraly Pérez-Edgar.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shane Wise.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

There are no known conflicts of interest for any author.

Ethics approval

Study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB number PRAMS00038109) of The Pennsylvania State University. Families were compensated for their participation.

Consent to participate

Participants and their parents provided written assent and consent for data collection.

Consent for publication

Participants and their parents provided written assent and consent for potential data sharing.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wise, S., Huang-Pollock, C. & Pérez-Edgar, K. Implementation of the diffusion model on dot-probe task performance in children with behavioral inhibition. Psychological Research 86, 831–843 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01532-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01532-3

Navigation