Skip to main content
Log in

Measuring Attentional Bias to Threat: Reliability of Dot Probe and Eye Movement Indices

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A variety of methodological paradigms, including dot probe and eye movement tasks, have been used to examine attentional biases to threat in anxiety disorders. Unfortunately, little attention has been devoted to the psychometric properties of measures from these paradigms. In the current study, participants selected for high and low social anxiety completed a dot probe and eye movement task using angry, disgust and happy facial expressions paired with neutral expressions. Results indicated that dot probe bias scores, eye movement first fixation indices, and eye movement proportions of viewing time in the first 1,500 ms had unacceptably low reliability. However, eye movement indices of attentional bias over the full 5,000 ms time course had excellent reliability. Individuals’ dot probe and eye movement biases were largely uncorrelated across the two tasks and demonstrated little relation with social anxiety scores. Implications for future research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Of course, negative reliabilities are theoretically impossible and are considered to be misestimates of zero.

  2. NimStim actors used in the dot probe task were 1, 2, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 18, 20, 21, 22, 23, 26, 28, 29, 31, 32, 33, 34, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, and 41. Actors used in the eye movement task were 1, 2, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 20, 21, 22, 23, 26, 28, 29, 31, 33, 34, 36, and 37.

  3. Images were positioned above and below the fixation cross, rather than side-by-side, since previous research has found the following: (1) the reliability of the dot probe with stimuli side-by-side was poor (Staugaard 2009) and (2) the probability of looking at the left image first in eye movement tasks was high (e.g., .76, Nelson et al. 2010; manuscript in preparation), regardless of stimulus type.

  4. Given that social desirability has been known to confound the results of attentional bias studies, correlations between SIAS scores and bias scores were computed again with SDS scores entered as a covariate. The pattern of results did not change.

  5. We tested the theory that the negative reliabilities are driven by individual differences in fixating on the top image by generating semi-random data that simulated the ‘look up bias’ while preserving individual differences in this tendency. The simulation replicated the first fixation data pattern. The simulation is available from the first author on request.

  6. One participant’s time course data could not be extracted due technical difficulties despite having completed the eye movement task.

  7. Correlations between SIAS scores and eye movement indices were computed again with SDS scores entered as a covariate and the pattern of results did not change.

References

  • Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., & Przeworski, A. (2003). Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: Facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(11), 1325–1335. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00039-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176–181. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, T., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 704–723. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asmundson, G. J. G., & Stein, M. B. (1994). Selective processing of social threat in patients with generalized social phobia: Evaluation using a dot-probe paradigm. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 8(2), 107–117. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(94)90009-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Holoshitz, Y., Eldar, S., Frenkel, T. I., Muller, D., Charney, D. S., et al. (2010). Life-threatening danger and suppression of attention bias to threat. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(6), 694–698. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070956.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 92–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bögels, S. M., & Mansell, W. (2004). Attention processes in the maintenance and treatment of social phobia: Hypervigilance, avoidance and self-focused attention. Clinical Psychology Review, 24(7), 827–856. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2004.06.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borgmann, K. W. U., Risko, E. F., Stolz, J. A., & Besner, D. (2007). Simon says: Reliability and the role of working memory and attentional control in the Simon task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 313–319. doi:10.3758/BF03194070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Millar, N. H. (2000). Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 14(6), 789–808. doi:10.1080/02699930050156636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., Bonham-Carter, C., Fergusson, E., Jenkins, J., et al. (1997). Attentional biases for emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 25–42. doi:10.1080/026999397380014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, J. D., DeWall, C. N., Schmidt, N. B., & Maner, J. K. (2010a). A tale of two threats: Social anxiety and attention to social threat as a function of social exclusion and non-exclusion threats. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34(5), 449–455. doi:10.1007/s10608-009-9254-x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, J. D., Maner, J. K., & Schmidt, N. B. (2010b). Difficulty disengaging attention from social threat in social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34(1), 99–105. doi:10.1007/s10608-008-9205-y.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M. G., & Avero, P. (2005). Time course of attentional bias to emotional scenes in anxiety: Gaze direction and duration. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3), 433–451.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisler, J. M., Bacon, A. K., & Williams, N. L. (2009). Phenomenological characteristics of attentional biases towards threat: A critical review. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33(2), 221–234. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9161-y.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203–216. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), r (pp. 69–93). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. M., Bailey, J. E., Diaper, A., Stirland, R., Renton, L. E., Benton, C. P., et al. (2011). Effects of 7.5% CO2 inhalation on allocation of spatial attention to facial cues of emotional expression. Cognition and Emotion, 25(4), 626–638. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.508887.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crocker, L., & Algina, J. (1986). Introduction to classical and modern test theory. Orlando, FL: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

  • Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., Oakman, J. M., Farvolden, P., van Ameringen, M., Mancini, C., et al. (2005). Individuals with social phobia are biased to become aware of negative faces. Visual Cognition, 12(1), 159–179. doi:10.1080/13506280444000175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eide, P., Kemp, A., Silberstein, R. B., Nathan, P. J., & Stough, C. (2002). Test-retest reliability of the emotional stroop task: Examining the paradox of measurement change. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 136(5), 514–520. doi:10.1080/00223980209605547.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. Hove, England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis.

  • Fischer, D. G., & Fick, C. (1993). Measuring social desirability: Short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(2), 417–424. doi:10.1177/0013164493053002011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1993). Attentional bias in anxiety: Selective or not? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(5), 487–493. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(93)90129-I.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble, A. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2010). The time-course of attention to emotional faces in social phobia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(1), 39–44. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.08.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, M., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Orienting and maintenance of gaze to facial expressions in social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 760–770. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerdes, A. B. M., Pauli, P., & Alpers, G. W. (2009). Toward and away from spiders: Eye-movements in spider-fearful participants. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116(6), 725–733. doi:10.1007/s00702-008-0167-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grös, D. F., Antony, M. M., Simms, L. J., & McCabe, R. E. (2007). Psychometric properties of the state-trait inventory for cognitive and somatic anxiety (STICSA): Comparison to the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI). Psychological Assessment, 19(4), 369–381. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.19.4.369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C., & Imada, S. (1997). Body, psyche, and culture: The relationship of disgust to morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, 107–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Havlicek, L. L., & Peterson, N. L. (1977). Effect of the violation of assumptions upon significance levels of the Pearson r. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 373–377. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.2.373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henson, R. K. (2001). Understanding internal consistency reliability estimates: A conceptual primers on coefficient alpha. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 177–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., & Eelen, P. (1999). Eye movement registration as a continuous index of attention deployment: Data from a group of spider anxious students. Cognition and Emotion, 13(4), 419–434. doi:10.1080/026999399379249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hope, D. A., Rapee, R. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Dombeck, M. J. (1990). Representations of the self in social phobia: Vulnerability to social threat. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14(2), 177–189. doi:10.1007/BF01176208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horwitz, S. K., & Horwitz, I. B. (2012). Small is beautiful: Implications of reliability and statistical power for testing the efficacy of HR interventions. Human Resource Management, 51, 143–160. doi:10.1002/hrm.20465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaspar, K., & König, P. (2011). Overt attention and context factors: The impact of repeated presentations, image type, and individual motivation. PLoS ONE, 6, e21719. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021719.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, M. O., Fleming, K., Bandy, C., Kim, J., & Zambetti, A. (2010a). Eye tracking and visual attention to threating stimuli in veterans of the Iraq war. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(3), 293–299. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.12.006.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, M. O., Fleming, K., Bandy, C., & Zambetti, A. (2010b). Attention to novel and target stimuli in trauma survivors. Psychiatry Research, 178(3), 501–506. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.10.009.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klumpp, H., & Amir, N. (2009). Examination of vigilance and disengagement of threat in social anxiety with a probe detection task. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International Journal, 22(3), 283–296. doi:10.1080/10615800802449602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion, 4(3), 312–317. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the fear of negative evaluation scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9(3), 371–375. doi:10.1177/0146167283093007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levens, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Updating positive and negative stimuli in working memory in depression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 654. doi:10.1037/a0020283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, J. W., Lawrence, M. A., McConnell, M. M., Eskes, G. A., Klein, R. M., & Shore, D. I. (2010). Appraising the ANT: Psychometric and theoretical considerations of the attention network test. Neuropsychology, 24(5), 637–651. doi:10.1037/a0019803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, E., Risko, E. F., Preston, F., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. (2010). Challenging the reliability and validity of cognitive measures: The case of the numerical distance effect. Acta Psychologica, 134, 154–161. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mansell, W., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., & Chen, Y. (2002). Attention to positive and negative social-evaluative words: Investigating the effects of social anxiety, trait anxiety and social threat. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International Journal, 15(1), 19–29. doi:10.1080/10615800290007263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(4), 455–470. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(9), 809–848. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2002). Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(12), 1403–1414. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00017-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2007). Anxiety and orienting of gaze to angry and fearful faces. Biological Psychology, 76(3), 163–169. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.005.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mühlberger, A., Wieser, M. J., & Pauli, P. (2008). Visual attention during virtual social situations depends on social anxiety. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11(4), 425–430. doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.0084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. R., & Davidshofer, C. O. (2005). Psychological testing: Principles and testing. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

  • Nelson, A., Quigley, L., Carriere, J., Purdon, C., & Smilek, D. (2010, June). The role of state and trait-anxiety on the time-course of selective attention towards emotional images. Boston, Massachusetts: Poster presented at the 6th World Congress of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies.

  • Nummenmaa, L., Hyönä, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6(2), 257–268. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ononaiye, M. S. P., Turpin, G., & Reidy, J. G. (2007). Attentional bias in social anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration and social-evaluative anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31(6), 727–740. doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9096-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pflugshaupt, T., Mosimann, U. P., von Wartburg, R., Schmitt, W., Nyffeler, T., & Müri, R. M. (2005). Hypervigilance-avoidance pattern in spider phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(1), 105–116. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.12.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pineles, S. L., & Mineka, S. (2005). Attentional biases to internal and external sources of potential threat in social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(2), 314–318. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pishyar, R., Harris, L. M., & Menzies, R. G. (2008). Responsiveness of measures of attentional bias to clinical change in social phobia. Cognition and Emotion, 22(7), 1209–1227. doi:10.1080/02699930701686008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quigley, L., Nelson, A. L., Carriere, J., Smilek, D., & Purdon, C. (2012). The effects of trait and state anxiety on attention to emotional images: An eye-tracking study. Cognition and Emotion, 26(8), 1390–1411. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.662892.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 741–756. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00022-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ree, M. J., French, D., MacLeod, C., & Locke, V. (2008). Distinguishing cognitive and somatic dimensions of state and trait anxiety: Development and validation of the state-trait inventory for cognitive and somatic anxiety (STICSA). Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36(3), 313–332. doi:10.1017/S1352465808004232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2006). Spider fearful individuals attend to threat, then quickly avoid it: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(2), 231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohner, J. (2002). The time-course of visual threat processing: High trait anxious individuals eventually avert their gaze from angry faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16(6), 837–844. doi:10.1080/02699930143000572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sattler, J. M. (2008). Assessment of children: Cognitive foundations. La Mesa, CA: Jerome M. Sattler, Publisher, Inc.

  • Schmidt, N. B., Richey, J. A., Buckner, J. D., & Timpano, K. R. (2009). Attention training for generalized social anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 5–14. doi:10.1037/a0013643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmukle, S. C. (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality, 19, 595–605. doi:10.1002/per.554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schofield, C. A., Johnson, A. L., Inhoff, A. W., & Coles, M. E. (2012). Social anxiety and difficulty disengaging threat: Evidence from eye-tracking. Cognition and Emotion, 26(2), 300–311. doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.602050.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, L. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). Attentional focus in social anxiety disorder: Potential for interactive processes. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(7), 1206–1221. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegrist, M. (1995). Reliability of the stroop test with single-stimulus presentation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81(3), 1295–1298. doi:10.2466/pms.1995.81.3f.1295.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Staugaard, S. (2009). Reliability of two versions of the dot-probe task using photographic faces. Psychology Science, 51(3), 339–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staugaard, S. (2010). Threatening faces and social anxiety: A literature review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(6), 669–690. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Staugaard, S. R., & Rosenberg, N. K. (2011). Processing of emotional faces in social phobia. Mental Illness, 3(1), 14–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S., Rist, F., & Gerlach, A. L. (2009). Influence of alcohol on the processing of emotional facial expressions in individuals with social phobia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48(2), 125–140. doi:10.1348/014466508X368856.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stolz, J. A., Besner, D., & Carr, T. H. (2005). Implications of measure of reliability for theories of priming: Activity in semantic memory is inherently noisy and uncoordinated. Visual Cognition, 12, 284–336. doi:10.1080/13506280444000030.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strahan, R., & Gerbasi, K. C. (1972). Short, homogeneous versions of the marlowe-crowne social desirability scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(2), 191–193. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197204)28:2<191:AID-JCLP2270280220>3.0.CO;2-G.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., et al. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242–249.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. A. (1983). Speed of information processing and general intelligence. Intelligence, 7(1), 53–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waechter, S., Besner, D., & Stolz, J. A. (2010). Visual word recognition: On the reliability of repetition priming. Visual Cognition, 18, 537–558. doi:10.1080/13506280902868603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weierich, M. R., Treat, T. A., & Hollingworth, A. (2008). Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 985–1018.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wieser, M. J., Pauli, P., Alpers, G. W., & Mühlberger, A. (2009a). Is eye to eye contact really threatening and avoided in social anxiety?—An eye-tracking and psychophysiology study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(1), 93–103. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.04.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wieser, M. J., Pauli, P., Weyers, P., Alpers, G. W., & Mühlberger, A. (2009b). Fear of negative evaluation and the hypervigilance-avoidance hypothesis: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116(6), 717–723. doi:10.1007/s00702-008-0101-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, R. H., Zimmerman, D. W., & Zumbo, B. D. (1995). Impact of measurement error on statistical power: Review of an old paradox. Journal of Experimental Education, 63(4), 363–370. doi:10.1080/00220973.1995.9943470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 3–47. doi:10.1080/02699930903205698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 665–681. doi:10.1080/02724980042000462.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2005). In A. Wenzel, D. C. Rubin (Eds.), Selective attention tasks in clinical research (pp. 97–117). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10870-006.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC) Alexander Graham Bell Canada Graduate Scholarship to S. W., a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) Joseph-Armand Bombardier Canadian Graduate Scholarship to A. L. N, and a SSHRC Insight Development Grant (430-2013-0916) to J. O. We would like to thank Mike Lawrence for his assistance with the analyses in R.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephanie Waechter.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Waechter, S., Nelson, A.L., Wright, C. et al. Measuring Attentional Bias to Threat: Reliability of Dot Probe and Eye Movement Indices. Cogn Ther Res 38, 313–333 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9588-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9588-2

Keywords

Navigation