Introduction

Vocal communication of animals has two main components: signallers emit acoustic signals, and receivers perceive them and process the information content of the signals (Maynard Smith and Harper 2003; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). Many avian lineages evolved fine-tuned acoustic communication systems and have a great variety of auditory signals for several functions, including songs (Catchpole and Slater 2008) and/or calls (Marler 2004). Oscine birds (Passeriformes), parrots (Psittaciformes), and hummingbirds (Trochilliformes) are able to learn acoustic elements from parents and neighbours, or even imitate different species (Kroodsma and Miller 1982). Other lineages are not vocal-learners and have simpler, innate vocalizations (Brenowitz 1991), which typically show less variation among nearby individuals.

Auditory communication in birds needs effective signals to transmit the information with no or minimal degradation to the receivers (Dooling 1982). However, emitted signals may differ from the received signals (Forrest 1994). For example, habitat structure (Morton 1975; Job et al. 2016), distance (McGregor and Krebs 1984; Mouterde et al. 2014), and anthropogenic traffic noise (Jung et al. 2020) may cause degradation in the structure of the vocal signal and, consequently, also in its information content (Wiley and Richards 1982). Both signallers and receivers show adaptations to environmental conditions to preserve the information of acoustic signals during transmission (Brumm and Naguib 2009). However, degraded sound may nonetheless give some specific but still useful information to the receiver, for example, about distance (McGregor and Krebs 1984).

Common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) are obligate avian brood parasites that lay their eggs in the nests of other bird species, and leave the incubation and rearing of their progeny to these hosts (Payne 2005; Erritzøe et al. 2012). Common cuckoos have a simple acoustic repertoire (Lei et al. 2005); their most famous advertisement call (“cu-coo”) is frequently emitted during the breeding season (Deng et al. 2019a; Yoo et al. 2020). Sometimes, this characteristic call has unique or aberrant features (Møller et al. 2016), and individual cuckoos can be identified based on their call variation; for example, an individual cuckoo was tracked in Germany by its unusual calls for several years (Naumann 1901).

During our long-term research on common cuckoos (Geltsch et al. 2017), we discovered a handful of individuals uttering both normal (“cu-coo”) and aberrant (“cu-kee”) calls (Fig. 1). Whereas we found several individuals with this second type of call in central Hungary, normal and aberrant calls both occurred together in the calling sequences of these same individuals. Here, we investigated whether this rare call type reduced the efficiency of the typical “cu-coo” call in male responses to territorial intrusion using playback experiments. We hypothesised that responses of territorial male cuckoos to aberrant call playbacks would be diminished because these rare signals did not indicate territorial intrusion functionally. Alternatively, the aberrant call might function normally if the aberration affected non-functional parts of the call, which did not contain any or critical signalling content.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Male common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) calls: (a) a normal call (“cu-coo”) with the typical two notes; (b) an aberrant call (“cu-kee”) with the different second note

Materials and methods

The study was conducted about 50 km south of Budapest, central Hungary, in a 20 km × 30 km area near Apaj (47° 6′ 53.9″ N; 19° 5′ 21.2″ E), where cuckoos parasitize great reed warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) in narrow reed beds along the sides of small irrigation channels (Moskát and Honza 2000). We recorded common cuckoo calls used for our playback study in May between 2017 and 2019. For sound recording, we used a Telinga Universal parabola dish with Rycote Hi Wind Cover, a Sennheiser ME-62 microphone, a K6 powering module, a FEL MX mono preamp, and a Marantz PMD-620 MKII recorder (sampling rate: 48 kHz, 24-bit quality) (Moskát et al. 2017).

To assess the extent of the uniqueness of the aberrant call type, we surveyed the Xeno-Canto (www.xeno-canto.com) database on May 1, 2020; this is a publicly collected and openly available library of avian calls and songs recorded by “citizen science” volunteers (Cooper 2016; Benedetti et al. 2018) and is likely biased by human preference for certain types of bird vocalizations (Blackburn et al. 2014). We also deposited and made available several normal “cu-koo” and three representative “cu-kee” calls (XC562704, XC562706, and XC562707) from our study site in Hungary into Xeno-Canto.

To analyse the acoustic (dis)similarity between the normal and aberrant call types recorded at our study site, we used Raven Pro 1.5 (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, USA) which includes a sound correlation feature to compute bioacoustic distance scores between two sound files (e.g. Louder et al. 2019; Hauber et al. 2020). Using this functionality, we generated similarity scores within and between the normal and aberrant calls (n = 10 similarity scores for each of the three types of comparisons) using the spectrogram correlation method.

To assess the functionality of normal vs. aberrant call types, we carried out a playback experiment from May 4 to May 18, 2020. We played cuckoo calls at points on the banks of channels where a male cuckoo’s advertisement call was just heard and the cuckoo was also seen from about 30–40 m (< 50 m). The playback equipment was set up at ca. 1.5 m of height, and the observer stayed hidden behind bushes and trees, but with a good view of the site. Sound files in wav 16-bit format were played by a Lenovo TAB 2 A7 tablet, connected with a 20-m sound cable to a JBL Xtreme (40 W) loudspeaker, and calls were broadcast at 70 dB at 1 m. It was not possible to record the data using blinded methods because our study involved clearly discriminable acoustic playbacks and simultaneously recording vocalizations of focal animals in the field.

For playbacks, we used five different playback files:

  1. (i)

    Normal advertising calls (“cu-coo”), which were frequently emitted by male common cuckoos in their breeding area (Fig. 1). Cuckoos emit this two-note call repeatedly. Previous playback studies revealed that territorial males approached the loudspeaker when a conspecific “cu-coo” call sequence was played (Moskát et al. 2017, 2018). Because the normal cuckoo call was the unmanipulated reference in this study, we used it as a control.

  2. (ii)

    Aberrant variants of the advertising calls (“cu-kee”). The first note of the “cu-coo” call was normal, and the second one was an aberrant variant, which sounded unusual to us (Fig. 1). Typically, this aberrant call type occurred in almost every year in our long-term study area, and up to 5% of the individuals were heard in the area in any given year emitting this call type (CM pers. obs.). Interestingly, most individual cuckoos that were detected with aberrant calls also emitted normal calls. This aberrant variant in call sequences was present in such individuals up to 90% of calling (CM pers. obs.).

To assess the functional components of male cuckoos’ conspecific communication signals, we also played single-note components of the full calls as follows:

  1. (iii)

    The first notes in cu-coo calls (“cu”), taken from normal “cu-coo” calls.

  2. (iv)

    The second notes in normal cuckoo calls (“coo”).

  3. (v)

    The aberrant second notes (“kee”).

We filtered out low-frequency noise (ca. below 200 kHz) and normalized for mean amplitude of all stimuli with the Audacity 2.3.3. program.

The structure of the playback files was a sequence of six notes that was repeated three times (altogether 30 s length), following by a 15-s pause. This sequence was repeated three times, without the last pause. Altogether, this playback file lasted 2 min (30-s notes +15-s pause + 30-s notes + 15-s pause and 30-s notes). All playback type files were composed similarly. We constructed 12 different files for each playback type. Because the number of recordings of aberrant calls was limited, we used recordings by an individual cuckoo 1–4 times, but always chose a different sequence of syllables to reduce potential pseudoreplication (Kroodsma 1989). For the same reason, we played every playback file only once, and we choose playback sites distant enough to be in different males’ territories (at least 500 m away). This minimal inter-trial distance was chosen based on a recent study in our population, using GPS telemetry, which revealed that female and male cuckoos are both territorial during their breeding season (Moskát et al. 2019); accordingly, we consider that potentially sampling the same male cuckoo repeatedly was negligible.

During the 2-min playback period, male cuckoos, identified by their frequent sex-specific cu-coo calls, were observed and their movements were dictated into a Tascam DR-05 sound recorder. The distances of male cuckoos were estimated after training with laser rangefinders (Bushnell Yardage Pro 800 and Suaoki 600 m). We also recorded the presence of female cuckoos (if any). In turn, female cuckoos were identified by their sex-specific bubbling calls, which were emitted less often than the male advertising calls (Deng et al. 2019b; Moskát and Hauber 2019; Xia et al. 2019); typically, females were not visible during the playback. In order to avoid the intersexual effects on the responses to our playback, we did not start the experiment at those sites where females and males were interacting, including flying together, or perching on the same branch of a tree. We measured five response variables on male cuckoos during the playbacks (Table 1). Altogether, 37 focal male cuckoos responded to our playbacks by approaching the loudspeaker during the 55 trials encompassing the five categories of call types (responses/playback trials: “cu-coo”: 12/12, “cu-kee”: 12/12, “cu”: 10/11, “coo”: 2/10, “kee”: 1/10). We also observed additional 16 male cuckoos near the speakers at these sites, where positive responses were detected. Male cuckoos frequently uttered their typical “cu-coo” calls during the playback (and also in the pre- and post-playback periods), and we easily observed and followed each of them in the semi-open habitat. In turn, female common cuckoos are more secretive in their behaviours, and never utter the sex-specific “cu-coo” call; therefore, sex-recognition of the responding individuals was not ambivalent. We detected female cuckoos in 16 out of the 55 playback trials at similar frequencies among the playback categories (χ2 = 5.556, df = 4, p = 0.235). No female showed any clear behavioural responses toward our playbacks (e.g. did not approach the speaker, etc.; Table 1). This finding agrees with other published studies that showed no female-specific attract in playbacks of the conspecific males’ “cu-coo” calls (i.e. Moskát et al. 2017; Tryjanowski et al. 2018; Xia et al. 2019).

Table 1 The list of common cuckoos’ response variables measured during the 2-min playback period

We used least-square means analysis of variance for the assessment of the biacoustic similarity scores in JMP 12.0 (SAS Institute, Cary, USA). Additional statistical tests were conducted in SPSS package ver. 17.0 (SPSS Corp., Chicago, IL) with alpha = 0.05. Specifically, binary logistic regressions and generalized linear models with the negative binomial model selection were used to assess effects on binary or continuous measures of cuckoos’ behaviour, respectively (Table 1).

Results

Our survey of the Xeno-Canto database revealed the that “cu-kee” type of aberrant call in common cuckoos is not restricted to our study site in central Hungary as we also identified it in the recordings of a single citizen scientist in Germany. His recording contained two “cu-kee” sound files, perhaps taken from the same individual cuckoo (XC244130 and XC246319).

Regarding the biacoustic similarity scores within and between call types, we found consistently high scores within both the “cu-coo” and the “cu-kee” calls, whereas the scores were significantly lower for the comparison between the two call types (F2,27 = 6.9, p = 0.004; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Acoustic similarity scores within and between “cu-coo” and “cu-kee” calls, as calculated by Raven 1.5 Pro spectrogram correlation function. Each shaded dot represents a unique data point (all n = 10 per each comparison type) and the black dot and shaded whiskers depict the mean + SE. Pairwise post hoc Tukey = comparisons are coo-coo vs. kee-coo: p = 0.0044, kee-kee vs. kee-coo: p = 0.024, and coo-coo vs. kee-kee: p = 0.77

Surprisingly, we observed statistically similar reactions of cuckoos to both of the playback types when we used either full-length normal or aberrant cuckoo calls (Fig. 3, Table S1). They typically approached the speaker, flew around it several times, perched on a nearby tree, and looked toward the source of the playback. In our second set of trials, when only a single note of the full cuckoo calls was used for playback, we obtained markedly different results. On the one hand, playbacks of the first note resulted in statistically similar responses as when the complete, two-note cuckoo calls were played (Fig. 3, Table S1). On the other hand, most of the cuckoos near the playback site showed significantly and drastically less response to the second note types, irrespective of normal or aberrant (Fig. 3, Table S1).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Male common cuckoos’ responses to playback trials with normal (“cu-coo”), aberrant (“cu-kee”), and partial (“cu”, “coo”, or “kee”) common cuckoo calls, when responses are made binary (yes/no). Significance levels were obtained by parameter estimation by the generalized linear model where the dependent variable was closest distance (NS: p > 0.05; *: p < 0.01; see also Table S1)

Binary logistic regression revealed that cuckoos’ behavioural responses (yes/no) depended on the type of the playback file (Table 2a). Another variable component of playbacks, which was the distance of the focal bird from the loudspeaker at the beginning of the playback (i.e. “starting distance”), did not co-vary with response metrics (Table 2a). Generalized linear models revealed that playback type was the only significant predictor (p < 0.001) that affected territorial male cuckoos’ approach to the speaker (“closest distance”; Table 2b, S1). This variable also similarly (p < 0.001) predicted the intensity of responses as indicated by the latency of approach (“latency”; Table 2c, S2).

Table 2 Analyses of male common cuckoos’ responses to normal (“cu-coo“, n = 12) and aberrant (“cu-kee”, n = 12) male common cuckoo calls, as well as fractions (“cu“, n = 11; “coo“, n = 10; or “kee“, n = 10) of these calls

Discussion

Our results revealed that “cu-kee” aberrant advertisement calls (Fig. 1) were acoustically distinct (Fig. 2) but contained the same communicative information for other male cuckoos as the normal “cu-coo” calls (Fig. 3). We showed that the first note of the cuckoo call functioned as the whole call, containing sufficient information to elicit responses by conspecific males. The second notes, either normal or aberrant, did not contain such information. A previous study on the two parts of the “cu-coo” call revealed that recognizing both parts was necessary for familiarity (Moskát et al. 2018). This suggests that cuckoos may use the aberrant note for familiarity or individual recognition.

In more complex environments and in complex social systems, birds typically use intricate communication systems (Peckre et al. 2019). Although common cuckoos live in diverse habitats, parasitize several host species, and exhibit active social interactions on their breeding ground (Davies 2000), they use a small vocal repertoire for acoustic communication, which is dominated by the males’ two-note “cu-coo” syllables (Tryjanowski et al. 2018; Moskát and Hauber 2019; Xia et al. 2019) over the less frequently heard female-specific bubbling call (Deng et al. 2019b; Moskát and Hauber 2019; Xia et al. 2019). The frequency of male cuckoo calls is low, ca. 500–750 Hz (Zsebök et al. 2017). This allows the “cu-coo” calls to be transmitted at high fidelity, because low-frequency sounds are less affected by small obstacles (e.g. leaves) in the field than high-frequency sounds (Slabbekoorn 2004). Consequently, cuckoo calls can be heard from 2 to 3 km away under good weather conditions (Møller et al. 2016). In turn, male common cuckoos are able to recognize subtle differences among the calls of different individuals (Jung et al. 2014; Li et al. 2017; Zsebök et al. 2017), which makes it possible to discriminate between intruding strangers and familiar neighbours in their territories (Moskát et al. 2017, 2018).

Male common cuckoos are known to vocalize using other, including aberrant forms of calls (Møller et al. 2016), but these are observed and reported rarely. Here, we studied the “cu-kee” call, which occurs in our population at low but consistent frequencies in almost each year of our research during the past 20+ years (Geltsch et al. 2017), whereas it is present in only two of a total of 1152 of common cuckoo recordings in the Xeno-Canto data base (also see above).

Overall, our study aimed to reveal whether and how animals can communicate with distorted or aberrant signals. We probed whether there was a threshold when information content was decipherable for its original function. This and other studies are needed to answer these questions on a wide range of lineages. We discovered that common cuckoos, surprisingly, could use both normal and aberrant acoustic signals for male-male communication effectively. In the future, however, new research should focus on the ontogenetic path and physiological causes that generate and produce aberrant but effective calls in this and other species to explain the biological origins of acoustic diversity in non-vocal-learning lineages.