Introduction

Parental provisioning is crucial for altricial birds because the young remain in the nest and depend on parental care for weeks after hatching. These species usually perform biparental care (Silver et al. 1985), and feeding by the male parent may significantly contribute to breeding success. Therefore, for the females, it may be vital to assess the parental skills of the potential mates already during the courtship period. Parental skills of the mate candidates may be indicated by their plumage, song and behavioural traits. Regarding plumage, numerous studies examined the correlation between colouration and feeding rate of the male parent. However, the overall support for a positive relationship between colour expression and feeding rate was not found to be convincing by a recent meta-analysis (Hegyi et al. 2015). The correlation between song traits and feeding rate of males has been rarely examined. The existing studies, however, seem to show that measures of song rate are related to feeding rate (Hofstad et al. 2002; Dolby et al. 2005), while measures of song complexity are not (Rinden et al. 2000; Hofstad et al. 2002; Dolby et al. 2005). The correlation between behavioural traits displayed during courtship and feeding rate of males, to our knowledge, has not yet been examined.

Several, non-exclusive theoretical frameworks suggest that such a relationship is likely to exist. Under the concept of phenotypic integration (Foster et al. 1992) or the existence of behavioural syndromes (Sih et al. 2004), we would expect a correlation between behaviours expressed during courtship and behaviours expressed during parental care. This is because functionally different behaviours could be governed by common genetic (Bakker 1986; van Oers et al. 2004) or physiological proximate mechanisms (Carere et al. 2003; Kralj-Fišer et al. 2007). In addition, if courtship and parental behaviour utilise the same limiting resources and are therefore traded off against each other (Stearns 1989), a correlation between these behaviours can be apparent. Based also on life-history (Stearns 1976) or pace-of-life (Ricklefs and Wikelski 2002; Réale et al. 2010) theory, parental investment is expected to correlate with behaviours of other social or ecological contexts. This is because expression of individual-specific behaviours could be coupled with the life-history of the individuals, which can be placed alongside a fast-slow continuum (Réale et al. 2010), and balanced according to the relative profit of current reproduction and residual reproductive value (Williams 1966).

A link between courtship and parental behaviour of males is also suggested indirectly by several empirical studies that found behavioural traits to play roles in mate choice (Otter and Ratcliffe 1996; Ophir and Galef 2003; Ophir et al. 2005; Kunc et al. 2006), differential allocation (Gwinner and Schwabl 2005; Grenna et al. 2014) or sex allocation (Ramsay et al. 2003; Szász et al. 2014).

One of the behaviours that seems to be likely to provide easily accessible information about future parental care is territorial aggression for the following reasons. First, aggressive disputes among males are open to be witnessed by females. In the laboratory, Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) females were willing to observe male-male confrontations, evaluated the outcome and used the information in mate choice (Ophir and Galef 2003; Ophir et al. 2005) suggesting that such a mechanism could work in the wild too. Free-living nightingale (Luscinia megarhynchos) females indeed seem to make decision on the basis of male aggressiveness. In this species, male-male confrontations are resolved vocally, and those males that overlapped with playback songs more frequently were more likely to pair up later (Kunc et al. 2006). Second, territoriality and parenting are known to be conflicting activities due to the contrasting effects of testosterone (Silverin 1980; Wingfield et al. 1987), and testosterone profile considerably varies among individuals (Kempenaers et al. 2008).

We aimed to investigate whether the aggressive behaviour of males during the courtship period predicts parental behaviour in collared flycatchers (Ficedula albicollis) making it a valuable information source during mate choice or subsequent investment of the females. Apart from its general importance for evolutionary biology, the question is particularly interesting because aggressiveness of males during the courtship period against a conspecific intruder predicted the brood sex ratio that was produced later by their breeding partner (Szász et al. 2014). Specifically, mates of aggressive males produced female-biased broods, while those of less aggressive males produced male-biased broods (Szász et al. 2014). This result suggests that females assessed the aggressiveness of their mates and adjusted their reproductive decisions accordingly. Male offsprings in our study population are more sensitive to rearing conditions than females both in terms of nestling performance (Rosivall et al. 2010) and lifetime reproductive success (Szász et al. 2017). Therefore, a negative relationship between aggressiveness of males and their later parental contribution could explain the observed brood sex ratio adjustment.

To find out whether a behaviour expressed during courtship could predict the quality of future parental care, and to better understand the role of nest-site defence aggression in sex allocation in our study population, we tested the response of territorial males to simulated intrusion and examined nestling feeding by the same males a few weeks later. We expected that the aggressive response and the feeding activity of males would be negatively related to each other.

Materials and methods

Study site and study species

Our study was conducted in a nest-box breeding population of the collared flycatcher in the Pilis-Visegrádi Mountains, Hungary (47° 43′ N, 19° 01′ E) over four breeding seasons (2013–2016). The study site is situated in a protected, middle-aged, oak-dominated forest, and consists ca. of 800 nest-boxes mainly occupied by collared flycatchers.

The collared flycatcher is a small, migratory, insectivorous, primarily socially monogamous, hole-nesting passerine that breeds in deciduous forests in Europe. Males start to arrive at the breeding site in the middle of April, and immediately start to occupy and defend nest-boxes and their surroundings, while females arrive a bit later and search for a mate (Cramp and Perrins 1993). Females lay one clutch per breeding season (except for replacement clutches) containing on average 5–7 eggs (with a range of 3–9 eggs) that hatch roughly 12 days after clutch completion. Nestlings remain in the nest for 14–16 days. The female parent incubates alone, while both parents feed the nestlings.

Behavioural assays

Nest-site defence aggression of males was quantified during the courtship period by simulated intrusion tests. The observations were performed blindly with respect to the morphological traits of the males. The standardised test protocol has been described in detail earlier (Garamszegi et al. 2006). Males singing and displaying at a particular nest-box were confronted with a caged, alive, conspecific decoy male. Tests were observed from a hiding spot in the vegetation with binoculars, and latency to attack the decoy was measured with a stopwatch. Latency to attack was the time (in seconds) elapsed between the detection of the decoy and the first intention to attack the decoy. We considered the males to detect the decoy when they re-appeared on their territory. We recorded the first intention to attack when the males first touched the cage of the decoy from any side for more than 1 s and showed clear signs of fighting propensity (such as ruffling plumage, flicking wings, jumping and pecking). Earlier, we found that latency to attack is an accurate measure of aggression in our study population because it was repeatable within a breeding season and related to other variables describing nest-site defence aggression (such as number and duration of attacks; Garamszegi et al. 2006, 2015). Males that did not attack the decoy during the test were given a score of 301 s (5 min is the maximal length of our test). All tests were done during the morning and early afternoon hours (test start time: 7:00 a.m.–2:00 p.m.) when birds were the most active. All assayed males were assumed to be unfamiliar with the decoy males because they were trapped in non-adjacent study plots. We used decoy males randomly across tests and recorded their morphology and identity (i.e. ring number). Previously, we found an effect of the size of the wing patch of the decoy on the attack latency of the resident (Garamszegi et al. 2006). However, in the respective study years (2013–2016), neither the age-standardised size of the wing patch of the decoy (General Linear Model (GLM) with year and resident binary age as fixed factors: n = 155 individuals, F(1, 149) = 0.783, p = 0.378) nor the identity of the decoy had significant effect on male territorial behaviour (log-likelihood ratio test between models containing and lacking decoy ID as random effect (resident binary age was a fixed effect): AIC = 350.71, 348.71 and log-Lik = − 171.36, −171.36, respectively; Chi2 ~ 0, p ~ 1). Therefore, we did not consider decoy morphology and identity further.

After the behavioural test, we caught the assayed males in their nest-box with traditional spring traps for ringing and measurement. We measured tarsus length with a calliper to the nearest 0.1 mm and body mass with Pesola spring balance to the nearest 0.1 g. We also measured the sizes of two white ornaments, the wing patch and the forehead patch, which are sexually selected and related to certain aspects of individual quality (Michl et al. 2002; Török et al. 2003; Garamszegi et al. 2004; Hegyi et al. 2007). To estimate wing patch size (hereafter WPS), we measured the length of the white bars on the 4th–8th primaries (Török et al. 2003) with a calliper to the nearest 0.1 mm. To estimate forehead patch size (hereafter FPS), we measured the maximum height and width of the patch (Hegyi et al. 2002) with a calliper to the nearest 0.1 mm. We also noted the age of the males based on the darkness of their remiges (brown indicates first-year and black indicates older males; Svensson 1992). After the measurements, we marked the assayed males on their belly feathers individually with ink for resighting, and released them.

Video recordings

We monitored all nesting attempts in our core study site to locate the assayed males. Resighting was done with binoculars while hiding in the vegetation either in the middle of the egg laying stage or shortly after hatching (when the hatchlings were 0–2 days old). In every year, we caught and identified approx. 40 males that were assayed for aggression (44, 39, 40 and 41 males in 2013, 2014, 2015 and 2016, respectively). We were subsequently able to follow the breeding performance of approx. half of these males in our study site (16, 21, 25 and 27 males in 2013, 2014, 2015 and 2016, respectively). Unfortunately, a large proportion of the initiated breeding attempts failed and could not be studied in every year due to complete or partial predation before or shortly after hatching (6, 12, 8, and 9 attempts in 2013, 2014, 2015 and 2016, respectively). We found that the level of nest-site defence aggression is independent of the probabilities of pairing and depredation (analysed on our long-term dataset covering 14 study years (2003–2016) and 295 individual males; Szász et al. 2019). Therefore, our video recorded sample of the assayed males should be representative of the natural range of attack latency in our study population. In our analyses, we did not consider the breeding attempt of those assayed males that bred outside of our core study site experiencing very different phenology (1 case), bred exceptionally late in the season when usually only replacement attempts occur (3 cases), were accidentally involved in an experiment because resighting was unsuccessful (2 cases) or were polygynous (2 cases).

Altogether, we had video recordings of the feeding activity of 42 assayed males and their breeding partners for the statistical analyses. Before the video recording, nests of the assayed males were undisturbed by trapping and measuring, and they were only checked to find out the date and success of hatching. We video recorded the feeding activity of the assayed males when the nestlings were 9–11 days old. The number of nestlings at recording ranged from 4 to 7 (mean ± SE = 5.7 ± 0.8). We started all records in the morning (video start time 7:26–9:13 a.m.) when feeding activity of birds peaks. The cameras were hidden in the vegetation roughly 20–30 m from the focal nest-box. Setting up and presence of the camera did not seem to cause lasting stress for the parents as they always returned to feed their nestlings within 2–3 min (mean ± SE = males: 171.6 ± 139.5 s; females: 146.0 ± 127.7 s). Moreover, some parents even continued feeding while we handled the camera. We intended to record at least 2 h of feeding (video length mean ± SE = 133 ± 21 min). Unfortunately, because of sudden rainstorms, we had to interrupt two of the videos resulting in shorter records (68 and 92 min). The inclusion of these shorter videos did not change the results qualitatively (results not shown), so we present the results for the larger dataset. After the video recording, we caught the parents in their nest-box with traditional spring taps, and measured their tarsus length, body mass, WPS and FPS in the same way as described above. Females cannot be reliably assigned to age categories based on plumage characteristics, so we determined their minimum age based on ringing history. Female parents ringed as nestlings in the previous year and ringed as adult in the respective year were assigned as first-year; otherwise, they were assigned as older. In seven cases, we failed to resight the assayed males before the standard handling procedures so their identity was only found out in hand and therefore, the video recording was done 1 or 2 days after the measurements from both parents were taken. The inclusion of these parents did not change the results qualitatively (results not shown), so we present the results for the complete dataset.

All videos were processed by the same person (ES) following a blinded protocol i.e. without knowledge on the morphology of individuals and their exact performance during the prior behavioural assays. The videos were watched at natural speed. Starting and ending time of all events when an individual was present were noted for the male and the female parent separately. Feeding events were considered to occur when the parents carried food item, and either gave it in from the entrance hole or brought it into the nest-box. When calculating feeding variables, we subtracted the time elapsed between the start of the video and the first feeding event of the individual (for the male and the female parent separately) from the whole length of the record, and calculated the number of feeding events without the first one. In case of disturbances involving the occurrence of a potential predator (either a co-worker (2 cases) or a woodpecker (6 cases)), video length and number of feeding events were also corrected by the duration of the disturbance and the first feeding event after the disturbance, respectively. Feeding rate was the corrected number of feeding events per hour and per nestling. Thereby, feeding rate was controlled for (1) individual differences in risk-avoidance, (2) video length and (3) differences in brood size.

Statistical analyses

We analysed the possible association between male aggression and nestling feeding activity in R statistical environment (version 3.1.1; R Core Team 2014) building GLM with Gaussian error (using the function “lm” of the package “lme4”; Bates et al. 2015).

The response variable was the feeding rate of males per hour per nestling. The focal explanatory variable was the attack latency of males that was log10(x + 1)-transformed to improve its distribution. We also considered the following explanatory variables. Year and calendar date of the video record compared to the yearly median calendar date of broods of 10-day-old nestlings in the study population (the mean ± SE age of the video recorded, analysed broods was 10.1 ± 0.3 days). This relative date was highly correlated with relative laying date (calendar date of the first egg; r = 0.847, p < 0.001). Binary age (first-year or older), tarsus length, body condition, WPS of each parent, FPS of the male parent and feeding rate (per hour per nestling) of the female parent were also included in the full model. Body condition was the residual of body mass/tarsus length regression in the years, and the sexes separately based on character values of the whole population. Attack latency, tarsus length, WPS and FPS were centered to the yearly mean values of the population in the sexes separately. In case of attack latency and WPS of males, centering was conducted in the age categories separately because first-year males are more aggressive during territorial challenge and bear smaller wing patches than older males (Török et al. 2003; Garamszegi et al. 2006). There was no confounding multicollinearity between the explanatory variables (all variance inflation factors < 1.454; Graham 2003; function “vif” of package “car”; Fox and Weisberg 2011). Because feeding rate was calculated per hour per nestling (see above), brood size was not considered in the models. Age of the nestlings and starting time of the video were also disregarded because they were standard across all nests (see above).

We applied backward stepwise elimination of non-significant terms (with a threshold of p > 0.05) to find the model best fitting the variation in male feeding activity. Then, the removed explanatory variables were added one-by-one to the final model to yield reliable parameter estimates (Hegyi and Laczi 2015). We present Type III Wald F tests. Applying information criterion-based model selection to find the best-fitting model yielded similar results (see Electronic supplementary material for the details).

Results

In our dataset, male collared flycatchers averaged 4.6 ± 1.3 (mean ± SE) feeding events per hour per nestling, and their breeding partners averaged the same (mean ± SE = 4.6 ± 1.3). Feeding rates of the parents did not significantly correlate with each other (r = 0.212, p = 0.178, n = 42).

Aggression during nest-site defence did not explain variance in male feeding rate. Furthermore, from the considered explanatory variables, only the year had a significant effect on male feeding rate, while all the other explanatory variables dropped out during the backward stepwise elimination procedure. Likewise, all explanatory variables turned out to be non-significant when tested in the final model (Table 1).

Table 1 Feeding rate (calculated per hour per nestling) of male collared flycatchers in relation to their level of nest-site defence aggression, their morphology and the morphology of their breeding partners (n = 42 broods)

Discussion

In a wild population of collared flycatchers, we examined whether the territorial performance of males during the courtship period could carry information about the amount of parental care they will provide. Earlier, we found that sex allocation was related to male nest-site defence aggression with female-biased offspring sex ratio in broods attended by more aggressive fathers and male-biased offspring sex ratio in broods attended by less aggressive fathers (Szász et al. 2014). Given that males are more sensitive to the quality of the rearing environment than females both in terms of nestling development (Rosivall et al. 2010) and lifetime reproductive success (Szász et al. 2017), we hypothesised that the observed sex allocation could be explained by a negative link between aggression and provisioning of the father. However, we did not find such a link as more and less aggressive fathers cared similarly for their nestlings. Though we cannot fully exclude the possibility that the relationship between aggression and provisioning is very weak and could not been detected because of the moderate sample size, there are several possible biological explanations for the lack of the hypothesised link.

The finding that more and less aggressive males provide a similar amount of parental care is in agreement with our long-term study on the fitness consequences of nest-site defence aggression (Szász et al. 2019). More and less aggressive males fledged a similar number of offspring, which recruited to the population with a similar probability (Szász et al. 2019). Together, our results suggest that in our study population, regarding reproductive success, there is no selective advantage of being either more or less aggressive in the context of nest-site competition. Some other studies also suggest that nestling feeding activity or net reproductive success could be similar across different behavioural phenotypes. For example, though problem-solver and non-solver great tit (Parus major) females differed in the spatial and temporal efficiency of foraging, their feeding rate and time spent on feeding did not differ (Cole et al. 2012). Furthermore, though problem-solvers laid larger clutches, they abandoned their nest between hatching and fledging more often than non-solvers resulting in no selective difference among them (Cole et al. 2012). Another study showed that chestnut thrush (Turdus rubrocanus) females preferred different nest-sites according to their level of stress tolerance, and those nest-sites had contrasting effects on nestling number and mass indicating no difference in net reproductive benefit of bold and shy mothers (Zhao et al. 2016). Lastly, though more aggressive blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus) males fed their nestlings at a lower rate, they elicited compensation from their breeding partners, so their reproductive success was not lower than that of less aggressive males (Mutzel et al. 2013).

It is also possible that expression of behavioural traits is considerably influenced by individual state and environmental quality, and the integration of behaviour does not carry over from one period to another. The study on western bluebirds (Sialia mexicana, Duckworth 2006) and blue tits (Mutzel et al. 2013) that found a negative relationship between male aggression and provisioning measured both behaviours on the same territory and in the same or close (respectively) time periods. In contrast, because we addressed the role of aggression in mate choice, we compared courtship behaviour with parental behaviour a month later (mean ± SE = 31.8 ± 12.1 days), and about half of the males (n = 20) eventually bred in another territory than its courtship territory, which could be responsible for the lack of the relationship. Some recent results in our study population support this idea. Escape ability of collared flycatcher males (a behavioural trait that was individual-specific within a period) declined from courtship to parental care, and the magnitude of the decline was partly explained by a change in their body condition (Jablonszky et al. 2017). Furthermore, consistency of song characteristics of males declined with change of nest-box and progress in time (Zsebők et al. 2017). However, male feeding rate was not explained by the interactive effect of latency to attack and change of nest-box (GLM with year as fixed factor: F(1, 35) = 1.059, p = 0.766) or the interactive effect of latency to attack and time elapsed between the behavioural assay and the feeding observation (GLM with year as fixed factor: F(1, 35) = 0.091, p = 0.310).

An alternative explanation for why we did not find a relationship between aggression and parental care relies on the seasonality of testosterone profile in migratory, monogamous passerines. Testosterone level is high during territory establishment and the fertile period of the social mate, and low during nestling care (Wingfield et al. 1987). This pattern is known to be advantageous, because in many species, experimentally prolonged high testosterone level encouraged territorial and courtship behaviours, while suppressed parental behaviours (Silverin 1980; Wingfield 1985; Hegner and Wingfield 1987; Ketterson et al. 1992). Under natural conditions, however, the drop in testosterone level from mating to parenting could be associated with a decline in the among-individual variance in testosterone levels (Kempenaers et al. 2008). We are not aware of any study specifically addressing this issue, but there is indication in the literature that this assumption may be right. In an experimental study in blue tits, the mean testosterone level of control males was six times lower during nestling feeding than nest building, and in parallel, the range of testosterone levels was two times lower (Foerster and Kempenaers 2004). Similarly, in black redstart (Phoenicurus ochruros) males, the mean of pre-treatment testosterone level was five times lower, and the range of pre-treatment testosterone levels was three times lower during nestling feeding than mate guarding (Villavicencio et al. 2014; C.P. Villavicencio pers. commun.). Most importantly, in the closely related pied flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca), in control males, the mean testosterone level was seven or 11 times lower during nestling feeding than before nest building depending on whether the male was unpaired or already paired when the first hormone measurement was taken, and the range of testosterone levels was four times lower (Silverin 1993). Moreover, after a simulated territorial intrusion, attacking pied flycatcher males had significantly higher testosterone levels than non-attacking pied flycatcher males before nest building, while there was no difference in the testosterone levels of attacking and non-attacking males when the intrusions were conducted in the nestling feeding period (Silverin 1993). Accordingly, differences in aggression among males during courtship may not manifest in correlated among-individual differences in nestling feeding.

The lack of the expected relationship between nest-site defence aggression and nestling provisioning does not necessarily exclude the possibility that territorial performance is under surveillance during mate choice. It is possible that females choose males with similar or complementary behavioural type, with whom they could provide more efficient parental care. In passerines exhibiting biparental care, successful breeding has shown to be influenced by the behavioural compatibility of the breeding partners (Both et al. 2005; Spoon et al. 2006; Schuett et al. 2011). Accordingly, nestling feeding activity and, in turn, reproductive success across pairs of different behavioural composition could be comparable due to effective behavioural coordination within the pairs. However, feeding activity of the breeding partners was not related to each other significantly; therefore, this possibility is not very likely in our case.

Besides territorial behaviour, the morphological traits of males or those of their breeding partners did not predict male nestling provisioning either. This is not surprising regarding that two earlier, experimental studies on parental care in our study population yielded similar results for the traits used in the present study (Kiss et al. 2013; Laczi et al. 2017). The first study was a full cross-fostering experiment and revealed a positive effect of WPS of the genetic mother on the feeding rate of both foster parents (Kiss et al. 2013). Phenotypic traits of the foster mother and father were not related to their feeding rate (Kiss et al. 2013). Therefore, it suggested that offspring innate quality rather than parental morphology determines feeding activity. Nestlings of large-patched mothers may have behaved more demandingly or conspicuously in foster nests, or may have elicited greater investment from their foster parents. The second study conducted brood size manipulation and found no effect of the patch sizes of either parent on nestling provisioning in any of the treatment groups (Laczi et al. 2017).

To summarise, aggressive performance of collared flycatcher males during territorial challenge in the courtship period does not seem to predict their future parental investment in nestling feeding. This result leaves open the question of why females apparently adjust the sex ratio of their brood to the nest-site defence aggression of their mates. More generally, future studies on other species and other behavioural traits are warranted to explore whether behavioural performances that are witnessed by females during mate choice may be indicators of future parental commitment and thereby adaptively modify female reproductive decisions.