Skip to main content
Log in

Resource availability and predation risk influence contest behavior and dominance hierarchies in crayfish

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theory-based predictions on asymmetries in animal contests have seldom been tested in situations where payoff asymmetries exist among contestants otherwise clearly matched for physical fighting capacity (i.e., resource holding potential, RHP). We examined the effects of food availability and predation risk on contest outcomes and the maintenance of dominance hierarchies in size-matched male noble crayfish pairs (Astacus astacus). Following theoretical predictions, dominant status established via contests staged in a non-resource context successfully predicted contest outcome (resource use) once resources were made available. Still, resource availability significantly increased the aggressive motivation of subordinates and sometimes resulted in hierarchy reversals. In subsequent resource-based contests, subordinates increased their fighting intensity, whereas dominants reduced their fighting motivation, potentially reflecting hunger-driven payoff asymmetries. Moreover, the data support the “asset-protection” principle by showing for the first time that predation risk lowered the aggressive motivation of dominants. At the same time, the percentage of fighting bouts won by subordinates increased significantly, leading to reversal of prior dominance relationships in 30 % of pairs. Compared to pairs where the hierarchy was retained through the different contests, pairs where dominance was reversed were characterized by smaller RHP asymmetries, which is again consistent with theory. These results show how animal contest behavior can be sensitive to the complex interactive effects of prior experience, resource value and RHP. Furthermore, in systems where repeated contests lead to dominance hierarchies, reversals within the hierarchy may occur more often when the RHP asymmetry between contestants is small.

Significance Statement

Contest outcome and resulting dominance status of contestants may be influenced by relative resource value and perceived costs involved in animal contests, especially when individuals are similar in major correlates of fighting ability, i.e., often body and weapon size. However, empirical evidence is scant. Using size-matched male crayfish pairs, we investigated the effects of food availability and predation threat on fighting behavior on individual and pair level and on hierarchy maintenance in repeated contests. Our study reveals novel evidence, indicating that crayfish are capable of changing their decisions during contests in response to food availability and predation threat, which can alter the hierarchy. For example, dominants reduced their aggressiveness in the presence of predation threat. Overall, hierarchy reversals were more likely in pairs where the difference in fighting ability between contestants was smaller.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ahvenharju T, Ruohonen K (2007) Agonistic behaviour of signal crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus Dana) in different social environments: effect of size heterogeneity on growth and food intake. Aquaculture 271:307–318. doi:10.1016/j.aquaculture.2007.05004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnott G, Elwood RW (2008) Information gathering and decision making about resource value in animal contests. Anim Behav 76:529–542. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.019

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayres-Peres L, Araujo PB, Santos S (2011) Description of the agonistic behavior of Aegla Longirostri (Decapoda: Aeglidae). J Crustac Biol 31:379–388. doi:10.1651/10-3422.1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnard C, Burk T (1979) Dominance hierarchies and the evolution of individual recognition. J Theor Biol 81:65–73. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(79)90081-X

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman DA, Moore PA (2003) Field observations of intraspecific agonistic behavior of two crayfish species, Orconectes rusticus and Orconectes virilis, in different habitats. Biol Bull 205:26–35. doi:10.2307/1543442

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman DA, Moore PA (2005) Prolonged exposure to social odours alters subsequent social interactions in crayfish (Orconectes rusticus). Anim Behav 70:311–318. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.10.026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bovbjerg RV (1953) Dominance order in the crayfish Orconectes virilis (Hagen). Physiol Zool 26:173–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briffa M (2013) Contests in crustaceans: assessments, decisions and their underlying mechanisms. In: Hardy ICW, Briffa M (eds) Animal contests. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 86–112

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Briffa M, Elwood RW (2010) Repeated measures analysis of contests and other dyadic interactions: problems of semantics, not statistical validity. Anim Behav 80:583–588. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.06.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briffa M, Hardy ICW (2013) Introduction to animal contests. In: Hardy ICW, Briffa M (eds) Animal contests. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 1–4

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Briffa M, Sneddon LU (2010) Contest behaviour. In: Westneat DE, Fox CW (eds) Evolutionary behavioral ecology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp 246–265

    Google Scholar 

  • Briffa M, Hardy ICW, Gammell MP, Jennings DJ, Clarke DD, Goubault M (2013) Analysis of animal contest data. In: Hardy ICW, Briffa M (eds) Animal contests. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 47–85

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Briffa M, Sneddon LU, Wilson AJ (2015) Animal personality as a cause and consequence of contest behaviour. Biol Lett 11:20141007. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.1007

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Buston P (2003) Social hierarchies: size and growth modification in clownfish. Nature 424:145–146. doi:10.1038/424145a

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark C (1994) Antipredator behaviour and the asset-protection principle. Behav Ecol 5:159–170. doi:10.1093/beheco/5.2.159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis KM, Huber R (2007) Activity patterns, behavioural repertoires, and agonistic interactions of crayfish: a non-manipulative field study. Behaviour 144:229–247. doi:10.1163/156853907779947300

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drews C (1993) The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour. Behaviour 125:283–313. doi:10.1163/156853993X00290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn M, Copelston M, Workman L (2004) Trade-offs and seasonal variation in territorial defence and predator evasion in the European Robin Erithacus rubecula. Ibis 146:77–84. doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.2004.00221.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Earley RL, Dugatkin LA (2006) Merging social hierarchies: effects on dominance rank in male green swordtail fish (Xiphophorus helleri). Behav Processes 73:290–298. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2006.07.001

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Earley RL, Hsu Y (2008) Reciprocity between endocrine state and contest behavior in the killifish, Kryptolebias marmoratus. Horm Behav 53:442–45. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.11.017

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards DH, Herberholz J (2005) Crustacean models of aggression. In: Nelson RJ (ed) The biology of aggression. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 38–61

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis L (1995) Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals—a cross-species comparison. Ethol Sociobiol 16:257–333. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(95)00050-U

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M (1985) Communication during aggressive interactions with particular reference to variation in choice of behaviour. Anim Behav 33:1152–1161. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80175-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Leimar O (1990) The evolution of fatal fighting. Anim Behav 39:1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Favati A, Leimar O, Lovlie H (2014) Personality predicts social dominance in male domestic fowl. PLoS One 9:e103535. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0103535

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fero K, Moore PA (2008) Social spacing of crayfish in natural habitats: what role does dominance play? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1119–1125. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0540-x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fero K, Simon JL, Jourdie V, Moore PA (2007) Consequences of social dominance on crayfish resource use. Behaviour 144:61–82. doi:10.1163/156853907779947418

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gherardi F, Atema J (2005) Memory of social partners in hermit crab dominance. Ethology 111:271–285. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01060.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gherardi F, Cioni A (2004) Agonism and interference competition in freshwater decapods. Behaviour 141:1297–1324. doi:10.1163/1568539042729702

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goessmann C, Hemelrijk C, Huber R (2000) The formation and maintenance of crayfish hierarchies: behavioural and self-structuring properties. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:418–428. doi:10.1007/s002650000222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guiasu RC, Dunham DW (1999) Agonistic contests in male form I Cambarus bartonii bartonii (Fabricius, 1798) (Decapoda, Cambaridae) crayfish and a comparison with contests of the same type in Cambarus robustus Girard, 1852. Crustaceana 72:1079–1091. doi:10.1163/156854099504031

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardy ICW, Briffa M (2013) Animal contests. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hemsworth R, Villareal W, Patullo BW, MacMillan DL (2007) Crustacean social behavioural changes in response to isolation. Biol Bull 213:187–195

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herberholz J, McCurdy C, Edwards DH (2007) Direct benefits of social dominance in juvenile crayfish. Biol Bull 213:21–27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirvonen H, Holopainen S, Lempiainen N, Selin M, Tulonen J (2007) Sniffing the trade-off: effects of eel odours on nocturnal foraging activity of native and introduced crayfish juveniles. Mar Freshw Behav Physiol 40:213–218. doi:10.1080/10236240701556919

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hock K, Huber R (2007) Effects of fighting decisions on formation and structure of dominance hierarchies. Mar Freshw Behav Physiol 40:45–61. doi:10.1080/10236240701237932

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huber R, Delago A (1998) Serotonin alters decisions to withdraw in fighting crayfish, Astacus astacus: the motivational concept revisited. J Comp Physiol A 182:573–583

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huber R, Smith K, Delago A, Isaksson K, Kravitz EA (1997) Serotonin and aggressive motivation in crustaceans: altering the decision to retreat. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:5939–5942

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Issa FA, Adamson DJ, Edwards DH (1999) Dominance hierarchy formation in juvenile crayfish Procambarus clarkii. J Exp Biol 202:3497–3506

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jakobsson S, Brick O, Kullberg C (1995) Escalated fighting behaviour incurs increased predation risk. Anim Behav 49:235–239. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(95)80172-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karavanich C, Atema J (1998) Individual recognition and memory in lobster dominance. Anim Behav 56:1553–1560. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0914

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly CD, Godin JGJ (2001) Predation risk reduces male-male sexual competition in the Trinidadian guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:95–100. doi:10.1007/s002650100410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemp DJ (2002) Sexual selection constrained by life history in a butterfly. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:1341–1345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemp DJ (2006) Ageing, reproductive value, and the evolution of lifetime fighting behaviour. Biol J Linn Soc 88:565–578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knowles RJ, Curtis TM, Crowell-Davis SL (2004) Correlation of dominance as determined by agonistic interactions with feeding order in cats. Am J Vet Res 65:1548–1556. doi:10.2460/ajvr.2004.65.1548

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivula K, Orell M, Rytkönen S (1996) Winter survival and breeding success of dominant and subordinate willow tits Parus montanus. Ibis 138:624–629. doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.1996.tb04763.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Kokko H (2013) Dyadic contests: modelling fights between two individuals. In: Briffa M (ed) Hardy ICW. Animal contests. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 5–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Kortet R, Hedrick A (2007) A behavioural syndrome in the field cricket Gryllus integer: intrasexual aggression is correlated with activity in a novel environment. Biol J Linn Soc 91:475–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaManna JR, Eason PK (2007) Effects of predator presence on territorial establishment. Behaviour 144:985–1001. doi:10.1163/156853907781871824

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magaña M, Alonso JC, Palacín C (2011) Age-related dominance helps reduce male aggressiveness in great bustard leks. Anim Behav 82:203–211. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.04.014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell SE, Delaney HD (1990) Designing experiments and analyzing data: a model comparison approach. Wadsworth, Belmont, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McGhee KE, Travis J (2010) Repeatable behavioural type and stable dominance rank in the bluefin killifish. Anim Behav 79:497–507

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore PA (2007) Agonistic behavior in freshwater crayfish. In: Duffy JE, Thiel M (eds) Evolutionary ecology of social and sexual systems. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp 90–114

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Parker G (1974) Assessment strategy and evolution of fighting behaviour. J Theor Biol 47:223–243. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(74)90111-8

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavey CR, Fielder DR (1996) The influence of size differential on agonistic behaviour in the freshwater crayfish Cherax cuspidatus (Decapoda: Parastacidae). J Zool 238:445–457. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1996.tb05405.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ranta E, Lindström K (1992) Power to hold sheltering burrows by juveniles of the signal crayfish, Pasifastacus leniusculus. Ethology 92:217–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudin FS, Briffa M (2012) Is boldness a resource-holding potential trait? Fighting prowess and changes in startle response in the sea anemone, Actinia equina. Proc R Soc B 282:1904–1910. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2418

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stocker AM, Huber R (2001) Fighting strategies in crayfish Orconectes rusticus (Decapoda, Cambaridae) differ with hunger state and the presence of food cues. Ethology 107:727–736. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00705.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tibbetts EA, Sheehan MJ, Dale J (2008) A testable definition of individual recognition. Trends Ecol Evol 23:356. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.03.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vye C, Cobb JS, Bradley T, Gabbay J, Genizi A, Karplus I (1997) Predicting the winning or losing of symmetrical contests in the American lobster Homarus americanus (Milne-Edwards). J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 217:19–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westman K, Nylund V (1984) Rapu ja ravustus. Weilin + Göös, Espoo. (In Finnish)

  • Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, and London, England

    Google Scholar 

  • Zulandt Schneider RA, Huber R, Moore PA (2001) Individual and status recognition in the crayfish, Orconectes rusticus: the effects of urine release on fight dynamics. Behaviour 138:137–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuur A, Ieno EN, Walker N, Saveliev AA, Smith GM (2009) Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Springer Science & Business Media

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the staff of the Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute Evo for obtaining the crayfish and Aki Puhka for his help in running the experiments. We are especially grateful to two anonymous referees and Darrell Kemp for their constructive comments and suggestions that helped us to improve the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christina Gruber.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was funded by the Academy of Finland (RK, CG; project no. 127398), the Emil Aaltonen foundation (RK, CG), and the Doctoral Programme in Wildlife Biology Research (LUOVA; CG).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All applicable international, national, and institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. The experiments were carried out with and within the limits of the permission (12/03) given by the committee of the Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute for animal experiments. This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

Informed consent

Informed consent was not obtained as the study did not involve human participants.

Additional information

Communicated by D. Kemp

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1

(PDF 185 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gruber, C., Tulonen, J., Kortet, R. et al. Resource availability and predation risk influence contest behavior and dominance hierarchies in crayfish. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 70, 1305–1317 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-016-2139-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-016-2139-6

Keywords

Navigation