Skip to main content
Log in

Homing tasks and distance matching tasks reveal different types of perceptual variables associated with perceiving self-motion during over-ground locomotion

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Self-motion perception refers to the ability to perceive how the body is moving through the environment. Perception of self-motion has been shown to depend upon the locomotor action patterns used to move the body through the environment. Two separate lines of enquiry have led to the establishment of two distinct theories regarding this effect. One theory has proposed that distances travelled during locomotion are perceived via higher order perceptual variables detected by the haptic perceptual system. This theory proposes that two higher order haptic perceptual variables exist, and that the implication of one of these variables depends upon the type of gait pattern that is used. A second theory proposes that self-motion is perceived via a higher order perceptual variable termed multimodally specified energy expenditure (MSEE). This theory proposes that the effect of locomotor actions patterns upon self-motion perception is related to changes in the metabolic cost of locomotion per unit of perceptually specified traversed distance. Here, we test the hypothesis that the development of these distinct theories is the result of different choices in methodology. The theory of gait type has been developed based largely on the results of homing tasks, whereas the effect of MSEE has been developed based on the results of distance matching tasks. Here we test the hypothesis that the seemly innocuous change in experimental design from using a homing task to using a distance matching task changes the type of perceptual variables implicated in self-motion perception. To test this hypothesis, we closely replicated a recent study of the effect of gait type in all details bar one—we investigated a distance matching task rather than a homing task. As hypothesized, this change yielded results consistent with the predictions of MSEE, and distinct from gait type. We further show that, unlike the effect of gait type, the effect of MSEE is unaffected by the availability of vision. In sum, our findings support the existence of two distinct types of higher order perceptual variables in self-motion perception. We discuss the roles of these two types of perceptual variables in supporting effective human wayfinding.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability statement

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  • Abdolvahab M, Carello C, Pinto C, Turvey MT, Frank TD (2015) Symmetry and order parameter dynamics of the human odometer. Biol Cybern 109(1):63–73

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banton T, Stefanucci J, Durgin F, Fass A, Proffitt D (2005) The perception of walking speed in a virtual environment. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 14(4):394–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brodoehl S, Klingner CM, Witte OW (2015) Eye closure enhances dark night perceptions. Sci Rep 5:10515

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brunec IK, Moscovitch M, Barense MD (2018) Boundaries shape cognitive representations of spaces and events. Trends Cogn Sci 22(7):637–650

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campos JL, Bülthoff HH (2012) Multimodal Integration during self motion in virtual reality. In: Murray MM, Wallace MT (eds) The neural bases of multisensory processes. CRC, Boca Raton, pp 603–627

    Google Scholar 

  • Campos JL, Butler JS, Bülthoff HH (2014) Contributions of visual and proprioceptive information to travelled distance estimation during changing sensory congruencies. Exp Brain Res 232(10):3277–3289

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carello C, Turvey M (2016) Dynamic (effortful) touch. Scholarpedia of touch. Atlantis Press, Paris, pp 227–240

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Carriot J, Brooks JX, Cullen KE (2013) Multimodal integration of self-motion cues in the vestibular system: active versus passive translations. J Neurosci 33(50):19555–19566

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chen X, McNamara TP, Kelly JW, Wolbers T (2017) Cue combination in human spatial navigation. Cogn Psychol 95:105–144

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chrastil ER, Warren WH (2014) Does the human odometer use an extrinsic or intrinsic metric? Atten Percept Psychophys 76(1):230–246

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J, Cooper P, Ono A (1963) The hare and the tortoise: a study of the tea-effect in walking and running. Acta Physiol (oxf) 21:387–393

    Google Scholar 

  • Dang S, Wu Y, Yan R, Tang H (2021) Why grid cells function as a metric for space. Neural Netw 142:128–137

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derdikman D, Whitlock JR, Tsao A, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Moser MB, Moser EI (2009) Fragmentation of grid cell maps in a multicompartment environment. Nat Neurosci 12:1325–1332

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diedrich FJ, Warren WH Jr (1995) Why change gaits? Dynamics of the walk-run transition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(1):183–202

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durgin FH, Akagi M, Gallistel CR, Haiken W (2009) The precision of locomotor odometry in humans. Exp Brain Res 193(3):429–436

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellmore TM, McNaughton BL (2004) Human path integration by optic flow. Spat Cogn Comput 4(3):255–272

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etienne AS, Jeffery KJ (2004) Path integration in mammals. Hippocampus 14(2):180–192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frissen I, Campos JL, Souman JL, Ernst MO (2011) Integration of vestibular and proprioceptive signals for spatial updating. Exp Brain Res 212(2):163–176

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glasauer S, Schneider E, Grasso R, Ivanenko YP (2007) Space–time relativity in self-motion reproduction. J Neurophysiol 97(1):451–461

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris LR, Jenkin M, Zikovitz DC (2000) Visual and non-visual cues in the perception of linear self motion. Exp Brain Res 135(1):12–21

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison SJ (2020) Human odometry with a two-legged hopping gait: a test of the gait symmetry theory. Ecol Psychol 32:58–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison SJ, Turvey MT (2009) Load affects human odometry for travelled distance but not straight-line distance. Neurosci Lett 462(2):140–143

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison SJ, Turvey MT (2019) Odometry. In: Vonk J, Shackelford T (eds) Encyclopedia of animal cognition and behavior. Springer, Cham

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison SJ, Bonnette S, Malone M (2020) For humans navigating without vision, navigation depends upon the layout of mechanically contacted ground surfaces. Exp Brain Res 238:917–930

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison SJ, Reynolds N, Bishoff B, Stergiou N (2021) Assessing the relative contribution of vision to odometry via manipulations of gait in an over-ground homing task. Exp Brain Res 239(4):1305–1316

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heft H (1996) The ecological approach to navigation: a Gibsonian perspective. In: Portugali J (ed) The construction of cognitive maps. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 105–132

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Holt KG, Wagenaar RO, Saltzman E (2010) A dynamic systems: constraints approach to rehabilitation. Braz J Phys Ther 14:446–463

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isenhower RW, Kant V, Frank TD, Pinto CM, Carello C, Turvey MT (2012) Equivalence of human odometry by walk and run is indifferent to self-selected speed. J Mot Behav 44(1):47–52

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israel I, Grasso R, Georges-Francois P, Tsuzuku T, Berthoz A (1997) Spatial memory and path integration studied by self-driven passive linear displacement. I. Basic Properties. J Neurophysiol 77(6):3180–3192

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kearns MJ (2003) The roles of vision and body senses in a homing task: the visual environment matters (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Brown University, Providence

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuo AD (2007) The six determinants of gait and the inverted pendulum analogy: a dynamic walking perspective. Hum Mov Sci 26(4):617–656

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL, Collins A, Wardell J (1987) Exploring environments by hand or foot: time-based heuristics for encoding distance in movement space. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 13:606–614

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee SA (2017) The boundary-based view of spatial cognition: a synthesis. Curr Opin Behav Sci 16:58–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee Y, Lee S, Carello C, Turvey MT (2012) An archer’s perceived form scales the “hitableness” of archery targets. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38:1125–1131

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marx E, Stephan T, Nolte A, Deutschländer A, Seelos KC, Dieterich M, Brandt T (2003) Eye closure in darkness animates sensory systems. Neuroimage 19(3):924–934

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Millikan R (1984) Language, thought and other biological categories. MIT, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Millikan R (1993) White queen psychology and other essays for Alice. MIT, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Navratilova Z, McNaughton BL (2014) Models of path integration in the hippocampal complex. In: Derdikman D, Knierim JJ (eds) Space, time and memory in the hippocampal formation. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 191–224

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Patla AE (1997) Understanding the roles of vision in the control of human locomotion. Gait Posture 5(1):54–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts TJ, Azizi E (2011) Flexible mechanisms: the diverse roles of biological springs in vertebrate movement. J Exp Biol 214(3):353–361

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders JB, Inman VT, Eberhart HD (1953) The major determinants in normal and pathological gait. J Bone Joint Surg 35(3):543–558

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz M (1999) Haptic perception of the distance walked when blindfolded. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25(3):852

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw R, Kinsella-Shaw J (1988) Ecological mechanics: a physical geometry for intentional constraints. Hum Mov Sci 7(2–4):155–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw RE, Kadar E, Sim M, Repperger DW (1992) The intentional spring: a strategy for modeling systems that learn to perform intentional acts. J Mot Behav 24(1):3–28

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shockley K, Carello C, Turvey MT (2004) Metamers in the haptic perception of heaviness and moveableness. Percept Psychophys 66(5):731–742

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan MV, Zhang S, Altwein M, Tautz J (2000) Honeybee navigation: nature and calibration of the" odometer". Science 287(5454):851–853

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT, Holt KG, Obusek J, Salo A, Kugler PN (1996) Adiabatic transformability hypothesis of human locomotion. Biol Cybern 74(2):107–115

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT, Shockley K, Carello C (1999) Affordance, proper function, and the physical basis of perceived heaviness. Cognition 73(2):B17–B26

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT, Romaniak-Gross C, Isenhower RW, Arzamarski R, Harrison SJ, Carello C (2009) Human odometry is gait-symmetry specific. Proc R Soc b Biol Sci 276:4309–4314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT, Harrison SJ, Frank TD, Carello C (2012) Human odometry verifies the symmetry perspective on bipedal gaits. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38:1014–1025

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH (1984) Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10(5):683–703

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH (2019) Non-euclidean navigation. J Exp Biol 222:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White EJ (2012) The role of multimodally specified effort in action relevant distance perception (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati

    Google Scholar 

  • White E, Shockley K, Riley MA (2013) Multimodally specified energy expenditure and action-based distance judgments. Psychon Bull Rev 20(6):1371–1377

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams MA, Galbraith C, Kane SK, Hurst A (2014) Just let the cane hit it" how the blind and sighted see navigation differently. In: Proceedings of the 16th international ACM SIGACCESS conference on computers and accessibility, pp 217–224

  • Witt JK, Riley MA (2014) Discovering your inner Gibson: reconciling action-specific and ecological approaches to perception–action. Psychon Bull Rev 21(6):1353–1370

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt JK, Stefanucci JK, Riener CR, Proffitt DR (2007) Seeing beyond the target: environmental context affects distance perception. Perception 36(12):1752–1768

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu P, Huang R, Wang J, Van Dam NT, Xie T, Dong Z et al (2014) Different topological organization of human brain functional networks with eyes open versus eyes closed. Neuroimage 90:246–255

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao M, Warren WH (2015) Environmental stability modulates the role of path integration in human navigation. Cognition 142:96–109

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was supported by the University of Nebraska at Omaha (Fund for Undergraduate Scholarly Experiences). Dr. Stergiou is supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIGMS/P20GM109090, NIA/R15AG063106, and NINDS/R01NS114282).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steven J. Harrison.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

None of the authors demonstrate any conflict of interest regarding this submission.

Additional information

Communicated by Francesco Lacquaniti.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Harrison, S.J., Reynolds, N., Bishoff, B. et al. Homing tasks and distance matching tasks reveal different types of perceptual variables associated with perceiving self-motion during over-ground locomotion. Exp Brain Res 240, 1257–1266 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-022-06337-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-022-06337-3

Keywords

Navigation