Skip to main content
Log in

Vertically arrayed stimuli and responses: transfer of incompatible spatial mapping to Simon task occurs regardless of response-device orientation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Conde et al. (Exp Brain Res 233:3313–3321, 2015) found that the Simon effect for vertically arrayed stimuli and responses was reduced after 100 prior practice trials with an incompatible mapping of the stimulus locations and responses. This finding was contrary to Vu’s (Mem Cognit 35:1463–1471, 2007) finding of no transfer effect with 72 trials of prior practice. Conde et al. proposed that the different results were due to their responses being coded as top and bottom in the frontal plane, whereas Vu’s were coded as far and near in the transverse plane. We conducted four experiments to test this possibility in which participants responded with keypresses using their thumbs on a numeric keypad held vertically (upright in the frontal plane) or horizontally (flat in the transverse plane). Experiment 1 showed that, without any prior practice, a similar sized Simon effect was obtained when the response device was oriented in the transverse plane as when it was oriented in the frontal plane. In Experiments 2 and 3 participants performed with the same device orientation in the incompatible practice and Simon transfer tasks, with orientation manipulated between-subjects in the former and within-subjects in the latter. The Simon effect was reduced in both cases, with no significant difference in transfer effect for transverse and frontal planes. In Experiment 4, the device orientation differed between the incompatible practice and Simon transfer tasks, and the Simon effect was reduced similarly across both response-device orientations. Thus, the differences between Conde et al.’s and Vu’s findings cannot be attributed to the response-device orientation. Our results are consistent with the view that people code response locations in the transverse plane as top and bottom, rather than far and near, in agreement with the terminology of “top row” and “bottom row” for computer keyboards.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ansorge U, Wühr P (2004) A response-discrimination account of the Simon effect. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:365–377

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan AH, Hoffmann ER (2012) Movement compatibility for configurations of displays located in three cardinal orientations and ipsilateral, contralateral and overhead controls. Appl Ergon 43:128–140

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Computer Hope (2017) Bottom row keys; top row keys. Downloaded from https://www.computerhope.com/jargon/t/top-row-keys.htm and https://www.computerhope.com/jargon/b/bottom-row-keys.htm

  • Conde EF, Fraga-Filho RS, Lameira AP, Mograbi DC, Riggio L, Gawryszewski LG (2015) Influence of short incompatible practice on the Simon effect: transfer along the vertical dimension and across vertical and horizontal dimensions. Exp Brain Res 233:3313–3321

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM, Deininger RL (1954) S-R compatibility: correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. J Exp Psychol 48:483–492

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iani C, Rubichi S, Gherri E, Nicoletti R (2009) Co-occurrence of sequential and practice effects in the Simon task: evidence for two independent mechanisms affecting response selection. Mem Cognit 37:358–367

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lu C-H, Proctor RW (1995) The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: a review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychon Bull Rev 2:174–207

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marini M, Iani C, Nicoletti R, Rubichi S (2011) Between-task transfer of learning from spatial compatibility to a color Stroop task. Exp Psychol 58:473–479

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markman AB, Brendl CM (2005) Constraining theories of embodied cognition. Psychol Sci 16:6–10

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ottoboni G, Iani C, Tessari A, Rubichi S (2013) Modulation of the affordance effect through transfer of learning. Q J Exp Psychol 66:2295–2302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PC Help Center (2013) What are the function keys at the top of my keyboard for? Downloaded from https://pc.net/helpcenter/answers/purpose_of_function_keys

  • Proctor RW, Lu CH (1999) Processing irrelevant location information: Practice and transfer effects in choice-reaction tasks. Mem Cognit 27:63–77

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor RW, Vu K-PL (2006) Stimulus–response compatibility principles: data, theory, and application. CRC Press, Boca Raton

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor RW, Yamaguchi M, Vu KL (2007) Transfer of noncorresponding spatial associations to the auditory Simon task. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 33:245–253

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor RW, Yamaguchi M, Zhang Y, Vu K-PL (2009) Influence of visual stimulus mode on transfer of acquired spatial associations. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 35:434–445

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor RW, Zhang Y (2010) “Mother Nature doesn’t have a bullet with your name on it”: coding with reference to one’s name or object location? J Exp Soc Psychol 46:336–343

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rigon J, Massaccesi S, Umiltà C (2011) A Simon effect for depth in three-dimensional displays. Am J Psychol 124:395–403

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon JR (1990) The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In: Proctor RW, Reeve TG (eds) Stimulus–response compatibility: an integrated perspective. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 31–86

    Google Scholar 

  • Tagliabue M, Zorzi M, Umiltà C, Bassignani F (2000) The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:648–670

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Umemura H (2015) Independent effects of 2-D and 3-D locations of stimuli in a 3-D display on response speed in a Simon task. Front Psychol 6:1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vu K-PL (2007) Influences on the Simon effect of prior practice with spatially incompatible mappings: transfer within and between horizontal and vertical dimensions. Mem Cognit 35:1463–1471

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vu K-PL, Pellicano A, Proctor RW (2005) No overall right–left prevalence for horizontal and vertical Simon effects. Percept Psychophys 67:929–938

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vu K-PL, Proctor RW (2001) Determinants of right–left and top–bottom prevalence effects for two-dimensional spatial compatibility effects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:813–828

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vu K-PL, Proctor RW, Pick DF (2000) Vertical versus horizontal spatial compatibility: right–left prevalence with bimanual keypress responses. Psychol Res 64:25–40

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Worringham CJ, Beringer DB (1989) Operator orientation and compatibility in visual-motor task performance. Ergonomics 32:387–399

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Worringham CJ, Beringer DB (1998) Directional stimulus–response compatibility: a test of three alternative principles. Ergonomics 41:864–880

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi M, Proctor RW (2012) Multidimensional vector model of stimulus–response compatibility. Psychol Rev 119:272–303

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert W. Proctor.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

This research has been conducted in compliance with ethical standards, under an approved IRB protocol from Purdue University.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhong, Q., Xiong, A., Vu, KP.L. et al. Vertically arrayed stimuli and responses: transfer of incompatible spatial mapping to Simon task occurs regardless of response-device orientation. Exp Brain Res 236, 175–185 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5116-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5116-z

Keywords

Navigation