Skip to main content

Cognitive Load Theory: What We Learn and How We Learn

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Learning, Design, and Technology

Abstract

The information that humans acquire can be divided into two categories. One category, biologically primary knowledge, is largely generic in nature leading to generic cognitive skills. It is critically important, and so we have evolved to acquire such skills without explicit tuition or conscious thought. The other category, biologically secondary knowledge, is largely domain specific, leading to domain-specific concepts and skills. This category consists of cultural knowledge that we are able to acquire but without the specific acquisition mechanisms of primary knowledge. Biologically secondary knowledge is the subject of almost all teaching and learning in educational contexts. Because we have not evolved to specifically acquire this knowledge, it is best acquired with explicit instruction and conscious effort. Cognitive load theory uses evolutionary educational psychology to determine the cognitive processes needed to acquire biologically secondary knowledge and the instructional procedures that, in accord with those cognitive processes, best facilitate learning. This chapter describes the theory and some of the more recent instructional procedures developed using the theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Baddeley, A. (1999). Human memory. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewoods Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychol Rev, 67, 380–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cogn Instr, 8, 293–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, O., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2015). The worked example effect, the generation effect, and element interactivity. J Educ Psychol, 107, 689–704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871/2003) The descent of man. London: Gibson Square

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (2007). Educating the evolved mind: Conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology. In J. S. Carlson & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on contemporary educational issues (pp. 1–99). Greenwich: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educ Psychol, 43, 179–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (2012). Evolutionary educational psychology. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 597–621). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, C.-Y., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2015). When should guidance be presented in physics instruction? Arch Sci Psychol, 3, 37–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J., & Sweller, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. J Educ Psychol, 93, 579–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educ Psychol, 38, 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, P., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential and inquiry-based teaching. Educ Psychol, 41, 75–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychol Sci, 15, 661–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leahy, W., & Sweller, J. (2011). Cognitive load theory, modality of presentation and the transient information effect. Appl Cogn Psychol, 25, 943–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. Am Psychol, 59, 14–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev, 63, 81–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes. J Educ Psychol, 87, 319–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, L., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. J Exp Psychol, 58, 193–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learn Instr, 12, 61–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1979). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H., & Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions. Cogn Psychol, 5, 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slamecka, N., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem, 4, 592–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2003). Evolution of human cognitive architecture. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 43, pp. 215–266). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous and germane cognitive load. Educ Psychol Rev, 22, 123–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2012). Human cognitive architecture: Why some instructional procedures work and others do not. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 295–325). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evol Psychol, 4, 434–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ Psychol Rev, 10, 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York, NY: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2014). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educ Psychol Rev, 26, 265–283. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9243-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, A., Leahy, W., Marcus, N., & Sweller, J. (2012). Cognitive load theory, the transient information effect and e-learning. Learn Instr, 22, 449–457. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.05.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Youssef-Shalala, A., Ayres, P., Schubert, C., & Sweller, J. (2014). Using a general problem-solving strategy to promote transfer. J Exp Psychol Appl, 20, 215–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Sweller .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing AG

About this entry

Cite this entry

Sweller, J. (2016). Cognitive Load Theory: What We Learn and How We Learn. In: Spector, M., Lockee, B., Childress, M. (eds) Learning, Design, and Technology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-17727-4_50-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-17727-4_50-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-17727-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference EducationReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Education

Publish with us

Policies and ethics