Skip to main content

Forms and Functions of Affective Synchrony

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Embodied Psychology

Abstract

The reproduction of another individual’s emotions in the self—the embodiment of perceived emotions—has been demonstrated to constitute one mechanism for emotional information processing. That is, seeing someone’s emotional expressions and using one’s own face to make the same expression helps the perceiver represent the emotion of the other. When members of a dyad mimic each other’s emotional expressions and by consequence converge in their underlying physiology over time, we say that the dyad has reached a state of affective synchrony. The present chapter brings together recent theorizing and research on physiological and expressive affective synchrony. We propose that affective synchrony serves three interrelated functions: it enables efficient information exchange, allows for interpersonal emotion regulation, and builds social bonds. We review evidence for the contexts in which affective synchrony arises, propose, and evaluate the benefits and costs of achieving these states, and end by suggesting paths for future research in this area.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 299.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abney, D., Paxton, A., Dale, R., & Kello, C. (2015). Movement dynamics reflect a functional role for weak coupling and role structure in dyadic problem solving. Cognitive Processing, 16, 325–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A. H., Bader, M., Bard, E. G., Boyle, E., Doherty, G. M., Garrod, S., et al. (1991). The HCRC map task corpus. Language and Speech, 34, 351–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atzil, S., Gao, W., Fradkin, I., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Growing a social brain. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 624–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atzil, S., Hendler, T., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Winetraub, Y., & Feldman, R. (2012). Synchrony and specificity in the maternal and the paternal brain: Relations to oxytocin and vasopressin. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 798–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahns, A. J., Crandall, C. S., Gillath, O., & Preacher, K. J. (2017). Similarity in relationships as niche construction: Choice, stability, and influence within dyads in a free choice environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 329–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C., & Broesch, J. (2012). Prepared social learning about dangerous animals in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 499–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckes, L., & Coan, J. A. (2011). Social baseline theory: The role of social proximity in emotion and economy of action. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 976–988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bietti, L. M., & Sutton, J. (2015). Interacting to remember at multiple timescales: Coordination, collaboration, cooperation and culture in joint remembering. Interaction Studies, 16, 419–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boker, S. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2002). A method for modeling the intrinsic dynamics of intraindividual variability: Recovering the parameters of simulated oscillators in multi-wave panel data. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 37, 127–160.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury, J. W., & Vehrencamp, S. L. (2011). Principles of animal communication (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busuito, A., Quigley, K. M., Moore, G. A., Voegtline, K. M., & DiPietro, J. A. (2019). In sync: Physiological correlates of behavioral synchrony in infants and mothers. Developmental Psychology, 55, 1034–1045.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, E. A. (2011). Temporal interpersonal emotion systems: The “TIES” that form relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 367–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, E. A., & Randall, A. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emotion Review, 5, 202–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Champagne, F. A. (2008). Epigenetic mechanisms and the transgenerational effects of maternal care. Frontiers in. Neuroendocrinology, 29, 386–397.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chatel-Goldman, J., Congedo, M., Jutten, C., & Schwartz, J. L. (2014). Touch increases autonomic coupling between romantic partners. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cheong, J., Molani, Z., Sadhukha, S., & Chang, L. J. (under review). Synchronized affect in shared experiences strengthens social connection.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coan, J. A. (2008). Toward a neuroscience of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 241–265). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coan, J. A., & Sbarra, D. A. (2015). Social baseline theory: The social regulation of risk and effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 87–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lending a hand social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychological Science, 17, 1032–1039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corner, G. W., Saxbe, D. E., Chaspari, T., Rasmussen, H. F., Perrone, L., Pettit, C., et al. (2019). Compassion in a heartbeat: Physiology during couples’ loss discussions. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 36, 1671–1694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crane, A. L., Bairos-Novak, K. R., Sacco, L. H., & Ferrari, M. C. O. (2018). The socially mediated recovery of a fearful fish paired with periodically replaced calm models. Proceedings of the Royal Society b: Biological Sciences, 285, 20180739.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Danyluck, C., & Page-Gould, E. (2019). Social and physiological context can affect the meaning of physiological synchrony. Scientific Reports, 9, 8222.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Derex, M., & Boyd, R. (2016). Partial connectivity increases cultural accumulation within groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 2982–2987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckland, N. S., Leyro, T. M., Mendes, W. B., & Thompson, R. J. (2019). The role of physiology and voice in emotion perception during social stress. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43, 493–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. (2002). Facial action coding system. Manual and Investigator’s Guide. Salt Lake City, UT Research Nexus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Favreau, A., Richard-Yris, M. A., Bertin, A., Houdelier, C., & Lumineau, S. (2009). Social influences on circadian behavioural rhythms in vertebrates. Animal Behaviour, 77, 983–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. (2007). Maternal-infant contact and child development: Insights from the kangaroo intervention. In L. L'Abate (Ed.), Low-cost approaches to promote physical and mental health: Theory, research, and practice (p. 323–351). New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. (2011). Maternal touch and the developing infant. In M. J. Hertenstein & S. J. Weiss (Eds.), The handbook of touch: Neuroscience, behavioral, and health perspectives (pp. 373–407). Springer Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. (2012). Bio-behavioral synchrony: A model for integrating biological and microsocial behavioral processes in the study of parenting. Parenting, 12, 154–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. (2014). Synchrony and the neurobiological basis of social affiliation. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Mechanisms of social connection: From brain to group (pp. 145–166). American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Singer, M., & Louzoun, Y. (2011). Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 569–577.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fellner, W., Bauer, G. B., & Harley, H. E. (2006). Cognitive implications of synchrony in dolphins: A review. Aquatic Mammals, 32, 511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feniger-Schaal, R., Noy, L., Hart, Y., Koren-Karie, N., Mayo, A. E., & Alon, U. (2016). Would you like to play together? Adults’ attachment and the mirror game. Attachment & Human Development, 18, 33–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer, E., & Helm, J. L. (2013). Dynamical systems modeling of physiological coregulation in dyadic interactions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 88, 296–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T., Healy, B. T., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990). Behavior-state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26, 7–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2015). A duet for one. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 390–405.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Galbusera, L., Finn, M. T. M., Tschacher, W., & Kyselo, M. (2019). Interpersonal synchrony feels good but impedes self-regulation of affect. Scientific Reports, 9, 14691.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Killingsworth, M. A., Eyre, R. N., & Wilson, T. D. (2009). The surprising power of neighborly advice. Science, 323, 1617–1619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golland, Y., Mevorach, D., & Levit-Binnun, N. (2019). Affiliative zygomatic synchrony in co-present strangers. Scientific Reports, 9, 3120.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heerey, E. A., & Crossley, H. M. (2013). Predictive and reactive mechanisms in smile reciprocity. Psychological Science, 24, 1446–1455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helm, J. L., Sbarra, D., & Ferrer, E. (2012). Assessing cross-partner associations in physiological responses via coupled oscillator models. Emotion, 12, 748–762.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, M. A. (1971). Cardiac rate regulated by nutritional factor in young rats. Science, 172, 1039–1041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. C., Jong, J., Bilkey, D., Whitehouse, H., Zollmann, S., McNaughton, C., et al. (2018). Synchrony and physiological arousal increase cohesion and cooperation in large naturalistic groups. Scientific Reports, 8, 127.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, O., & Wheatley, T. (2017). Pupil dilation patterns spontaneously synchronize across individuals during shared attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146, 569–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotions in early development (pp. 57–86). Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action: Theory and data. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 54, 59–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koban, L., Ramamoorthy, A., & Konvalinka, I. (2019). Why do we fall into sync with others? Interpersonal synchronization and the brain’s optimization principle. Social Neuroscience, 14, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karvonen, A., Kykyri, V. L., Kaartinen, J., Penttonen, M., & Seikkula, J. (2016). Sympathetic nervous system synchrony in couple therapy. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 42, 383–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L. (2013). Behavioral mimicry and interpersonal synchrony. In J. A. Hall & M. L. Knapp (Eds.), Nonverbal communication (pp. 539–575). De Gruyter Mouton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Launay, J., Tarr, B., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large-scale human social bonding. Ethology, 122, 779–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, V., Byrne, E., Clackson, K., Georgieva, S., Lam, S., & Wass, S. (2017). Speaker gaze increases information coupling between infant and adult brains. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201702493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1983). Marital interaction: Physiological linkage and affective exchange. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 587–597.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Likowski, K. U., Weyers, P., Seibt, B., Stöhr, C., Pauli, P., & Mühlberger, A. (2011). Sad and lonely? Sad mood suppresses facial mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35, 101–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, S., Rovine, M. J., Cousino Klein, L., & Almeida, D. M. (2013). Synchrony of diurnal cortisol pattern in couples. Journal of Family Psychology, 27, 579–588.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G., & Jeuniaux, P. (2012). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cognitive Science, 36, 1404–1426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Main, A., Paxton, A., & Dale, R. (2016). An exploratory analysis of emotion dynamics between mothers and adolescents during conflict discussions. Emotion, 16, 913–928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marci, C. D., Ham, J., Moran, E., & Orr, S. P. (2007). Physiologic correlates of perceived therapist empathy and social-emotional process during psychotherapy. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 103–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. D., Abercrombie, H. C., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2018). Functionally distinct smiles elicit different physiological responses in an evaluative context. Scientific Reports, 8, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayo, O., & Gordon, I. (2020). In and out of synchrony-Behavioral and physiological dynamics of dyadic interpersonal coordination. Psychophysiology, e13574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (2011). Shared emotions and joint action. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2, 355–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, L. K., Griffiths, J. L., Richardson, M. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 52–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitkidis, P., McGraw, J. J., Roepstorff, A., & Wallot, S. (2015). Building trust: Heart rate synchrony and arousal during joint action increased by public goods game. Physiology & Behavior, 149, 101–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mønster, D., Håkonsson, D. D., Eskildsen, J. K., & Wallot, S. (2016). Physiological evidence of interpersonal dynamics in a cooperative production task. Physiology & Behavior, 156, 24–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulder, R. G., Boker, S. M., Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2018). Determining synchrony between behavioral time series: An application of surrogate data generation for establishing falsifiable null-hypotheses. Psychological Methods, 23, 757–773.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, B. A., Costello, T. H., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2018). Is empathic contagion helpful or harmful? Overlooked heterogeneity in the Empathy Index. Psychological Assessment, 30, 1703–1708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noy, L., Dekel, E., & Alon, U. (2011). The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 20947–20952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oullier, O., de Guzman, G. C., Jantzen, K. J., Lagarde, J., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2008). Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding. Social Neuroscience, 3, 178–192.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 711–729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palumbo, R. V., Marraccini, M. E., Weyandt, L. L., Wilder-Smith, O., McGee, H. A., Liu, S., et al. (2017). Interpersonal autonomic physiology: A systematic review of the literature. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21, 99–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Papp, L. M., Pendry, P., Simon, C. D., & Adam, E. K. (2013). Spouses’ cortisol associations and moderators: Testing physiological synchrony and connectedness in everyday life. Family Process, 52, 284–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, A. (2009). Dynamic emotional processing in experiential therapy: Two steps forward, one step back. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 113–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paxton, A., & Dale, R. (2013). Argument disrupts interpersonal synchrony. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2092–2102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, C. L. (2003). Coregulation in mother-infant dyads: Links to infants’ cardiac vagal tone. Psychological Reports, 92, 307–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2006). Synchrony: A core concept for a constructivist approach to psychotherapy. Constructivism in the Human Sciences, 11, 150–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 284–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychological Science, 9, 45–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, R. G., Barnard, K., & Butler, E. A. (2015). Distinguishing emotional co-regulation from co-dysregulation: An investigation of emotional dynamics and body-weight in romantic couples. Emotion, 15, 45–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 68, 252–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H., & Su, Y.-H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 403–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26, 867–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riehle, M., Kempkensteffen, J., & Lincoln, T. M. (2017). Quantifying facial expression synchrony in face-to-face dyadic interactions: Temporal dynamics of simultaneously recorded facial EMG signals. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 41, 85–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reich, C. M., Berman, J. S., Dale, R., & Levitt, H. M. (2014). Vocal synchrony in psychotherapy. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33, 481–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of human performance: Reaction processes and attention in human skill. Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxbe, D. E., Beckes, L., Stoycos, S. A., & Coan, J. A. (2020). Social allostasis and social allostatic load: A new model for research in social dynamics, stress, and health. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15, 469–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxbe, D., & Repetti, R. L. (2010). For better or worse? Coregulation of couples’ cortisol levels and mood states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 92–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schanberg, S. M., Ingledue, V. F., Lee, J. Y., Hannun, Y. A., & Bartolome, J. V. (2003). PKC alpha mediates maternal touch regulation of growth-related gene expression in infant rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 1026–1030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 227–247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. C., & O’Brien, B. (1997). Evaluating the dynamics of unintended interpersonal coordination. Ecological Psychology, 9, 189–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. (2007). Multiple dopamine functions at different time courses. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 259–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuyler, B. S., Kral, T. R. A., Jacquart, J., Burghy, C. A., Weng, H. Y., Perlman, D. M., et al. (2014). Temporal dynamics of emotional responding: Amygdala recovery predicts emotional traits. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 176–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 70–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seth, A. K., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Extending predictive processing to the body: Emotion as interoceptive inference. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 227–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sharon-David, H., Mizrahi, M., Rinott, M., Golland, Y., & Birnbaum, G. E. (2018). Being on the same wavelength: Behavioral synchrony between partners and its influence on the experience of intimacy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 0265407518809478. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407518809478

  • Skewes, J. C., Skewes, L., Michael, J., & Konvalinka, I. (2015). Synchronised and complementary coordination mechanisms in an asymmetric joint aiming task. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 551–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, A. L., Osnaya, A., Patel, B., & Perry, S. P. (2019). Mimicking others’ nonverbal signals is associated with increased attitude contagion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00322-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. T. (2003). Expression of emotion in nonhuman animals. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Series in affective science. Handbook of affective sciences (p. 457–480). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suveg, C., Shaffer, A., & Davis, M. (2016). Family stress moderates relations between physiological and behavioral synchrony and child self-regulation in mother–preschooler dyads. Developmental Psychobiology, 58, 83–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, G. J., Silbert, L. J., & Hasson, U. (2010). Speaker–listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 14425–14430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamborini, R., Novotny, E., Prabhu, S., Hofer, M., Bente, G., Grall, C., et al. (2018). The effect of behavioral synchrony with black or white virtual agents on outgroup trust. Computers in Human Behavior, 83, 176–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorson, K. R., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2018). Measuring physiological influence in dyads: A guide to designing, implementing, and analyzing dyadic physiological studies. Psychological Methods, 23, 595–616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tschacher, W., & Meier, D. (2019). Physiological synchrony in psychotherapy sessions. Psychotherapy Research, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behavior. Developmental Science, 7, 261–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11, 262–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallacher, R. R., Nowak, A., & Zochowski, M. (2005). Dynamics of social coordination: The synchronization of internal states in close relationships. Interaction Studies, 6, 35–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wass, S. V., Smith, C. G., Clackson, K., Gibb, C., Eitzenberger, J., & Mirza, F. U. (2019). Parents mimic and influence their infant’s autonomic state through dynamic affective state matching. Current Biology, 29, 2415–2422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, S. F., West, T. V., Karnilowicz, H. R., & Mendes, W. B. (2017). Affect contagion between mothers and infants: Examining valence and touch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146, 1043–1051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, S. F., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2014). Stress contagion: Physiological covariation between mothers and infants. Psychological Science, 25, 934–942.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, T. V., Koslov, K., Page-Gould, E., Major, B., & Mendes, W. B. (2017). Contagious anxiety: Anxious European Americans can transmit their physiological reactivity to African Americans. Psychological Science, 28, 1796–1806.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatley, T., Kang, O., Parkinson, C., & Looser, C. E. (2012). From mind perception to mental connection: Synchrony as a mechanism for social understanding. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6, 589–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woltering, S., Lishak, V., Elliott, B., Ferraro, L., & Granic, I. (2015). Dyadic attunement and physiological synchrony during mother-child interactions: An exploratory study in children with and without externalizing behavior problems. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 37, 624–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, A., Rychlowska, M., Korb, S., & Niedenthal, P. (2016). Fashioning the face: Sensorimotor simulation contributes to facial expression recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 227–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yun, K., Watanabe, K., & Shimojo, S. (2012). Interpersonal body and neural synchronization as a marker of implicit social interaction. Scientific Reports, 2, 959.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, F., Wood, A., Mutlu, B., & Niedenthal, P. (under review). Faces synchronize when communication through spoken language is prevented.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adrienne Wood .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Wood, A., Lipson, J., Zhao, O., Niedenthal, P. (2021). Forms and Functions of Affective Synchrony. In: Robinson, M.D., Thomas, L.E. (eds) Handbook of Embodied Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-78471-3_17

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics