Skip to main content

Reading and Spelling Skills in Transparent Orthographies: Phonological Encoding and Rapid Automatized Naming in Turkish

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Reading-Writing Connections

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 19))

Abstract

The aim of the present paper was to investigate the influence of phonological encoding (PE) and rapid automatized naming (RAN) on word reading and word spelling in Turkish. Seventy-seven students attending Grade 3 and Grade 4 participated in the study. Tests of word reading, word spelling (dictation), PE and RAN were administered in order to examine the relationships among the variables. The results showed that both RAN and PE were significant predictors of word reading and word spelling above and beyond grade level. Correlational analyses revealed that as the grade level increased, RAN demonstrated stronger correlations with word reading and word spelling while the influence of PE on these two measures tended to decrease. The findings are discussed in terms of the development of phonological and orthographic processing with reference to the transparent orthography of the Turkish language.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Non-linear transformations were applied by using reflect and logarithm formula both for PE and word spelling tests.

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aidinis, A., & Nunes, T. (2001). The role of different levels of phonological awareness in the development of reading and spelling in Greek. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 145–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albuquerque, C. P. (2012). Rapid naming contributions to reading and writing acquisition of European Portuguese. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 775–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aşlıyan, R., Günel, K., & Filiz, A. (2006). Türkçe otomatik heceleme sistemi ve hece istatistikleri. Paper presented at Akademik Bilişim’06 + BilgiTek IV, Pamukkale Üniversitesi, Denizli.

    Google Scholar 

  • Babayiğit, S. (2009). Reading and spelling development in transparent alphabetic orthographies: Points of convergence, divergence and arising issues. In C. Wood & V. Connelly (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on reading and spelling (pp. 133–148). Routledge: Park Square.

    Google Scholar 

  • Babayiğit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2007). Preliterate phonological awareness and early literacy skills: Evidence from Turkish. Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 394–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babayiğit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2010). Component processes of early reading, spelling and narrative writing skills in Turkish: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 539–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakır, H., & Babür, N. (2009). Türkçe Hızlı Otomatik İsimlendirme (HOTI) Testleri’nin geliştirilmesi ve ön bulgular (Development of tests for Turkish Rapid Automatized Naming: Preliminary results). Paper presented at 18. National Congress of Educational Sciences, Ege University, İzmir.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourassa, D. C., & Treiman, R. (2014). Spelling development and disability in English. In C. A. Stone, E. R. Silliman, B. J. Ehren, & G. P. Wallach (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders (2nd ed., pp. 569–583). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1993). Text reading and rereading: Determinants of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 133–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (2002). Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6(1), 51–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dos Santos, M. T. M., & Befi-Lopes, D. M. (2012). Vocabulary, phonological awareness and rapid naming: Contributions for spelling and written production. Jornal da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 24(3), 269–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoğlu, A. (2006). How language characteristics influence Turkish literacy development. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 219–229). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoğlu, A. Y., & Öney, B. (1999). A cross-linguistic comparison of phonological awareness and word recognition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 281–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107–143). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. C., & Hooper, A. M. (2001). It is easier to learn to read in Welsh than in English: Effects of orthographic transparency demonstrated using frequency-matched cross-linguistic reading tests. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 22, 571–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: A comparison between Scandinavian and U.S./Australian children. Dyslexia, 16, 119–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentry, J. R. (2004). The science of spelling: The explicit specifics that make great readers and writers (and spellers!). Portsmouth: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency in English and Greek: A cross-linguistic comparison. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Torppa, M., Manolitsis, G., Lyytinen, H., & Parrila, R. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 321–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2016). The anatomy of the RAN-reading relationship. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29, 1793–1815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Siegel, L. S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J., McIntosh, D., & Huffman, L. (2004). The role of phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming, and orthographic processing in word reading. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 22, 233–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, R., & Aaron, P. G. (2005). Spelling: Its development, assessment, instruction and the science of it. Perspectives, 31, 3–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Speed of processing, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30, 949–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemp, N. (2009). The acquisition of spelling patterns: Early, late or never? In C. Wood & V. Connelly (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on reading and spelling (pp. 76–91). Park Square: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I., Liberman, A. M., Mattingly, I., & Shankweiler, D. (1980). Orthography and the beginning reader. In J. F. Kavanagh & R. L. Venezky (Eds.), Orthography, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 137–153). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, G. W., & Cornwall, A. (1995). The relationship between phonological awareness and reading and spelling achievement eleven years later. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 523–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. A., & Foy, J. G. (2003). Phonological awareness, speech development, and letter knowledge in preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 149–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic segmentation, not onset–rime segmentation, predicts early reading and spelling skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 154–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2006). The cognitive bases of learning to read and spell in Greek: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 427–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öney, B., & Durgunoğlu, A. Y. (1997). Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 18, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, T. C., Georgiou, G., & Kendeou, P. (2009a). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in Greek: Findings from a longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 528–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G., & Kendeou, P. (2009b). The dimensionality of phonological abilities in Greek. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 127–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G., & Georgiou, G. K. (2016). How is RAN related to reading fluency? A comprehensive examination of the prominent theoretical accounts. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1997). The psycholinguistics of reading and spelling. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 21–38). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2003). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic sensitivity and naming speed in the reading and spelling performance of first-grade children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 287–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., & Stuart, M. (2014). Deficits in orthographic knowledge in childrenp at rapid automatized naming (RAN) tasks? Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(3), 192–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R., & Frederickson, N. (2005). Beyond phonology: What else is needed to describe the problems of below-average readers and spellers? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 399–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R. S., Pillay, V., & Melidona, S. (2008). Rapid serial naming is a unique predictor of spelling in children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(3), 235–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scharer, P., & Zutell, J. (2013). The development of spelling. In J. Larson & J. Marsh (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of early childhood literacy (pp. 448–483). London: SAGE Publications Ltd.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smythe, I., Everatt, J., Al-Menaye, N., He, X., Capellini, S., Gyarmathy, E., et al. (2008). Predictors of word-level literacy amongst Grade 3 children in five diverse languages. Dyslexia, 14, 170–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Powell, D., Quinlan, P., & Garwood, H. (2010). Visual processing deficits in children with slow RAN performance. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 266–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stainthorp, R., Powell, D., & Stuart, M. (2013). The relationship between rapid naming and word spelling in English. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(4), 371–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 402–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Bourassa, D. C. (2000). The development of spelling skill. Topics in Language Disorders, 20(3), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhagen, W., Aarnoutse, C., & Van Leeuwe, J. (2010). Spelling and word recognition in grades 1 and 2: Relations to phonological awareness and naming speed in Dutch children. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 31, 59–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (2000). The question of naming-speed deficits in developmental reading disability: An introduction to the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 322–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. B. (2005). Rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating stimulus tests (RAN/RAS). Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study is part of the first author’s MA dissertation completed at Boğaziçi University. We would like to thank the school principal, the teachers, and the participating students who cooperated with us during data collection in a state school in İstanbul. Without their support, it would not have been possible for us to conduct this study. Portions of this study were supported by a grant from TÜBİTAK (Turkish Scientific and Technological Research Council, project number 115K023) to the senior authors (Babür, Haznedar and Erçetin). Earlier versions of this study were presented at three conferences – the 29th National Conference on Turkish Linguistics (2015) in Kocaeli, the 7th World Conference on Psychology, Counseling and Guidance (2016) in Kuşadası, The Child Language Symposium (2018) at Reading University, UK. We would like to extend our appreciation to the audience therein and the anonymous reviewers for their valuable feedback.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Belma Haznedar .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Candan, E., Babür, N., Haznedar, B., Erçetin, G. (2020). Reading and Spelling Skills in Transparent Orthographies: Phonological Encoding and Rapid Automatized Naming in Turkish. In: Alves, R.A., Limpo, T., Joshi, R.M. (eds) Reading-Writing Connections. Literacy Studies, vol 19. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38811-9_12

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38811-9_12

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-38810-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-38811-9

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics