Skip to main content
Log in

A mixed-effects expectancy-valence model for the Iowa gambling task

  • Society for Computers in Psychology
  • Published:
Behavior Research Methods Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Iowa gambling task (IGT; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994) was developed to simulate real-life decision making under uncertainty. The task has been widely used to examine possible neurocognitive deficits in normal and clinical populations. Busemeyer and Stout (2002) proposed the expectancy-valence (EV) model to explicitly account for individual participants’ repeated choices in the IGT. Parameters of the EV model presumably measure different psychological processes that underlie performance on the task, and their values may be used to differentiate individuals across different populations. In the present article, the EV model is extended to include both fixed effects and subject-specific random effects. The mixed-effects EV model fits the nested structure of observations in the IGT naturally and provides a unified procedure for parameter estimation and comparisons among groups of populations. We illustrate the utility of the mixed-effects approach with an analysis of gender differences using a real data set. A simulation study was conducted to verify the advantages of this approach.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., & Damasio, H. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (Part I): Impaired activation of somatic states in substance-dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1675–1689.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boeka, A. G., & Lokken, K. L. (2006). The Iowa gambling task as a measure of decision making for women with bulimia nervosa. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 741–745. doi:10.10170S1355617706060887

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolla, K. I., Eldreth, D. A., Matochik, J. A., & Cadet, J. L. (2004). Sex-related differences in a gambling task and its neural correlates. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1226–1232. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh083

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Busemeyer, J. R., & Stout, J. C. (2002). A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: Decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task. Psychological Assessment, 14, 252–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C., Patte, K., Tweed, S., & Curtis, C. (2007). Personality traits associated with decision-making deficits. Personality & Individual Differences, 42, 279–290. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, L., & Rovine, M. J. (2007). Multilevel models for experimental psychologists: Foundations and illustrative examples. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 101–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isella, V., Mapelli, C., Morieli, N., Pelati, O., Franceschi, M., & Appollonio, I. M. (2008). Age-related qualitative and quantitative changes in decision making ability. Behavioral Neurology, 19, 59–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., & Hocevar, D. (1985). Application of confirmatory factor analysis to the study of self-concept: First- and higher-order factor models and their invariance across groups. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 562–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, R. (1971). Algorithm AS 47: Function minimization using a simplex procedure. Applied Statistics, 20, 338–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (1995). Approximations to the loglikelihood function in the nonlinear mixed-effects model. Journal of Computational & Graphical Statistics, 4, 12–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects model in S and S-Plus. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team (2006). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software and manual]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 803–814.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rouder, J. N., Lu, J., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., & Jiang, Y. (2005). A hierarchical model for estimating response time distributions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 195–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAS Institute, Inc. (2004). SAS/STAT user’s guide (Version 9.1). Cary, NC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheu, C.-F., Chen, C.-T., Su, Y.-H., & Wang, W.-C. (2005). Using SAS PROC NLMIXED to fit item response theory models. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 202–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheu, C.-F., Lee, Y.-H., & Shih, P.-Y. (2008). Analyzing recognition performance with sparse data. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 722–727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stockard, J., O’Brien, R. M., & Peters, E. (2007). The use of mixed models in a modified Iowa gambling task and a prisoner’s dilemma game. Judgment & Decision Making, 2, 9–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, J. C., Busemeyer, J. R., Lin, A., Grant, S. J., & Bonson, K. R. (2004). Cognitive modeling analysis of the decision-making processes used by cocaine abusers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 742–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, R., Lasthuis, W., den Heijer, E., van der Harst, J., & Spruijt, B. (2006). Toward a rodent model of the Iowa gambling task. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 470–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verdejo-Garcia, A., Benbrook, A., Funderburk, F., David, P., Cadet, J. L., & Bolla, K. I. (2007). The differential relationship between cocaine use and marijuana use on decision-making performance over repeat testing with the Iowa gambling task. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 90, 2–11. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.02.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wetzels, R., Vandekerckhove, J., Tuerlinckx, F., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2008, July). Bayesian parameter estimation in the expectancy valence model of the Iowa gambling task. Paper presented at the 41st Mathematical Psychology Meeting, Washington, DC.

  • Wolfinger, R. D. (1999). Fitting nonlinear mixed models with the new NLMIXED procedure. In Proceedings of the 24th Annual SAS Groups International Conference (SUGI24) (Paper 287). Gary, NC: SAS Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yechiam, E., Busemeyer, J. R., Stout, J. C., & Bechara, A. (2005). Using cognitive models to map relations between neuropsychological disorders and human decision making deficits. Psychological Science, 16, 973–978. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01646.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chung-Ping Cheng.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cheng, CP., Sheu, CF. & Yen, NS. A mixed-effects expectancy-valence model for the Iowa gambling task. Behavior Research Methods 41, 657–663 (2009). https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.3.657

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.3.657

Keywords

Navigation