Skip to main content
Log in

Evidence That Habit Moderates the Implicit Belief-Behavior Relationship in Health Behaviors

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

Theory suggests that implicit beliefs are more likely to be associated with spontaneous, impulsive health behaviors (e.g., smoking, snacking), while controlled, consciously accessible beliefs are more likely to be associated with behaviors that require deliberation and reasoned decision-making (e.g., physical activity, healthy food selection). Consistent with these predictions, we proposed that as behaviors become habitual, they require less deliberation to enact and are thus more likely to be controlled by non-conscious processes, as indicated by stronger associations with implicit beliefs. The present study tested the moderating effect of habit on the effects of implicit beliefs on two health behaviors.

Method

Two samples of university students completed measures of attitude, habit, and implicit beliefs for free-sugar intake (sample 1) and heavy episodic drinking (sample 2) at an initial time point, with follow-up behavioral measures taken at 2 and 4 weeks later, respectively.

Results

Path analyses indicated that attitude and habit predicted behavior in both samples, and habit moderated the implicit belief-behavior relationship in both samples. The effect of implicit beliefs on behavior was larger among participants reporting strong habits. Implicit beliefs did not moderate the effect of explicit attitudes on behavior.

Conclusion

Findings provide preliminary evidence that experiencing health behaviors as habitual is associated with a stronger implicit beliefs-health behavior relationship.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

References

  1. Hagger MS, Cameron L, Hamilton K, Hankonen N, Lintunen T. Handbook of behavior change. New York, NY: New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2020.

  2. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991;50:179–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Armitage CJ, Conner M. Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analytic review. Br J Soc Psychol. 2001;40:471–99. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466601164939.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Evans JSBT, Stanovich KE. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8:223–41. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Verplanken B, Aarts H. Habit, attitude, and planned behaviour: is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity? Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 1999;10:101–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779943000035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Hagger MS, Hamilton K. Changing behaviour using integrated theories. In: Hagger MS, Cameron LD, Hamilton K, Hankonen N, Lintunen T, editors. Handbook of behavior change. Cambridge University Press; 2020.

  7. Deutsch R, Strack F. Changing behaviour using the reflective-impulsive model. In: Hagger MS, Cameron LD, Hamilton K, Hankonen N, Lintunen T, editors. Handbook of behavior change. Cambridge University Press; 2020.

  8. Gardner B, Rebar A, Lally P. Habit interventions. In: Hagger MS, Cameron K, Hamilton K, Hankonen N, Lintunen T, editors. The Handbook of Behavior Change. Cambridge University Press; 2020.

  9. Strack F, Deutsch R. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2004;8:220–47. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hagger MS, Trost N, Keech JJ, Chan DKC, Hamilton K. Predicting sugar consumption: application of an integrated dual-process, dual-phase model. Appetite. 2017;116:147–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Caudwell KM, Keech JJ, Hamilton K, Mullan BA, Hagger MS. Reducing alcohol consumption during pre-drinking sessions: testing an integrated behaviour-change model. Psychol Health 2019:1–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2018.1518527.

  12. Phipps DJ, Hagger MS, Hamilton K. Predicting limiting ‘free sugar’ consumption using an integrated model of health behavior. Appetite 2020;150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104668.

  13. Phipps DJ, Hannan TE, Rhodes RE, Hamilton K. A dual process model of affective and instrumental attitudes in predicting physical activity. Psychol Sport Exerc 2021:101899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2021.101899.

  14. Hamilton K, Gibbs I, Keech JJ, Hagger MS. Reasoned and implicit processes in heavy episodic drinking: An integrated dual‐process model. Br J Health Psychol. 2020;25:189–209.

  15. Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann EL, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-Analysis of predictive validity. J Pers Soc Psychol 2009;97:17–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575.

  16. Brown DJ, Hagger MS, Hamilton K. The mediating role of constructs representing reasoned-action and automatic processes on the past behavior-future behavior relationship. Soc Sci Med. 2020;258:113085.

  17. Gardner B, de Bruijn G-J, Lally P. A systematic review and meta-analysis of applications of the self-report habit index to nutrition and physical activity behaviours. Ann Behav Med. 2011;42:174–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-011-9282-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Hofmann W, Gawronski B, Gschwendner T, Le H, Schmitt M. A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005;31:1369–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205275613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Fazio RH. Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: the MODE model as an integrative framework. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1990;23:75–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60318-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Perugini M, Richetin J, Zogmaister C. Prediction of behavior. In: Gawronski B, Payne K, editors. Handbook of implicit social cognition: measurement, theory, and applications, vol. 10. New York, NY. Guilford Press: US; 2010. p. 255–78.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Mazar A, Wood W. Defining habit in psychology. In: Verplanken B, editor. The psychology of habit. Springer; 2018. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-97529-0.

  22. Janssen T, Wood MD, Larsen H, Peeters M, Vollebergh WAM, Wiers RW. Investigating the joint development of approach bias and adolescent alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clin Exp Res 2015;39:2447–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.12899.

  23. Verplanken B, Orbell S. Reflections on past behavior: a self-report index of habit strength. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2003;33:1313–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Orbell S, Verplanken B. The automatic component of habit in health behavior: habit as cue-contingent automaticity. Health Psychol. 2010;29:374. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. van Bree RJH, van Stralen MM, Mudde AN, Bolman C, de Vries H, Lechner L. Habit as mediator of the relationship between prior and later physical activity: a longitudinal study in older adults. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2015;19:95–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.03.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Wilson TD, Lindsey S, Schooler TY. A model of dual attitudes. Psychol Rev. 2000;107:101–26. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.107.1.101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Perugini M. Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. Br J Soc Psychol. 2005;44:29–45. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466604X23491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Conner MT, Perugini M, O’Gorman R, Ayres K, Prestwich A. Relations between implicit and explicit measures of attitudes and measures of behavior: evidence of moderation by individual difference variables. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2007;33:1727–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207309194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wood W, Labrecque JS, Lin P-Y, Rünger D. Habits in dual process models. Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind 2014:371–85.

  30. Hagger MS. Redefining habits and linking habits with other implicit processes. Psychol Sport Exerc 2020:101606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101606.

  31. Bluemke M, Friese M. Reliability and validity of the Single-Target IAT (ST-IAT): assessing automatic affect towards multiple attitude objects. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2008;38:977–97. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:1464–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Phipps DJ. IAT Constructor: a simplified method of creating IAT based experiments for millisecond Software. OSF 2019. https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/msu2v.

  34. Ajzen I. Constructing a theory of planned behavior questionnaire 2006.

  35. Gardner B, Abraham C, Lally P, de Bruijn G-J. Towards parsimony in habit measurement: testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:102. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-9-102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Kock N. WarpPLS user manual: Version 6.0. Laredo, TX: ScriptWarp Systems. 2018.

  37. Kock N. Stable P value calculation methods in PLS-SEM. ScriptWarp Systems. 2014.

  38. Tenenhaus M, Amato S, Esposito VV. A global goodness-of-fit index for PLS structural equation modelling. Proceedings of the XLII SIS Scientific Meeting. 2004;1:739–42.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Hagger MS, Polet J, Lintunen T. The reasoned action approach applied to health behavior: role of past behavior and tests of some key moderators using meta-analytic structural equation modeling. Soc Sci Med. 2018;213:85–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.07.038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82:62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Karpinski A, Steinman RB, Hilton JL. Attitude importance as a moderator of the relationship between implicit and explicit attitude measures. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005;31:949–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. McNeely J, Cleland CM, Strauss SM, Palamar JJ, Rotrosen J, Saitz R. Validation of self-administered single-item screening questions (SISQs) for unhealthy alcohol and drug use in primary care patients. J Gen Intern Med. 2015;30:1757–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hamilton K, White KM, Cuddihy T. Using a single-item physical activity measure to describe and validate parents’ physical activity patterns. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2012;83:340–5. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2012.10599865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Loftfield E, Yi S, Immerwahr S, Eisenhower D. Construct validity of a single-item, self-rated question of diet quality. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2015;47:181–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kyra Hamilton.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 587 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Phipps, D.J., Hagger, M.S. & Hamilton, K. Evidence That Habit Moderates the Implicit Belief-Behavior Relationship in Health Behaviors. Int.J. Behav. Med. 29, 116–121 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-021-09975-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-021-09975-z

Keywords

Navigation