Skip to main content
Log in

‘The Sweet Taste of Maluma’: Crossmodal Associations Between Tastes and Words

  • Published:
Chemosensory Perception

Abstract

Several recent studies have demonstrated the existence of crossmodal associations (or correspondences) between tastes and non-words, such as ‘maluma’ and ‘takete’. In the present study, we further explore these associations, using tastes and flavors solutions. Participants rated the stimuli on scales anchored by either non-words, taste descriptors, or other descriptors. The results confirm the existence of consistent crossmodal associations between tastes/flavors and words. Moreover, the results of principal components analysis suggest that these associations are likely based on the evaluation of the ‘hedonic value’ and ‘activity’ of the stimuli, two of the three dimensions first identified by Osgood (Psychol Bull 49:197–237, 1952), and since then repeatedly documented in the evaluation of a variety of sensory stimuli. The crossmodal associations elicited by non-words are particularly relevant for the naming/labelling of food products, since they might elicit expectations prior to consumption (and hence potentially be used to avoid any disconfirmation of expectation).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abel GA, Glinert LH (2008) Chemotherapy as language: sound symbolism in cancer medication names. Soc Sci Med 66:1863–1869

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett MS (1954) A note on multiplying factors for various chi square approximations. J Roy Stat Soc B 16:296–298

    Google Scholar 

  • Cardello AV, Maller O, Kapsalis JG et al (1982) Perception of texture by trained and consumer panelists. J Food Sci 47:1186–1197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Civille GV, Lawless HT (1986) The importance of language in describing perceptions. J Sens Stud 1:203–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark CC, Lawless HT (1994) Limiting response alternatives in time-intensity scaling: an examination of the halo-dumping effect. Chem Senses 19:583–594

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crisinel A-S, Spence C (2010) As bitter as a trombone: synesthetic correspondences in non-synesthetes between tastes and flavors and musical instruments and notes. Atten Percept Psycho 72:1994–2002

    Google Scholar 

  • Deliza R, Macfie HJH (1997) The generation of sensory expectation by external cues and its effect on sensory perception and hedonic ratings: a review. J Sens Stud 2:103–128

    Google Scholar 

  • De Saussure F (1916) Cours de linguistique générale [Course in general linguistics]. Payot, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Dichter E (1971) The strategy of selling with packaging. Package Engineering Magazine July:16a–16c

  • Dionysius of Halicarnassus (1910) The letters: their classification, qualities, and mode of production. In: Rhys Robert W (trans and ed) On literary composition. Macmillan, London, p 147

    Google Scholar 

  • Fónagy I (1963) Die Metaphern in der Phonetik [The metaphors in phonetics]. Mouton, The Hague

    Google Scholar 

  • Fónagy I (2001) A research instrument. In: Languages within language: an evolutive approach, John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 337–357

  • Gallace A, Boschin E, Spence C (2010) On the taste of “Bouba” and “Kiki”: an exploration of word–food associations in neurologically normal participants. Cogn Neurosci 2:34–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson-Vaux G, Crisinel A-S, Spence C (submitted) Smelling shapes: crossmodal correspondences between odors and shapes. Chem Senses

  • Hettinger TP, Gent JF, Marks LE, Frank ME (1999) A confusion matrix for the study of taste perception. Percept Psychophys 61:1510–1521

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hinton L, Nichols J, Ohala JJ (eds) (1994) Sound symbolism. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser H (1970) A second generation Little Jiffy. Psychometrika 35:401–415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser H (1974) An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika 39:31–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1929) Gestalt psychology. Liveright, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1947) Gestalt psychology: an introduction to new concepts in modern Psychology. Liveright, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Klink RR (2000) Creating brand names with meaning: the use of sound symbolism. Market Lett 11:5–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawless H (1982) Paradoxical adaptation to taste mixtures. Physiol Behav 29:149–152

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer D, Pathman T, Mondloch CJ (2006) The shape of boubas: sound–shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Dev Sci 9:316–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mccully C (2009) The sound structure of English. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Meiselman HL, Dzendolet E (1967) Variability in gustatory quality identification. Percept Psychophys 2:496–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ngo M, Misra R, Spence C (2011) Assessing the shapes and speech sounds that people associate with chocolate samples varying in cocoa content. Food Qual Prefer 22:567–572

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nygaard LC, Cook AE, Namy LL (2009) Sound to meaning correspondences facilitates word learning. Cognition 112:181–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Mahony M, Goldenberg M, Stedmon J et al (1979) Confusion in the use of the taste adjectives “sour” and “bitter”. Chem Sens Flav 4:301–318

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood CE (1952) The nature and measurement of meaning. Psychol Bull 49:197–237

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood CE, Suci GJ, Tannenbaum PH (1957) The measurement of meaning. University of Illinois Press, Urbana

    Google Scholar 

  • Schifferstein HNJ (2001) Effects of product beliefs on product perception and liking. In: Frewer L, Risvik E, Schifferstein H (eds) Food, people and society: a European perspective of consumers’ food choices. Springer Verlag, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Simner J, Cuskley C, Kirby S (2010) What sound does that taste? Cross-modal mappings across gustation and audition. Perception 39:553–569

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simner J, Haywood SL (2009) Tasty non-words and neighbours: the cognitive roots of lexical-gustatory synaesthesia. Cognition 110:171–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C (2012) Managing sensory expectations concerning products and brands: capitalizing on the potential of sound and shape symbolism. J Consum Psychol 22:37–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Gallace A (2011) Tasting shapes and words. Food Qual Prefer 22:290–295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yorkston E, Menon G (2004) A sound idea: phonetic effects of brand names on consumer judgments. J Consum Res 31:43–51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zannoni M (1997) Approaches to translation problems of sensory descriptors. J Sens Stud 12:239–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anne-Sylvie Crisinel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Crisinel, AS., Jones, S. & Spence, C. ‘The Sweet Taste of Maluma’: Crossmodal Associations Between Tastes and Words. Chem. Percept. 5, 266–273 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-012-9133-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-012-9133-9

Keywords

Navigation