Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Can visual cognitive neuroscience learn anything from the philosophy of language? Ambiguity and the topology of neural network models of multistable perception

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Necker cube and the productive class of related stimuli involving multiple depth interpretations driven by corner-like line junctions are often taken to be ambiguous. This idea is normally taken to be as little in need of defense as the claim that the Necker cube gives rise to multiple distinct percepts. In the philosophy of language, it is taken to be a substantive question whether a stimulus that affords multiple interpretations is a case of ambiguity. If we take into account what have been identified as hallmark features of ambiguity and look at the empirical record, it appears that the Necker cube and related stimuli are not ambiguous. I argue that this raises problems for extant models of multistable perception in cognitive neuroscience insofar as they are purported to apply to these stimuli. Helpfully, similar considerations also yield reasons to suggest that the relevant models are well motivated for other instances of multistable perception. However, a different breed of model seems to be required for the Necker cube and related stimuli. I end with a sketch how one may go about designing such a model relying on oscillatory patters in neural firing. I suggest that distinctions normally confined to the philosophy of language are important for the study of perception, a perspective with a growing number of adherents.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14
Fig. 15
Fig. 16
Fig. 17
Fig. 18

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term “ambiguity” may have other connotations that are not represented in these diagrams, but this is not important for the arguments to follow.

References

  • Albers, J. (1977). Despite straight lines. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashworth, E. J. (1991). Signification and modes of signifying in thirteenth-century logic: A preface to aquinas on analogy. Medieval Philosophy and Theology., 1, 39–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atlas, J. (1989). Philosophy without ambiguity. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atlas, J. (2005). Logic, meaning, and conversation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Babich, S., & Standing, L. (1981). Satiation effects with reversible figures. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 203–210.

  • Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I. (2001). Recognizing depth-rotated objects: A review of recent research and theory. Spatial Vision, 13, 241–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I., & Ju, G. (1988). Surface vs. edge based determinants of visual recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 20(1), 38–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, C., & Cooper, G. F. (1970). Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nature, 228, 477–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blasdel, G., Obermayer, K., & Kiorpes, L. (1995). Organization of ocular dominance and orientation columns in the striate cortex of neonatal macaque monkeys. Visual Neuroscience, 12, 589–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borisyuk, R., Chik, D., & Kazanovich, Y. (2009). Visual perception of ambiguous figures: Synchronization based neural models. Biological Cybernetics, 100, 491–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borsellino, A., De Marco, A., Allazetta, A., Rinsei, S., & Bartolini, B. (1972). Reversal time distribution in the perception of visual ambiguous stimuli. Kybernetik, 10, 139.

  • Britz, J., Landis, T., & Michels, C. M. (2009). Right parietal brain activity precedes perceptual alternation of bistable stimuli. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 55–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. (2004). How can we construct a science of consciousness? In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences III. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, B., Stryker, M. P., & Bonhoeffer, T. (1996). Development of orientation preference maps in ferret primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(20), 6443–6453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. (1959). Perception of reversible figures after brain injury. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 81, 765–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Shulman, G. L., Miezin, F. M., & Petersen, S. E. (1995). Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction. Science, 270, 802–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crair, M. C., Gillespie, D. C., & Stryker, M. P. (1998). The role of visual experience in the development of columns in cat visual cortex. Science, 279, 566–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 97–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 119–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, S., (ms). (1989). The attentional foundations of coherence. Manuscript, UCLA.

  • Deregowski, J. (1969). Perception of the two-pronged trident by two- and three- dimensional perceivers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 9–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deregowski, J. (1989). Real space and represented space: Cross-cultural perspectives. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 51–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deregowski, J., & Bentley, A. M. (1986). Perception of pictorial space by Bushmen. International Journal of Psychology, 21, 743–752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deregowski, J., & Dziurawiec, S. (1986). Some aspects of comprehension of technical diagrams: An intercultural study. Le Travail Humain, 49, 43–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (1991). Preattentive recovery of three-dimensional orientation from line drawings. Psychological Review, 98(3), 335–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J. A., & Ballard, D. H. (1982). Connectionist models and their properties. Cognitive Science, 6, 205–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 474–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., Feldman, L. B., & Katz, L. (1990). Phonological ambiguity and lexical ambiguity: Effects on visual and auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16, 569–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillam, B. (1979). Even possible figures can look impossible. Perception, 8, 229–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, G. (2011). The Semiotic Spectrum. Doctoral dissertation. Rutgers University.

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In J. Kimball (Ed.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 3, pp. 139–151). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. M., Hainline, L., Abramov, I., et al. (1988). The distribution of fixation durations in infants and naïve adults. Vision Research, 28, 419–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayworth, K. J., & Biederman, I. (2006). Neural evidence for intermediate representations in object recognition. Vision Research, 46, 4026–4031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hochberg, J. E., & Brooks, V. (1962). Pictorial recognition as an unlearned ability. American Journal of Psychology, 75, 624–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isoglu-Alkac, U., & Strüber, D. (2006). Necker cube reversals during long-term EEG recordings: Sub-bands of alpha activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 59(2), 179–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kayaert, G., Biederman, I., Op de Beeck, H., & Vogels, R. (2005). Tuning for shape dimensions in macaque inferior temporal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 212–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kayaert, G., Biederman, I., & Vogels, R. (2003). Shape tuning in macaque inferior temporal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 3016–3027.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N., Stanley, D., & Harris, A. (1999). The fusiform face area is selective for faces not animals. NeuroReport, 10, 183–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawabata, N. (1987). Interpretive process of depth in line drawing. Systems and computers in Japan, 18(7), 103–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawabata, N., & Yamagami, K. (1978). Visual fixation points and depth perception. Vision Research, 18, 853–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klink, P. C., van Ee, R., Nijs, M. M., Bruwer, G. J., Noest, A. J., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2008). Early interactions between neuronal adaptation and voluntary control of perceptual choices in bistable vision. Journal of Vision, 8(5), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koralus, P. (2010). Semantics in Philosophy and Cognitive Neuroscience. PhD Dissertation. Princeton University.

  • Koralus, P. (2013). Attention, Consciousness, and the semantics of questions. Synthese, doi:10.1007/s11229-013-0382-1.

  • Koralus, P. (2014). The erotetic theory of attention: Questions, focus, and distraction. Mind and Language, 29(1), 26–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Cortical regions involved in processing object shape. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(9), 3310–3318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leopold, D. A., & Logothetis, N. K. (1999). Multistable phenomena: Changing views in perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 254–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, G. M., & Toppino, T. C. (2004). Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: Alternating views of reversible figures. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 748–768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, G. M., Toppino, T. C., & Kostenbauder, J. F. (1983). As the cube turns: Evidence for two processes in the perception of a dynamic reversible figure. Perception and Psychophysics, 34(1), 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, D. (1984). Perceptual organization and visual recognition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.

  • Mathes, B., Strüber, D., Stadler, M. A., & Basar-Eroglu, C. (2006). Voluntary control of Necker cube reversals modulates the EEG delta- and gamma-band response. Neuroscience Letters, 402, 145–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCone, E., Kanwisher, N., & Duchaine, B. (2007). Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 8–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, M., & Tong, F. (2004). Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision, 4, 539–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno-Bote, R., Rinzel, J., & Rubin, N. (2007). Noise-induced alternations in an attractor network model of perceptual bistability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 98, 1125–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mulder, J. A., & Dawson, R. J. M. (1990). Reconstructing polyhedral scenes from single two- dimensional images: The orthogonality hypothesis. In P. K. Patel-Schneider (Ed.), Proceedings of the 8th Biennial Conference of the CSCSI (pp. 238–244). Palo Alto, CA: Morgan-Kaufmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogawa, Y., Isokawa, T., Matsui, N., Murata, T. (2000). “A neural network model for perceptual alternation of ambiguous figures”. Proceedings IEEE Intl. Workshop on robot and human interactive communication, pp. 264–269.

  • O’Reilly, R., & Munakata, Y. (2000). Computational explorations in cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peissig, J. J., Wasserman, E. A., Young, M. E., & Biederman, I. (2002). Learning an object from multiple views enhances its recognition in an orthogonal rotational axis in pigeons. Vision Research, 42, 2051–2062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. N. (1968). “Cubic corners”. Quarterly Progress Report Research Lab in Electronics, No. 89, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Perkins, D. N. (1972). Visual discrimination between rectangular and nonrectangular para lelpipeds. Perception and Psychophysical, 12, 396–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. N. (1971). Geometry and the Perception of Pictures: Three Studies, Project Zero, Technical Report no. 5. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Peterson, M. A., & Gibson, B. S. (1991). Directing spatial attention within an object: altering the functional equivalence of shape descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 17(1), 170–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pheiffer, C. H., Eure, S. B., & Hamilton, C. B. (1956). Reversible figures and eye- movements. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 452–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pylkkänen, L., & McElree, B. (2007). An MEG study of silent meaning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1905–1921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reisberg, D., & O’Shaughnessy, M. (1984). Diverting subjects’ concentration slows figural reversals. Perception, 13(4), 461–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, J. E., & Gibson, T. L. (1997). Extended visual fixation in young infants: Look distributions, heart rate changes, and attention. Child Development, 68, 1041–1056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romei, V., Brodbeck, V., Michel, C., Amedi, A., Pascual-Leone, A., & Thut, G. (2008). Spontaneous fluctuations in posterior alpha-band EEG activity reflect variability in excitability of human visual areas. Cerebral Cortex, 18(9), 2010–2018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serre, T., Wolf, L., Bileschi, S., Riesenhuber, M., & Poggio, T. (2007). Robust object recognition with cortex-like mechanisms. IEEE Transaction Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 29(3), 411–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seubert, J., Humphreys, G. W., Muller, H. J., & Gramann, K. (2008). Straight after the turn: the role of the parietal lobes in egocentric space processing. Neurocase, 14(2), 204–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1981). Perceptual organization. In M. Kubovy & J. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychophysical complementarity. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1990). Mind sights. New York: Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shevelev, I. A., Lazareva, N. A., Novikova, R. V., Tikhomirov, A. S., Sharaev, G. A., & Cuckiridze, D. Y. (2001). Tuning to Y-like figures in the cat striate neurons. Brain Research Bulletin, 54(5), 543–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strüber, Daniel, & Stadler, Michael. (1999). Differences in top-down influences on the reversal rate of different categories of reversible figures. Perception, 28, 1185–1196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suppes, P., Cohen, M., Laddaga, R., Anliker, J., & Floyd, R. (1983). A procedural theory of eye movements in doing arithmetic. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 27(4), 341–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, K. (1996). Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 19, 109–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, A. (1991). The cognitive brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vidal, J. R., Chaumon, M., O’Reagan, J. K., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2006). Visual grouping and the focusing of attention induce gamma-band oscillations at different frequencies in human magnetoencephalogram signals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(11), 1850–1862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogels, R., Biederman, I., Bar, M., & Lorincz, A. (2001). Inferior temporal neurons show greater sensitivity to nonaccidental than metric differences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 134, 444–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witkin, A. P., & Tenenbaum, J. M. (1983). On the role of structure in vision. In J. Beck, B. Hope, & A. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Human and machine vision (pp. 481–543). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ware, C. (1995). Dynamic stereo displays. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 310–316).

  • Wright, R. D., & Ward, L. M. (2008). Orienting of attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (2000). Attention is fast but volition is slow. Nature, 406, 691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., & Friedman-Hill, S. R. (1992). The role of symmetry in visual search. Psychological Science, 3(3), 194–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yonas, A., & Arteberry, M. E. (1994). Infants perceive spatial structure specified by line drawings. Perception, 23, 1427–1435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yonas, A., Cleaves, W. T., & Pettersen, L. (1978). Development of sensitivity to pictorial depth. Science, 200, 77–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. W., & Deregowski, J. (1981). Learning to see the impossible. Perception, 10, 91–105.

  • Zwicky, A. M., & Sadock, J. M. (1975). Ambiguity tests and how to fail them. In L. Kimball (Ed.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 4, pp. 1–36). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Philipp Koralus.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Koralus, P. Can visual cognitive neuroscience learn anything from the philosophy of language? Ambiguity and the topology of neural network models of multistable perception. Synthese 193, 1409–1432 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0518-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0518-y

Keywords

Navigation