Skip to main content
Log in

Investigating neural representations: the tale of place cells

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While neuroscientists often characterize brain activity as representational, many philosophers have construed these accounts as just theorists’ glosses on the mechanism. Moreover, philosophical discussions commonly focus on finished accounts of explanation, not research in progress. I adopt a different perspective, considering how characterizations of neural activity as representational contributes to the development of mechanistic accounts, guiding the investigations neuroscientists pursue as they work from an initial proposal to a more detailed understanding of a mechanism. I develop one illustrative example involving research on the information-processing mechanisms mammals employ in navigating their environments. This research was galvanized by the discovery in the 1970s of place cells in the hippocampus. This discovery prompted research in what the activity of these cells represents and how place representations figure in navigation. It also led to the discovery of a host of other types of neurons—grid cells, head-direction cells, boundary cells—that carry other types of spatial information and interact with place cells in the mechanism underlying spatial navigation. As I will try to make clear, the research is explicitly devoted to identifying representations and determining how they are constructed and used in an information processing mechanism. Construals of neural activity as representations are not mere glosses but are characterizations to which neuroscientists are committed in the development of their explanatory accounts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I have address van Gelder’s arguments in Bechtel (2008) and Chemero’s arguments in Kaplan and Bechtel (2011).

  2. An important part of Shagrir’s argument is that in determining what computation the oculomotor system is performing the researchers turned to the relation between the position and velocity of the eyes that the system was representing—it is because the relation between the represented properties is integration this the brain needs to perform integration over representations.

  3. In making this case Morgan also draws attention to a condition in Dretske’s (1988) account of representations, that the representation be used in the “production of movements whose successful outcome depends on what is being indicated” (p. 70). Below I contend that representations arise in systems that have the task of controlling other systems. This makes their use in the control of behavior central to accounts of representations.

  4. Morgan shows that some of the objections Ramsey has raised about representations in current neuroscience are more properly concerned with mental representations, where such representations figure in the mental models of agents. For neuroscience, the relevant notion of representation needs to be dissociated from the role representations play in mental models and treated as processes within a processing system that stands in for their contents without the organism being conscious of them.

  5. The language I use in this paragraph to describe the project of identifying representations is very similar to that which McCauley and I (Bechtel and McCauley 1999; McCauley and Bechtel 2001) used in discussing identity claims in science—they are proposed at the outset of inquiry and by the time a research endeavor has developed around them, revising and elaborating on them, it would seem perverse to the scientists to propose that the relation in question was mere correlation, not identity. In fact claims about representations can be viewed as instances of identity claims since what researches are doing is identifying constituents of mechanisms as representations.

  6. This is not to ignore neural phenomena that involve local transformations within the brain that are also of considerable interest to neuroscientists. In addition to ongoing metabolic activities and processes of gene expression, there are operations involving electrical potentials resulting from movements of ions across membranes which can sometimes give raise to action potentials in the absence of external stimuli. Some of this endogenous activity, such as synchronized oscillations in electrical potentials, may be extremely important to the brain’s capacity to execute cognitive operations when it receives sensory input (Abrahamsen and Bechtel 2011).

  7. Processing information so as to coordinate responses to conditions inside or external to the organism is not unique to organisms with brains. Bacteria perform a vast array of information processing functions using chemical rather than neural signaling, and this has led a number of researchers to refer to bacterial cognition (see, for example, Shapiro 2007). Plants also use chemical signaling to execute a broad range of behaviors that has inspired a field some refer to as plant neurobiology (for discussion, see Garzon and Keijzer 2011).

  8. See Craver (2003) for a discussion of the research on the hippocampus that led to the discovery of long-term potentiation as a laboratory technique before it, and the hippocampus more generally, became associated with learning and memory.

  9. Miller and Best (1980) established the necessity of input from entorhinal cortex for hippocampal function by showing that lesions to the entorhinal cortex impaired the navigational abilities of rats and the responsiveness of hippocampal place cells.

  10. Ranck provides a reflective discussion of the prospects and challenges of such a project noting several reasons the project might fail: the firing of a neuron “may signal something not directly related to overt behavior, such as drive state, or some idea the rat has, or the blood level of some substance. The firing of the neuron might be part of some internal timing mechanism, or a mechanism in memory retrieval. The firing of the neuron might be significant only in some neural net, and therefore, firing of a single neuron may not be interpretable.” With the hippocampus, however, he proposes that the strategy does work, but contends that correlation is not enough—one must determine how the information is transformed: “to be able to apply this approach to hippocampal formation we must know behavioral correlates of almost all inputs and outputs of the system and see what transformations occur.” He distinguishes two strategies, one in which researchers seek out conditions in which the neuron fires (during this stage “the behavior of the neuron shapes the behavior of the experimenter”), and a second in which a more systematic protocol is employed that also considers its firing frequency and patterns. Ranck’s discussion is particularly interesting in light of Ramsey’s claims, discussed above, that neuroscience only uses a receptor notion of representation. On Ranck’s view, understanding the behavior of neurons requires going beyond identifying it as a receptor and determining how what it represents is affected by the context within the organism.

  11. In the wake of the initial research recording from hippocampal cells, O’Keefe collaborated with Nadel on a lesion experiment in which they lesioned the major input and output pathway from the hippocampus, the fornix. They found that these rats were unable to learn to locate water that was always at the same location, but showed somewhat improved performance in locating water that was always marked by the same cue (light). This indicated that the rats required the representation of its current location in allocentric space provided by CA1 cells in order to use place information in navigating their environment.

  12. Leutgelb et al.’s findings are, on the face, inconsistent with those of Muller and Kubie noted above in which change from a circular to a rectangular enclosure resulted in global remapping. This may be explained by the fact, that in Muller and Kubie’s study the rats were first trained on both enclosures when they were next to each other in a common room and so had presumably developed place codes for each (Colgin et al. 2008). The substitution of one enclosure for another thus elicited the distinct encodings that had already been acquired for each.

  13. O’Keefe and Burgess (1996) investigated how place fields might remap as a result of changes in the dimensions of the enclosure by comparing the responsiveness of the same neurons in enclosures that differed in the length of one or both walls—a small square, vertical rectangle (vertical wall extended), horizontal rectangle, and large square. This revealed that changing the length of the wall could cause place fields to expand, or sometimes split into two separate fields.

  14. Both Wills et al. and Leutgeb et al. interpret their studies in light of the characterization of CA3 as constituting an attractor network. Wills et al. treat the abrupt transition in the place fields as indicative of an attractor network due to the recurrent connections in CA3 following the sparse coding imposed by DG. Although Leutgeb et al.’s results are in tension with a simple attractor network with a single global attractor, they construe the hysteresis effects as showing that more than a feedforward process is at work (perhaps recurrent networks with multiple attractors).

  15. The labeling of the frequency ranges of oscillations detected with EEG (or as local field potentials with implanted electrodes) as alpha, beta, reflects the order in which oscillations in a frequency range were discovered. The range labeled theta in the hippocampus extended higher (into the traditional alpha band) than the 4–8 Hz band associated with cortical theta waves.

  16. See Buzsáki (2005) for a review and references. He concludes (p. 828): “Despite seven decades of hard work on rabbits, rats, mice, gerbils, guinea pigs, sheep, cats, dogs, old world monkeys, chimpanzees, and humans by outstanding colleagues, to date, there is no widely agreed term that would unequivocally describe behavioral correlate(s) of this prominent brain rhythm. By exclusion, the only firm message that can be safely concluded from this brief summary is that in an immobile animal no theta is present, provided that no changes occur in the environment (and the animal is not ‘thinking’).”

  17. For a discussion of this and other roles oscillation and their synchronization play in neural computing, see Buzsáki (2005). In this paper I have focused on research analyzing the responses of individual neurons. This focus was a consequence of the fact that until recently researchers could only record from individual neurons. Increasingly, though, researchers are recording from populations of neurons. This enables them to focus on the interactive behavior. Regions such as CA3, with many recurrent connections, exhibit especially rich dynamic behavior, but in fact most brain regions exhibit a fair amount of recurrent connectivity between neurons, resulting in what Hebb referred to as cell assemblies. As Buzsáki (2010) argues, assemblies of neurons generating action potentials form dynamically in the brain through mechanisms that temporarily alter pre- and post-synaptic mechanisms. These mechanisms give raise to specific constellations of synaptic weights he refers to as synapsembles.

  18. More recent research from the same group, however, has shown that MEC responses gradually diminish when the hippocampus is inhibited by \(\hbox {GABA}_\mathrm{A}\) receptor agonist muscimol. I return to these studies below.

  19. The researchers, though, did find a distinct pattern across the entire MEC: the spacing of vertices and the size of the fields in which neurons responded expanded monotonically the further from the dorsal tip on the border with the perirhinal cortex (where the fields extended approximately 30 cm) to the ventral tip (3 m) (Brun et al. 2008). This mirrors the expansion of place fields as one moves along the dorsoventral axis of the hippocampus.

  20. As with place cells, there is not a topographical mapping of these cells onto directions. These cells only respond to direction in the horizontal plane and are largely unaffected by pitch or roll of the animal’s head as well as its ongoing behavior. The pattern of activity is stable in a given environment over several days.

  21. Using a task in which the animal had to make a correct choice to acquire a reward, Dudchenko and Taube (1997) showed that typically head direction cells rotated their firing fields, and then, and only then, did the animal perform the correct behavior. Other studies from the same laboratory have found more problematic relations between activity of head-direction cells and behavior (Golob et al. 2001).

  22. When visual cues were removed, head direction and place cells both often rotated in unpredictable but correlated ways (Knierim et al. 1995).

  23. The discovery of yet other kinds of cells also supports such an account. Independently, Solstad et al. (2008) and Savelli et al. (2008) discovered a type of cell in the dorsocaudal quarter of the MEC and adjacent parasubiculum that fires only when the rat is along one or sometimes several borders of an enclosure. Accordingly, they named these cells border cells. These cells typically fire along most of the length of a border, and if the enclosure is extended on the side of the border, their firing fields are likewise expanded. If a new wall is inserted parallel to the border, a new firing field emerges on the same side of that wall as the field at the enclosure boundary. When the walls are removed and the platform simply drops off at the previous boundary, the boundary cells continue to respond. The behavior of these cells is also not affected when one enclosure is substituted for another. Still another type of cell carrying spatial information, boundary vector cells, were initially predicted on the basis of computational models designed to account for the remapping of place fields as a result of stretching the enclosure in a given direction (O’Keefe and Burgess 1996). Individual cells were postulated to have receptive fields at different distances from the rat and the cells were hypothesized to fire whenever an enclosure boundary intersects that field, regardless of the direction in which the rat was moving. Lever et al. (2009) identified a population of such cells in the subiculum that fit this description. The firing of these cells was unaffected by such factors as the color or material of the boundary, and whether it was a wall or a drop. These cells do not remap in the wake of changes to enclosures that yield remapping of place cells or head direction cells. Recent research indicates that grid cells are also found in the presubiculum and the parasubiculum, where they are collocated with head direction and border cells (Boccara et al. 2010).

  24. There is not space here to discuss research on how path integration might be performed. Most of this research has taken the form of developing computational models. Fundamentally, two modeling strategies have been pursued—oscillatory interference models in which the interference between multiple oscillators serves to track locations and attractor network models in which networks of interacting components create dynamically transformable attractors (see Giocomo et al. 2011).

  25. The fact that place cells typically carried information about more than spatial location was already clear in the remapping studies described above. For some researchers, especially those also influenced by research on human memory that viewed the hippocampus as fundamental to the encoding of episodic memories, this suggested a view in which the hippocampus was not an abstract map but served to relate spatial information to other content related to events the animal had experienced (Eichenbaum et al. 1999).

  26. This point is nicely made by Moser and Moser “Finally, it is important to realize that the hippocampal–parahippocampal circuit only forms a representation. Representations can only influence navigation to the extent that information about the animal’s location is transferred to brain regions involved in planning and initiating movement” (2008, p. 1151).

  27. Burke et al. (2005) addressed specifically the question of whether spatial differentiation was registered first in PPC and communicated to the hippocampal regions, or whether it was first developed in the hippocampal regions and then influenced PPC. Projections to the hippocampus originate in the deeper layers of PPC while the projections from the hippocampus are received in the superficial layers. The researchers showed, using a analysis of gene expression, that the differential representation of an enclosure in different rooms affected the superficial layers of PPC, not the deep layers, showing that the processing of location in the hippocampus regulate PPC behavior, not vice versa.

  28. This study shared the limitation that the mazes used were far less complex than the routes rats navigate in the wild. Thus, despite its strong evidence for preplay, it does not conclusively answer the question of what role replay and preplay might subserve in real world navigation. One supportive indication came from Davidson, Kloosterman, and Wilson’s (2009) demonstration that when rats are required to run on a straight track, ripple events get chained together for longer tracks. They suggested that this chaining might be mediated by reentrant processing between the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, supporting the idea that replay during the SPW state might be a general mechanism employed in learning a navigational route.

References

  • Abrahamsen, A., & Bechtel, W. (2011). From reactive to endogenously active dynamical conceptions of the brain. In K. Plaisance & T. Reydon (Eds.), Philosophy of behavioral biology (pp. 329–366). New York: Springer.

  • Andersen, R. A., & Buneo, C. A. (2002). Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25, 189–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barry, C., Hayman, R., Burgess, N., & Jeffery, K. J. (2007). Experience-dependent rescaling of entorhinal grids. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 682–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W. (2008). Mental mechanisms: Philosophical perspectives on cognitive neuroscience. London: Routledge.

  • Bechtel, W. (2011). Representing time of day in circadian clocks. In A. Newen, A. Bartels, & E.-M. Jung (Eds.), Knowledge and representation. Palo Alto, CA: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (2005). Explanation: A mechanist alternative. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 36, 421–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W., & McCauley, R. N. (1999). Heuristic identity theory (or back to the future): The mind-body problem against the background of research strategies in cognitive neuroscience. In M. Hahn & S. C. Stoness (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 67–72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Bechtel, W., & Richardson, R. C. (1993/2010). Discovering complexity: Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 1993 edition published by Princeton University Press.

  • Boccara, C. N., Sargolini, F., Thoresen, V. H., Solstad, T., Witter, M. P., Moser, E. I., et al. (2010). Grid cells in pre- and parasubiculum. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 987–994.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnevie, T., Dunn, B., Fyhn, M., Hafting, T., Derdikman, D., Kubie, J. L., et al. (2013). Grid cells require excitatory drive from the hippocampus. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 309–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bostock, E., Muller, R. U., & Kubie, J. L. (1991). Experience-dependent modifications of hippocampal place cell firing. Hippocampus, 1, 193–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brun, V. H., Otnæss, M. K., Molden, S., Steffenach, H.-A., Witter, M. P., Moser, M.-B., et al. (2002). Place cells and place recognition maintained by direct entorhinal–hippocampal circuitry. Science, 296, 2243–2246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brun, V. H., Solstad, T., Kjelstrup, K. B., Fyhn, M., Witter, M. P., Moser, E. I., et al. (2008). Progressive increase in grid scale from dorsal to ventral medial entorhinal cortex. Hippocampus, 18, 1200–1212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R. L. (2010). The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 27–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2011). Models of place and grid cell firing and theta rhythmicity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 21, 734–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, S. N., Chawla, M. K., Penner, M. R., Crowell, B. E., Worley, P. F., Barnes, C. A., et al. (2005). Differential encoding of behavior and spatial context in deep and superficial layers of the neocortex. Neuron, 45, 667–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buzsáki, G. (1986). Hippocampal sharp waves: Their origin and significance. Brain Research, 398, 242–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buzsáki, G. (2005). Theta rhythm of navigation: Link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus, 15, 827–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buzsáki, G. (2010). Neural syntax: Cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron, 68, 362–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buzsáki, G., Lai-Wo, S. L., & Vanderwolf, C. H. (1983). Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Research Reviews, 6, 139–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2000). Anti-representationalism and the dynamical stance. Philosophy of Science, 67, 625–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). Language and nature. Mind, 104, 1–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colgin, L. L., Denninger, T., Fyhn, M., Hafting, T., Bonnevie, T., Jensen, O., et al. (2009). Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature, 462, 353–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colgin, L. L., & Moser, E. I. (2010). Gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. Physiology, 25, 319–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colgin, L. L., Moser, E. I., & Moser, M.-B. (2008). Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping. Trends in Neurosciences, 31, 469–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craver, C. F. (2003). The making of a memory mechanism. Journal of the History of Biology, 36, 153–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. (1989). Meaning and mental representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, T. J., Kloosterman, F., & Wilson, M. A. (2009). Hippocampal replay of extended experience. Neuron, 63, 497–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diba, K., & Buzsaki, G. (2007). Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1241–1242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dretske, F. I. (1988). Explaining behavior: Reasons in a world of causes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dudchenko, P. A., & Taube, J. S. (1997). Correlation between head direction cell activity and spatial behavior on a radial arm maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egan, F. (2010). Computational models: A modest role for content. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 41, 253–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eichenbaum, H., Dudchenko, P., Wood, E., Shapiro, M., & Tanila, H. (1999). The hippocampus, memory, and place cells: Is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron, 23, 209–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fyhn, M., Hafting, T., Treves, A., Moser, M.-B., & Moser, E. I. (2007). Hippocampal remapping and grid realignment in entorhinal cortex. Nature, 446, 190–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Witter, M. P., Moser, E. I., & Moser, M.-B. (2004). Spatial representation in the entorhinal cortex. Science, 305, 1258–1264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garzón, F., & Rodríguez, Á. (2009). Where is cognitive science heading? Minds and Machines, 19, 301–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garzon, P. C., & Keijzer, F. (2011). Plants: Adaptive behavior, root-brains, and minimal cognition. Adaptive Behavior, 19, 155–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giocomo, L. M., Moser, M. B., & Moser, E. I. (2011). Computational models of grid cells. Neuron, 71, 589–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golob, E. J., Stackman, J. R. W., Wong, A. C., & Taube, J. S. (2001). Correlation between head direction cell activity and spatial behavior on a radial arm maze. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 285–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodridge, J. P., & Taube, J. S. (1995). Preferential use of the landmark navigational system by head direction cells in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 49–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Bonnevie, T., Moser, M.-B., & Moser, E. I. (2008). Hippocampus-independent phase precession in entorhinal grid cells. Nature, 453, 1248–1252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M.-B., & Moser, E. I. (2005). Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature, 436, 801–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haselager, P., de Groot, A., & van Rappard, H. (2003). Representationalism versus anti-representationalism: A debate for the sake of appearance. Philosophical Psychology, 16, 5–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasselmo, M. E., Bodelón, C., & Wyble, B. P. (2002). A proposed function for hippocampal theta rhythm: Separate phases of encoding and retrieval enhance reversal of prior learning. Neural Computation, 14, 793–817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huxter, J., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. A. (2003). Independent rate and temporal coding in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Nature, 425, 828–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeffery, K. J., Gilbert, A., Burton, S., & Strudwick, A. (2003). Preserved performance in a hippocampal-dependent spatial task despite complete place cell remapping. Hippocampus, 13, 175–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, O., & Lisman, J. E. (2000). Position reconstruction from an ensemble of hippocampal place cells: Contribution of theta phase coding. Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, 2602–2609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jirsa, V. K., Fuchs, A., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1998). Connecting cortical and behavioral dynamics: Bimanual coordination. Neural Computing, 10, 2019–2045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, D. M., & Bechtel, W. (2011). Dynamical models: An alternative or complement to mechanistic explanations? Topics in Cognitive Science, 3, 438–444.

  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knierim, J., Kudrimoti, H., & McNaughton, B. (1995). Place cells, head direction cells, and the learning of landmark stability. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1648–1659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, B., & Walkey, J. (1987). Behavioural and anatomical studies of the posterior parietal cortex in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 23, 127–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 91–196). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Leutgeb, J. K., Leutgeb, S., Treves, A., Meyer, R., Barnes, C. A., McNaughton, B. L., et al. (2005a). Progressive transformation of hippocampal neuronal representations in “Morphed” environments. Neuron, 48, 345–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leutgeb, S., Leutgeb, J. K., Barnes, C. A., Moser, E. I., McNaughton, B. L., & Moser, M.-B. (2005b). Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science, 309, 619–623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leutgeb, S., Leutgeb, J. K., Treves, A., Moser, M.-B., & Moser, E. I. (2004). Distinct ensemble codes in hippocampal areas CA3 and CA1. Science, 305, 1295–1298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lever, C., Burton, S., Jeewajee, A., O’Keefe, J., & Burgess, N. (2009). Boundary vector cells in the subiculum of the hippocampal formation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9771–9777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lever, C., Wills, T., Cacucci, F., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2002). Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature, 416, 90–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machamer, P., Darden, L., & Craver, C. F. (2000). Thinking about mechanisms. Philosophy of Science, 67, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, E. J., Qin, Y., Leonard, B., Skaggs, W. E., McNaughton, B. I., & Barnes, C. A. (1995). Interactions between location and task affect the spatial and directional firing of hippocampal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 7079–7094.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. C. (1971). Simple memory: A theory for archicortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 262, 23–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. C. (1982). Vision: A computation investigation into the human representational system and processing of visual information. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, R. N., & Bechtel, W. (2001). Explanatory pluralism and the heuristic identity theory. Theory and Psychology, 11, 736–760.

  • McNaughton, B. L., Battaglia, F. P., Jensen, O., Moser, E. I., & Moser, M.-B. (2006). Path integration and the neural basis of the ’cognitive map’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 663–678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, V. M., & Best, P. J. (1980). Spatial correlates of hippocampal unit activity are altered by lesions of the fornix and entorhinal cortex. Brain Research, 194, 311–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. G. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moita, M. A. P., Rosis, S., Zhou, Y., LeDoux, J. E., & Blair, H. T. (2004). Putting fear in its place: Remapping of hippocampal place cells during fear conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 7015–7023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, A. (2014). Representations gone mental. Synthese, 191, 213–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moser, E. I., & Moser, M.-B. (2008). A metric for space. Hippocampus, 18, 1142–1156.

  • Moser, E. I., Kropff, E., & Moser, M.-B. (2008). Place cells, grid cells, and the brain’s spatial representation system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 69–89.

  • Muller, R. U., & Kubie, J. L. (1987). The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 1951–1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, R. U., Kubie, J. L., Bostock, E., Traube, J. S., & Quirk, G. (1991). Spatial firing correlates of neurons in the hippocampal formation of freely moving rats. In J. Paillard (Ed.), Brain and space (pp. 296–333). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, R. U., Kubie, J. L., & Ranck, J. B. (1987). Spatial firing patterns of hippocampal complex-spike cells in a fixed environment. The Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 1935–1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, R. U., Quirk, G. J., & Kubie, J. L. (1989). Back-propagation calculations of hippocampal place cell firing fields. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 15, 403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, R. U., Ranck, J. B., & Taube, J. S. (1996). Head direction cells: Properties and functional significance. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 196–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navratilova, Z., Giocomo, L. M., Fellous, J.-M., Hasselmo, M. E., & McNaughton, B. L. (2012). Phase precession and variable spatial scaling in a periodic attractor map model of medial entorhinal grid cells with realistic after-spike dynamics. Hippocampus, 22, 772–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A. (1976). Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat. Experimental Neurology, 51, 78–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A., & Burgess, N. (1996). Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons. Nature, 381, 425–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A., & Burgess, N. (2005). Dual phase and rate coding in hippocampal place cells: Theoretical significance and relationship to entorhinal grid cells. Hippocampus, 15, 853–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A., & Conway, D. H. (1978). Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: Why they fire where they fire. Experimental Brain Research, 31, 573–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A., & Dostrovsky, J. (1971). The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely moving rat. Brain Research, 34, 171–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A., & Recce, M. L. (1993). Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus, 3, 317–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, J. A., & Speakman, A. (1987). Single unit activity in the rat hippocampus during a spatial memory task. Experimental Brain Research, 68, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parron, C., & Save, E. (2004). Evidence for entorhinal and parietal cortices involvement in path integration in the rat. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 349–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pastalkova, E., Itskov, V., Amarasingham, A., & Buzsáki, G. (2008). Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science, 321, 1322–1327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quirk, G., Muller, R., & Kubie, J. (1990). The firing of hippocampal place cells in the dark depends on the rat’s recent experience. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 2008–2017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quirk, G., Muller, R., Kubie, J., & Ranck, J. (1992). The positional firing properties of medial entorhinal neurons: Description and comparison with hippocampal place cells. The Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 1945–1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, W. (2007). Representation reconsidered. Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ranck, J. B. (1973). Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. Part I. Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. Experimental Neurology, 41, 462–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ranck, J. B. (1986). Head direction cells in the deep cell layer of dorsal presubiculum in freely moving rats. In G. Buzsáki & C. H. Vanderwolf (Eds.), Electrical activity of the archicortex (pp. 217–220). Budapest: Academia Kiado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redish, A. D., Rosenzweig, E. S., Bohanick, J. D., McNaughton, B. L., & Barnes, C. A. (2000). Dynamics of hippocampal ensemble activity realignment: Time versus space. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 9298–9309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, R. S., Ziegler, M., Winocur, G., Grady, C. L., & Moscovitch, M. (2004). “I have often walked down this street before”: fMRI Studies on the hippocampus and other structures during mental navigation of an old environment. Hippocampus, 14, 826–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sargolini, F., Fyhn, M., Hafting, T., McNaughton, B. L., Witter, M. P., Moser, M.-B., et al. (2006). Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science, 312, 758–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savelli, F., Yoganarasimha, D., & Knierim, J. J. (2008). Influence of boundary removal on the spatial representations of the medial entorhinal cortex. Hippocampus, 18, 1270–1282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20, 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shagrir, O. (2010). Marr on computational-level theories. Philosophy of Science, 77, 477–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shagrir, O. (2012). Structural representations and the brain. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 63(3), 519–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shagrir, O., & Bechtel, W. (2011). Marr’s computational-level theories and delineating phenomena. In D. M. Kaplan (Ed.), Integrating psychology and neuroscience: Prospects and problems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, J. A. (2007). Bacteria are small but not stupid: Cognition, natural genetic engineering and socio-bacteriology. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C, 38, 807–819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skaggs, W. E., & McNaughton, B. L. (1998). Spatial firing properties of hippocampal CA1 populations in an environment containing two visually identical regions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 8455–8466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skaggs, W. E., McNaughton, B. L., Wilson, M. A., & Barnes, C. A. (1996). Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus, 6, 149–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solstad, T., Boccara, C. N., Kropff, E., Moser, M.-B., & Moser, E. I. (2008). Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex. Science, 322, 1865–1868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiers, H. J., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). The neuroscience of remote spatial memory: A tale of two cities. Neuroscience, 149, 7–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffenach, H. A., Witter, M., Moser, M. B., & Moser, E. I. (2005). Spatial memory in the rat requires the dorsolateral band of the entorhinal cortex. Neuron, 45, 301–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swoyer, C. (1991). Structural representation and surrogative reasoning. Synthese, 87, 449–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taube, J. S., Muller, R. U., & Ranck, J. B. (1990a). Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 420–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taube, J. S., Muller, R. U., & Ranck, J. B. (1990b). Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 436–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Gelder, T., & Port, R. (1995). It’s about time: An overview of the dynamical approach to cognition. In R. Port & T. van Gelder (Eds.), It’s about time. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitlock, J. R., Sutherland, R. J., Witter, M. P., Moser, M.-B., & Moser, E. I. (2008). Navigating from hippocampus to parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 14755–14762.

  • Wills, T. J., Lever, C., Cacucci, F., Burgess, N., & O’Keefe, J. (2005). Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science, 308, 873–876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zinyuk, L., Kubik, S., Kaminsky, Y., Fenton, A. A., & Bures, J. (2000). Understanding hippocampal activity by using purposeful behavior: Place navigation induces place cell discharge in both task-relevant and task-irrelevant spatial reference frames. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 3771–3776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I conducted the research for this paper while a Fellow at the Institute for Advanced Studies at Hebrew University and their support is gratefully acknowledged. I thank members of the Mind and Brain Research Group at the Institute for productive discussions. I also received valuable feedback from audiences at the Center for Theoretical Neuroscience at the University of Waterloo, the Southern Society for Philosophy and Psychology, the University of Amsterdam, and the Rotman Institute at the University of Western Ontario.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William Bechtel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bechtel, W. Investigating neural representations: the tale of place cells. Synthese 193, 1287–1321 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0480-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0480-8

Keywords

Navigation