Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Syntactic skills in sentence reading comprehension among Chinese elementary school children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study examined the role of syntactic skills for reading comprehension in Chinese. Two hundred and seventy-two Chinese children were tested on their phonological processing, orthographic, morphological, syntactic, and literacy skills at Grades 1 and 2. Hierarchical multiple regression results showed that syntactic skills, in terms of word order, connective usage, and knowledge of morphosyntactic structure (measured by an oral cloze task) in Grade 1, significantly predicted sentence reading comprehension in Grade 2 after controlling for the children’s age, IQ, and word level reading-related cognitive skills in Grade 1, and word reading in Grade 2. As in alphabetic languages, syntactic skills are essential for reading comprehension in Chinese. The unique roles of individual syntactic skills for understanding sentences in Chinese are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1989). Functionalism and the competition model. In B. MacWhinney & E. Bates (Eds.), The cross-linguistic study of sentence processing (pp. 3–73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., McNew, S., MacWhinney, B., Devescovi, A., & Smith, S. (1982). Functional constraints on sentence processing: A cross-linguistic study. Cognition, 11, 245–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., Deutsch, A., & Liberman, I. Y. (1990). Syntactic competence and reading ability in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 147–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (2003). Text comprehension and its relation to coherence and cohesion in children’s fictional narratives. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 335–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (1996). The nature of the relationship between comprehension skill and the ability to tell a story. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chao, Y. R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M. J., Lau, L. L., & Yung, F. Y. (1993). Development of component skills in reading Chinese. International Journal of Psychology, 28, 481–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Tracking the unique effects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, general knowledge, and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeFrancis, J. (1984). The Chinese language: Fact and fantasy. Honolulu: The University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (R. A. N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, A., & Bentin, S. (1996). Attention factors mediating syntactic deficiency in reading-disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 386–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. E., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography in morphological awareness. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

  • Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gong, S. Y., & Peng, D. L. (2005). Advances in the researches on the development of syntactic awareness and the relationship between syntactic awareness and reading comprehension. Psychological Science (China), 28, 754–756. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 563–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hakes, D. T. (1980). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. -H., Chan, D. W. -O., Lee, S. -H., Tsang, S. -M., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 43–75.

  • Ho, C. S. -H., Chan, D. W. -O., Tsang, S. -M., & Lee, S. -H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38, 543–553.

  • Ho, C. S. -H., Chan, D., Tsang, S. -M., & Lee, S. -H. (2000). The Hong Kong test of specific learning difficulties in reading and writing. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team.

  • Ho, C. S. -H., Ng, T. -T., & Ng, W. -K. (2003). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35, 849–878.

  • Hu, Y. S. (2003). 現代漢語 [新版] (Modern Chinese (New Addition)). Hong Kong: Joint Publishing. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (2005). Insights into second language reading: A cross-linguistic approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ku, Y. M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 399–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leung, M. T., & Lee, A. (2002). The Hong Kong Corpus of Primary School Chinese. Paper presented at the 9th meeting of the international clinical.

  • Li, W., Anderson, R. C., Nagy, W., & Zhang, H. (2002). Facets of metalinguistic awareness that contribute to Chinese literacy. In W. Li, J. S. Gaffney, & J. L. Packard (Eds.), Chinese children’s reading acquisition: Theoretical and pedagogical issues (pp. 87–111). Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

  • Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to read. In D. Shankweiler & I. Y. Liberman (Eds.), Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 1–33). Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, I. M. (1999). A comparison study of the structure of thinking shown in Chinese and English sentences. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 41, 131–165. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, X. M. (2004). 現代漢語句子生成問題研究:一個以語序為樣本的探索 (A study on Modern Chinese sentence production: an exploration into a sample of word-order). Shanghai Shi, China: Hua dong shi fan da xue chu ban she. [In Chinese].

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S. -H. (2005). Predictors of beginning reading in Chinese and English: A 2-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergartners. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 117–144.

  • McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mori, Y., & Nagy, W. (1999). Integration of information from context and word elements in interpreting novel kanji compounds. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 80–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Factors influencing syntactic awareness skills in normal readers and poor comprehenders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 229–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 637–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J., & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 339–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M. (2001). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic awareness and reading: A longitudinal study from Kindergarten to Grade 1. First Language, 21, 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2003). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic awareness, and naming speed in the reading and spelling performance of first-grade children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 287–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2004). Predictive influence of phonological processing, morphological/syntactic skill, and naming speed on spelling performance. Brain and Cognition, 55, 368–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, C., Tunmer, W. E., & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Children’s capacity to correct grammatical violations in sentences. Journal of Child Language, 11, 129–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Wightman, J. (1994). The role of phonology in young children learning to read words: The direct mapping hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 42–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., & Brady, S. (1992). Identifying the causes of reading disability. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 275–305). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. R., & Hudson, J. A. (1991). Tell me a make-believe story: Coherence and cohesion in young children’s picture-elicited narratives. Developmental Psychology, 27, 960–974.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Wu, N. (2000). Phonetic awareness: Knowledge of orthographic-phonology relationships in the character acquisition of Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 56–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Zhang, H. C. (1995). Incidental learning of word meanings while reading: A Chinese and American cross-cultural study. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 76–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1988). Development of grammatical sensitivity, phonological, and short-term memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 28–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • So, D., & Siegel, L. S. (1997). Learning to read Chinese: Semantic, syntactic, phonological and working memory skills in normally achieving and poor Chinese readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, Z. G. (1992). 4–8 year-old Chinese children’s judgment ability in sentence acceptability. Psychological Science (China), 5, 23–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590–604, 612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

  • Treiman, R. (1991). Phonological awareness and its roles in learning to read and spell. In D. J. Sawyer & B. J. Fox (Eds.), Phonological awareness in reading: The evolution of current perspectives (pp. 159–189). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tsang, K. K. S., & Stokes, S. (2001). Syntactic awareness of Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 28, 703–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., Herriman, M. L., & Nesdale, A. R. (1988). Metalinguistic abilities and beginning reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 134–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., Nesdale, A. R., & Wright, A. D. (1987). Syntactic awareness and reading acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 25–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., & Nesdale, A. R. (1985). Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 417–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Kleeck, A. (1982). The emergence of linguistic awareness: A cognitive framework. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly - Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 237–265.

  • Wang, L. (1955). 中國語法理論 (Theories of Chinese grammar). Beijing: Zhonghua Shu ju. [In Chinese].

  • Yang, H., & Peng, D. (1997). The learning and naming of Chinese characters of elementary school children. In H. C. Chen (Ed.), Cognitive processing of Chinese and related Asian languages (pp. 323–346). Hong Kong: The Chinese University of Hong Kong Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Y. (1979). 康熙字典 [Kangxi Dictionary]. Beijing: The Commercial Press. [In Chinese]

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by READ & WRITE: A Jockey Club Learning Support Network from The Hong Kong Jockey Club Charities Trust. Special thanks are due to Ms. Suk-han Lee and Ms. Suk-man Tsang, Education Bureau, Hong Kong Special Administrative Region Government, Hong Kong, China, for their valuable advices and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Connie Suk-han Ho.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chik, P.Pm., Ho, C.Sh., Yeung, Ps. et al. Syntactic skills in sentence reading comprehension among Chinese elementary school children. Read Writ 25, 679–699 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9293-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9293-4

Keywords

Navigation