Skip to main content
Log in

Representational Pseudoneglect: A Review

  • Review
  • Published:
Neuropsychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Pseudoneglect, the tendency to be biased towards the left-hand side of space, is a robust and consistent behavioural observation best demonstrated on the task of visuospatial line bisection, where participants are asked to centrally bisect visually presented horizontal lines at the perceived centre. A number of studies have revealed that a representational form of pseudoneglect exists, occurring when participants are asked to either mentally represent a stimulus or explore a stimulus using touch in the complete absence of direct visuospatial processing. Despite the growing number of studies that have demonstrated representational pseudoneglect there exists no current and comprehensive review of these findings and no discussion of a theoretical framework into which these findings may fall. An important gap in the current representational pseudoneglect literature is a discussion of the developmental trajectory of the bias. The focus of the current review is to outline studies that have observed representational pseudoneglect in healthy participants, consider a theoretical framework for these observations, and address the impact of lifespan factors such as cognitive ageing on the phenomenon.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiello, M., Jacquin-Courtois, S., Merola, S., Ottaviani, T., Tomaiuolo, F., Bueti, D., et al. (2012). No inherent left and right side in human 'mental number line’: evidence from right brain damage. Brain, 135(8), 2492–2505.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashkenazi, S., & Henik, A. (2010). A dissociation between physical and mental number bisection in developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2861–2868.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Azañón, E., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2008). Spatial remapping of tactile events: assessing the effects of frequent posture changes. Communicative and Integral Biology, 1(1), 45–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azañón, E., Camacho, K., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2010). Tactile remapping beyond space. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31(10), 1858–1867.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bachmann, V., Fischer, M. H., Landolt, H. P., & Brugger, P. (2010). Asymmetric prefrontal cortex functions predict asymmetries in number space. Brain and Cognition, 74(3), 306–311.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baek, M. J., Lee, B. H., Kwon, J. C., Park, J. M., Kang, S. J., Chin, J., et al. (2002). Influence of final search direction on tactile line bisection in normal subjects. Neurology, 58(12), 1833–1838.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett-Cowan, M., Jenkin, H., Dyde, R., Jenkin, M., & Harris, L. (2013). Asymmetrical representation of body orientation. Journal of Vision, 13(2).

  • Bartolomeo, P., & Chokron, S. (2002). Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(2), 217–234.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolomeo, P., D’ Erme, P., & Gainotti, G. (1994). The relationship between visuospatial and representational neglect. Neurology, 44(9), 1710–1714.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(4), 286–308.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berteletti, I., Lucangeli, D., Piazza, M., Dehaene, S., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Numerical estimation in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 545–551.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beschin, N., Cazzani, M., Della Sala, S., & Spinazzola, L. (1996). Ignoring left and far: an investigation of tactile neglect. Neuropsychologia, 34(1), 41–49.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beschin, N., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. H. (1997a). What the eyes perceive, the brain ignores: a case of pure unilateral representational neglect. Cortex, 33(1), 3–26.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beschin, N., Cubelli, R., Della Sala, S., & Spinazzola, L. (1997b). Left of what? The role of egocentric coordinates in neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 63(4), 483–489.

    CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E., & Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14(1), 129–133.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain, 125, 2012–2022.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blankenburg, F., Ruff, C. C., Bestmann, S., Bjoertomt, O., Josephs, O., Deichmann, R., et al. (2010). Studying the role of human parietal cortex in visuospatial attention with concurrent TMS-fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2702–2711.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bourlon, C., Duret, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., Azouvi, P., Loeper-Jeny, C., Merat-Blanchard, M., et al. (2010). Vocal response times to real and imagined stimuli in spatial neglect: a group study and single-case report. Cortex, 47(5), 536–546.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1980). Pseudoneglect - effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18(4–5), 491–498.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, J. L., Nettleton, N. C., Nathan, G., & Wilson, L. (1986). Tactual-kinesthetic matching of horizontal extents by the long-term blind—absence or reversal of normal left—side underestimation. Neuropsychologia, 24(2), 261–264.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, J. L., Nettleton, N. C., Wilson, L. E., & Bradshaw, C. S. (1987). Line bisection by left-handed preschoolers: a phenomenon of symmetrical neglect. Brain and Cognition, 6(4), 377–385.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, J. B., & Kirk, A. (1999). Line bisection performance of normal adults: two subgroups with opposite biases. Neurology, 53, 527–532.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brodie, E. E. (2010). Reflecting upon line bisection: mirror reversal increases the magnitude of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1517–1520.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brodie, E. E., & Pettigrew, L. E. L. (1996). Is left always right? Directional deviations in visual line bisection as a function of hand and initial scanning direction. Neuropsychologia, 34(5), 467–470.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R. (1968). Spatial and verbal components of act of recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22(5), 349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, J. L., & Brandimonte, M. (2013). Left-ear-driven representational pseudoneglect for mentally represented real-world scenes created from aural-verbal description. Cognitive Processing. (in press).

  • Brooks, J. L., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. (2011a). Tactile rod bisection in the absence of visuospatial processing in children, mid-age and older adults. Neuropsychologia, 49(12), 3392–3398.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, J. L., Logie, R. H., McIntosh, R. D., & Della Sala, S. (2011b). Representational pseudoneglect for an auditory-driven spatial working memory task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(11), 2168–2180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bultitude, J. H., & Davies, A. M. A. (2006). Putting attention on the line: investigating the activation-orientation hypothesis of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1849–1858.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., Grady, C. L., Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A. R., Tulving, E., Kapur, S., et al. (1997a). Age-related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: a positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(1), 391–400.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., McIntosh, A. R., Tulving, E., Nyberg, L., & Grady, C. L. (1997b). Age-related differences in effective neural connectivity during encoding and recall. Neuroreport, 8(16), 3479–3483.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Houle, S., Mangels, J. A., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Age-related differences in neural activity during item and temporal-order memory retrieval: a positron emission tomography study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 197–206.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., & McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. NeuroImage, 17(3), 1394–1402.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calabria, M., & Rossetti, Y. (2005). Interference between number processing and line bisection: a methodology. Neuropsychologia, 43(5), 779–783.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Silvanto, J., Pascual-Leone, A., & Battelli, L. (2009). The role of the angular gyrus in the modulation of visuospatial attention by the mental number line. NeuroImage, 44(2), 563–568.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Fantino, M., Tinti, C., Silvanto, J., & Vecchi, T. (2010). Crossmodal interaction between the mental number line and peripersonal haptic space representation in sighted and blind individuals. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(4), 885–890.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Fantino, M., Silvanto, J., Tinti, C., & Vecchi, T. (2011a). Blind individuals show pseudoneglect in bisecting numerical intervals. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(4), 1021–1028.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Fantino, M., Silvanto, J., Vallar, G., & Vecchi, T. (2011b). Tapping effects on numerical bisection. Experimental Brain Research, 208, 21–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Fantino, M., Tinti, C., Pascual-Leone, A., Silvanto, J., & Vecchi, T. (2011c). Spatial biases in peripersonal space in sighted and blind individuals revealed by a haptic line bisection paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(4), 1110–1121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Fantino, M., Mancini, F., Mattioli, F. C., & Vallar, G. (2012a). Listening to numbers affects visual and haptic bisection in healthy individuals and neglect patients. Neuropsychologia, 50(5), 913–925.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, Z., Lega, C., Vecchi, T., & Vallar, G. (2012b). Listening to white noise counteracts visual and haptic pseudoneglect. Perception, 41(11), 1395–1398.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, P., Goedert, K. M., Murray, E., Kelly, K., Ahmeti, S., & Barrett, A. M. (2011). Spatial bias and right hemisphere function: sex-specific changes with aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(3), 455–462.

    CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chokron, S., & De Agostini, M. (1995). Reading habits and line bisection: a developmental approach. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(1), 51–58.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chokron, S., & Imbert, M. (1993). Influence of reading habits on line bisection. Cognitive Brain Research, 1(4), 219–222.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chokron, S., Colliot, P., Bartolomeo, P., Rhein, F., Eusop, E., Vassel, P., et al. (2002). Visual, proprioceptive and tactile performance in left neglect patients. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 1965–1976.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Çiçek, M., Deouell, L. Y., & Knight, R. T. (2009). Brain activity during landmark and line bisection tasks. Frontiers Human Neuroscience, 3, 7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cocchini, G., Watling, R., Della Sala, S., & Jansari, A. (2007). Pseudoneglect in back space. Brain and Cognition, 63(1), 79–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coolican, J., Eskes, G. A., McMullen, P. A., & Lecky, E. (2008). Perceptual biases in processing facial identity and emotion. Brain and Cognition, 66(2), 176–187.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coudereau, J. P., Gueguen, N., Pratte, M., & Sampaio, E. (2006). Tactile precision in right-handed archery experts with visual disabilities: a pseudoneglect effect? Laterality, 11(2), 170–180.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cowie, R., & Hamill, G. (1998). Variation among nonclinical subjects on a line bisection task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 834.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J., & Ferber, S. (2006). Revisiting unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), 987–1006.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darling, S., Logie, R. H., & Della Sala, S. (2012). Representational pseudoneglect in line bisection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(5), 879–883.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Agostini, M., Curt, F., Tzortzis, C., & Dellatolas, G. (1999). Comparing left and right hand in line bisection at different ages. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15(3), 379–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Della Sala, S., Gray, C., Baddeley, A., Allamano, N., & Wilson, L. (1999). Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuospatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 37(10), 1189–1199.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Della Sala, S., Darling, S., & Logie, R. H. (2010). Items on the left are better remembered. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(5), 848–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dellatolas, G., Coutin, T., & De Agostini, M. (1996). Bisection and perception of horizontal lines in normal children. Cortex, 32, 705–715.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Di Luca, S., Pesenti, M., Vallar, G., & Girelli, L. (2013). Numbers reorient visuospatial attention during cancellation tasks. Experimental Brain Research, 225(4), 549–557.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, C. A., & Intraub, H. (2009). Spatial asymmetries in viewing and remembering scenes: Consequences of an attentional bias? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), 1251–1262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolcos, F., Rice, H. J., & Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 819–825.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ebersbach, M., Luwel, K., Frick, A., Onghena, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2008). The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5-to 9-year old children: evidence for a segmented linear model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99(1), 1–17.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Failla, C. V., Sheppard, D. M., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2003). Age and responding-hand related changes in performance of neurologically normal subjects on the line-bisection and chimeric-faces tasks. Brain and Cognition, 52(3), 353–363.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Shah, N. J., Weiss, P. H., Halligan, P. W., Grosse-Ruyken, M., et al. (2000a). Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI. Neurology, 54(6), 1324–1331.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Weiss, P. H., Shah, N. J., Toni, I., Halligan, P. W., et al. (2000b). ‘Where’ depends on ‘what’: a differential functional anatomy for position discrimination in one- versus two-dimensions. Neuropsychologia, 38(13), 1741–1748.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Weiss, P. H., & Zilles, K. (2001). The neural basis of vertical and horizontal line bisection judgments: an fMRI study of normal volunteers. NeuroImage, 14(1), S59–S67.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (1996). Bisection performance indicates spatial word representation. Cognitive Brain Research, 4(3), 163–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex, 44(4), 386–392.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., & Stumpp, T. (2001). An investigation of the time course of flanker effects in the landmark task. Neuropsychologia, 39(6), 586–596.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foxe, J. J., McCourt, M. E., & Javitt, D. C. (2003). Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis. NeuroImage, 19(3), 710–726.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, A., Mohr, C., & Brugger, P. (2012). Representational pseudoneglect and reference points both influence geographical location estimates. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(2), 277–284.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fujii, T., Fukatsu, R., Yamadori, A., & Kimura, I. (1995). Effect of age on the line bisection test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(6), 941–944.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gainotti, G. (2010). The role of automatic orienting of attention towards ipsilesional stimuli in non-visual (tactile and auditory) neglect: a critical review. Cortex, 46(2), 150–160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gamberini, L., Seraglia, B., & Priftis, K. (2008). Processing of peripersonal and extrapersonal space using tools: evidence from visual line bisection in real and virtual environments. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1298–1304.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garza, J. P., Eslinger, P. J., & Barrett, A. M. (2008). Perceptual-attentional and motor-intentional bias in near and far space. Brain and Cognition, 68(1), 9–14.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Van den Bossche, L., Fias, W., et al. (2010). Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 180–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilmore, C. S., Clementz, B. A., & Berg, P. (2009). Hemispheric differences in auditory oddball responses during monaural versus binaural stimulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73(3), 326–333.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giorgio, A., Watkins, K. E., Chadwick, M., James, S., Winmill, L., Douaud, G., et al. (2010). Longitudinal changes in grey and white matter during adolescence. NeuroImage, 49(1), 94–103.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gobel, S. M., Calabria, M., Farne, A., & Rossetti, Y. (2006). Parietal rTMS distorts the mental number line: simulating ‘spatial’ neglect in healthy subjects. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), 860–868.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, G., & Shelly, C. (1981). Does the right hemisphere age more rapidly than the left. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3(1), 65–78.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, J., & Snyder, L. H. (2010). Spatial and non-spatial functions of the parietal cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(6), 731–740.

    CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guariglia, C., Padovani, A., Pantano, P., & Pizzamiglio, L. (1993). Unilateral neglect restricted to visual-imagery. Nature, 364(6434), 235–237.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hach, S., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2012). Touching base: the effect of participant and stimulus modulation factors on a haptic line bisection task. Laterality, 17(2), 180–201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., & Wade, D. T. (1989). Visuospatial neglect—underlying factors and test sensitivity. Lancet, 2(8668), 908–911.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halligan, P. W., Manning, L., & Marshall, J. C. (1991). Hemispheric activation vs spatiomotor cueing in visual neglect—a case-study. Neuropsychologia, 29(2), 165–176.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halligan, P. W., Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., & Vallar, G. (2003). Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 125–133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartman, M., & Warren, L. H. (2005). Explaining age differences in temporal working memory. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 645–656.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, M., Milner, A. D., & Roberts, R. C. (1995). An investigation of hemispatial neglect using the landmark task. Brain and Cognition, 27(1), 59–78.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, M., Pool, T. D., Roberson, M. J., & Olk, B. (2000). Effects of visible and invisible cueing procedures on perceptual judgments in young and elderly subjects. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 22–31.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatin, B., Sykes, L., & Tottenham, C. (2012). The relationship between collisions and pseudoneglect: is it right? Cortex, 48(8), 997–1008.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatta, T., & Yamamoyto, M. (1986). Hemispheric asymmetries in a tactile bisection task - effects of hemispace of presentation. Neuropsychologia, 24(2), 265–269.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, M., Ergun, G., Yazgan, Y., & Gunturkun, O. (2002). Sex differences in line bisection as a function of hand. Neuropsychologia, 40(3), 235–240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, M., Waldie, K. E., & Corballis, M. C. (2003). Developmental changes in line bisection: a result of callosal maturation? Neuropsychology, 17(1), 155–160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, M., Kirk, I. J., & Corballis, M. C. (2004). Influence of task complexity on manual asymmetries. Cortex, 40(1), 103–110.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heber, I. A., Siebertz, S., Wolter, M., Kuhlen, T., & Fimm, B. (2010). Horizontal and vertical pseudoneglect in peri- and extrapersonal space. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), 160–166.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., & Valenstein, E. (1979). Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect. Annals of Neurology, 5(2), 166–170.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., & Van Den Abell, T. (1979). Right hemispheric dominance for mediating cerebral activation. Neuropsychologia, 17(3–4), 315–321.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(6), 435–448.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, V., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Distinct cerebral pathways for object identity and number in human infants. PLoS Biology, 6(2), e11.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 93–110.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jimenez-Jimenez, J. F., Calleja, M., Alonso-Navarro, H., Rubio, L., Navacerrada, F., Pilo-de-la-Fuente, B., et al. (2011). Influence of age and gender in motor performance in healthy subjects. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 302(1–2), 72–80.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, W., Logie, R. H., & Brockmole, J. R. (2010). Working memory tasks differ in factor structure across age cohorts: implications for dedifferentiation. Intelligence, 38, 513–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kadosh, R. C., Muggleton, N., Silvanto, J., & Walsh, V. (2010). Double dissociation of format-dependent and number-specific neurons in human parietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 20(9), 2166–2171.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kazandjian, S., Cavezian, C., Zivotofsky, A. Z., & Chokron, S. (2010). Bisections in two languages: When number processing, spatial representation, and habitual reading direction interact. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 4031–4037.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1970). Cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychologica, 33, 193.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klisz, D. (1978). Neuropsychological evaluation in older persons.

  • Laeng, B., Buchtel, H. A., & Butter, C. M. (1996). Tactile rod bisection: hemispheric activation and sex differences. Neuropsychologia, 34(11), 1115–1121.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazzouni, L., Ross, B., Voss, P., & Lepore, F. (2010). Neuromagnetic auditory steady-state responses to amplitude modulated sounds following dichotic or monaural presentation. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121(2), 200–207.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levander, M., Tegner, R., & Caneman, G. (1993). Tactile line-bisection in normal subjects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76(3), 831–836.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., & Brugger, P. (2007). Exploring number space by random digit generation. Experimental Brain Research, 180, 655–665.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., & Brugger, P. (2008a). Looking for the answer: The mind’s eye in number space. Neuroscience, 151(3), 725–729.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., Schwarz, U., Schubiger, M., & Brugger, P. (2008b). Head turns bias the brain’s internal random generator. Current Biology, 18, R60–R62.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., Nicholls, M. E. R., Towse, J. N., Bradshaw, J. L., & Brugger, P. (2010). Lucky numbers: spatial neglect affects physical, but not representational, choices in a lotto task. Cortex, 46(5), 685–690.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, A. M., Nicholls, M. E. R., Mattingley, J. B., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2008a). Left to right: Representational biases for numbers and the effect of visuomotor adaptation. Cognition, 107(3), 1048–1058.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, A. M., Nicholls, M. E. R., Mattingley, J. B., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2008b). Numerical processing overcomes left neglect for the Greyscales task. Neuroreport, 19(8), 835–838.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, A. M., Nicholls, M. E. R., Mattingley, J. B., Chapman, H. L., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2009). Pseudoneglect for the bisection of mental number lines. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(5), 925–945.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2006). On the nature of near space: effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), 977–981.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2007). Spatial attention and the mental number line: evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1400–1407.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2010). Bisecting the mental number line in near and far space. Brain and Cognition, 72(3), 362–367.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., Lourenco, S. F., & Francisco, A. (2012). Approaching stimuli bias attention in numerical space. Acta Psychologica, 140, 129–132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2009). Multiple spatial representations of number: evidence for co-existing compressive and linear scales. Experimental Brain Research, 193(1), 151–156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, L., Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1990). Individual variation in line bisection: a study of normal subjects with application to the interpretation of visual neglect. Neuropsychologia, 28, 647–655.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattingley, J. B., Pierson, J. M., Bradshaw, J. L., Phillips, J. G., & Bradshaw, J. A. (1993). To see or not to see—the effects of visible and invisible cues on line bisection judgments in unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia, 31(11), 1201–1215.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattingley, J. B., Berberovic, N., Corben, L., Slavin, M. J., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2004). The Greyscales task: a perceptual measure following unilateral hemispheric of attentional bias damage. Neuropsychologia, 42(3), 387–394.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt, M. E., & Garlinghouse, M. (2000). Asymmetries of visuospatial attention are modulated by viewing distance and visual field elevation: pseudoneglect in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cortex, 36(5), 715–731.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt, M. E., Garlinghouse, M., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2005). Unilateral visual cueing and asymmetric line geometry share a common attentional origin in the modulation of pseudoneglect. Cortex, 41(4), 499–511.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGeorge, P., Beschin, N., Colnaghi, A., Rusconi, M. L., & Della Sala, S. (2007). A lateralized bias in mental imagery: evidence for representational pseudoneglect. Neuroscience Letters, 421(3), 259–263.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, K. M., Craggs, J. G., Benjamin, M. L., Crosson, B., & White, K. D. (2009). Age-related changes in motor control during unimanual movements. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 3(4), 317–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, R. D., Schindler, I., Birchall, D., & Milner, A. D. (2005). Weights and measures: a new look at bisection behaviour in neglect. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 833–850.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, A. D., Brechmann, M., & Pagliarini, L. (1992). To halve and to halve not—an analysis of line bisection judgments in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 30(6), 515–526.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2009). Children’s Early mental number line: logarithmic or decomposed linear? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 503–515.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mohr, H. M., Mues, Robol, V., & Sireteanu. (2010). Altered mental number line in Amblyopia: reduced pseudoneglect corresponds to a decreased bias in number estimation. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1775–1781.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, M. J., Hole, G. J., & Glennerster, A. (1990). Biases and sensitivities in geometrical illusions. Vision Research, 30(11), 1793–1810.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mort, D. J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S. K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C., et al. (2003). The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain, 126, 1986–1997.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nalcaci, E., Cicek, M., Kalaycioglu, C., & Yavuzer, S. (1997). Pseudoneglect of males and females on a spatial, short-term memory task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84(1), 99–105.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nichelli, P., Rinaldi, M., & Cubelli, R. (1989). Selective spatial attention and length representation in normal subjects and in patients with unilateral spatial neglect. Brain and Cognition, 9(1), 57–70.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., & Loftus, A. M. (2007). Pseudoneglect and neglect for mental alphabet lines. Brain Research, 1152, 130–138.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., & McIlroy, A. M. (2010). Spatial cues affect mental number line bisections. Experimental Psychology, 57(4), 315–319.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., & Roberts, G. R. (2002). Can free-viewing perceptual asymmetries be explained by scanning, pre-motor or attentional biases? Cortex, 38(2), 113–136.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Mattingley, J. B., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2005). The effect of strategy on pseudoneglect for luminance judgements. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 71–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Loftus, A., Mayer, K., & Mattingley, J. (2007). Things that go bump in the right: the effect of unimanual activity on rightward collisions. Neuropsychologia, 45(5), 1122–1126.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Kramer, A., & Loftus, A. (2008a). Pseudoneglect for mental alphabet lines is affected by prismatic adaptation. Experimental Brain Research, 191(1), 109–115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Loftus, A. M., & Gevers, W. (2008b). Look, no hands: a perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(2), 413–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Loftus, A. M., Orr, C. A., & Barre, N. (2008c). Rightward collisions and their association with pseudoneglect. Brain and Cognition, 68(2), 166–170.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nijboer, T. C. W., Kollen, B. J., & Kwakkel, G. (2013). Time course of visuospatial neglect early after stroke: a longitudinal cohort study original research article. Cortex, 49(8), 2021–2027.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, J. F., & Bartholomew, A. N. (2011). Blindness enhances tactile acuity and haptic 3-D shape discrimination. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(7), 2323–2331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H. C., Kaufmann, L., Zoppoth, S., & Willmes, K. (2004). On the development of the mental number line: More, less, or never holistic with increasing age? Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1199–1211.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri, M., Rausei, V., Koch, G., Torriero, S., Turriziani, P., & Caltigrone, C. (2004). Overestimation of numerical distances in the left side of space. Neurology, 63(11), 2139–2141.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Opfer, J. E., & Furlong, E. E. (2011). How numbers bias preschoolers’ spatial search. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 682–695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Opfer, J. E., Thompson, C. A., & Furlong, E. E. (2010). Early development of spatial-numeric associations: evidence from spatial and quantitative performance of preschoolers. Developmental Science, 13(5), 761–771.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paiement, P., Champoux, F., Bacon, B. A., Lassonde, M., Gagne, J. P., Mensour, B., et al. (2008). Functional reorganization of the human auditory pathways following hemispherectomy: an fMRI demonstration. Neuropsychologia, 46(12), 2936–2942.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299–320.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paterson, A., & Zangwill, O. L. (1944). Disorders of visual space perception associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain, 67, 331–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philip, J., & Hatwell, Y. (1998). Effects of cueing and of the direction of scanning on the tactile line bisection of normal adults. Cahiers De Psychologie Cognitive-Current Psychology of Cognition, 17(1), 31–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porac, C., Searleman, A., & Karagiannakis, K. (2006). Pseudoneglect: evidence for both perceptual and attentional factors. Brain and Cognition, 61(3), 305–311.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 4(7), 1863–1874.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prakash, R. S., Erickson, K. I., Colcombe, S. J., Kim, J. S., Voss, M. W., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Age-related differences in the involvement of the prefrontal cortex in attentional control. Brain and Cognition, 71(3), 328–335.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prodan, C. I., Orbelo, D. M., & Ross, E. D. (2007). Processing of facial blends of emotion: support for right hemisphere cognitive aging. Cortex, 43(2), 196–206.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Przybyla, A., Haaland, K. Y., Bagesteiro, L. B., & Sainburg, R. L. (2011). Motor asymmetry reduction in older adults. Neuroscience Letters, 489(2), 99–104.

    CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pulsipher, D. T., Seidenberg, M., & Hermann, B. P. (2009). Corpus callosum maturation and line bisection performance in healthy children. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 3(4), 307–316.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Regolin, L. (2006). The case of the line-bisection: when both humans and chickens wander left. Cortex, 42, 101–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter Lorenz, P. A., Kinsbourne, M., & Moscovitch, M. (1990). Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain and Cognition, 12(2), 240–266.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetz, C., et al. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 174–187.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, I. H., & Marshall, J. (1993). Unilateral neglect: clinical and experimental studies. Hove: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I., & Palmer, S. (1990). The legacy of Gestalt psychology. Scientific American, 263(6), 84–90.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roig, M., & Cicero, F. (1994). Hemisphericity style, sex, and performance on a line-bisection task—an exploratory-study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78(1), 115–120.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roman, A., Fathi, E., & Santiago, J. (2013). Spatial biases in understanding descriptions of static scenes: the role of reading and writing direction. Memory and Cognition, 41(4), 588–599.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rorden, C., & Karnath, H. O. (2004). Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(10), 813–819.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusconi, E., Dervinis, M., Verbruggen, F., & Chambers, C. (2013). Critical time course of right fronto-parietal involvement in mental number space. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(3), 465–483.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403–428.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30(4), 507–514.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sampaio, E., & Chokron, S. (1992). Pseudoneglect and reversed pseudoneglect among left-handers and right-handers. Neuropsychologia, 30(9), 797–805.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sampaio, E., & Philip, J. (1991). Sensory and motor aspects of pseudoneglect, hemifield, and hemispace in the tactile modality. Brain and Cognition, 16(1), 74–82.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sampaio, E., Gouarir, C., & Mvondo, D. (1995). Tactile and visual bisection tasks by sighted and blind children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 11(1), 109–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, I., Clavagnier, S., Karnath, H. O., Derex, L., & Perenin, M. T. (2006). A common basis for visual and tactile exploration deficits in spatial neglect? Neuropsychologia, 44(8), 1444–1451.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, R., & Peigneux, P. (2011). Age-related changes in visual pseudoneglect. Brain and Cognition, 76(3), 382–389.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, R., Dehon, H., & Peigneux, P. (2013). Lateralized processing of false memories and pseudoneglect in aging. Cortex, 49(5), 1314–1324.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenwiesner, M., Krumbholz, K., Ruebsamen, R., Fink, G. R., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2007). Hemispheric asymmetry for auditory processing in the human auditory brain stem, thalamus, and cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 17(2), 492–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sisk, C. L., & Zehr, J. L. (2005). Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26(3–4), 163–174.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stam, C. J., & Bakker, M. (1990). The prevalence of neglect—superiority of neuropsychological over clinical methods of estimation. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 92(3), 229–235.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, J. L., Juhasz, B. J., Slattery, T. J., & Barth, H. C. (2011). Adults’ number-line estimation strategies: evidence from eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(3), 557–563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Urbanski, M., Duffau, H., Volle, E., Lévy, R., Dubois, P., et al. (2005). Direct evidence for a parietal-frontal pathway subserving spatial awareness in humans. Science, 309(5744), 2226–2228.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell’Acqua, F., Frokel, S., Simmons, A., Vergani, F., Murphy, D., et al. (2011). A lateralised brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 14(10), 1245–1246.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Tomaiuolo, F., Aiello, M., Merola, S., Silvetti, M., Lecce, F., et al. (2012). Damage to white matter pathways in subacute and chronic spatial neglect: a group study and 2 single-case studies with complete virtual “in vivo” tractography dissection. Cerebral Cortex, 11.

  • Thiel, A., & Sireteanu. (2009). Strabismic amblyopes show a bilateral rightward bias in a line bisection task: evidence for a visual attention deficit. Vision Research, 49, 287–294.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toba, M.-N., Cavanagh, P., & Bartolomeo, P. (2011). Attention biases the perceived midpoint of horizontal lines. Neuropsychologia, 49(2), 238–246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turnbull, O. H., & McGeorge, P. (1998). Lateral bumping: a normal subject analogue to the behaviour of patients with hemispatial neglect. Brain and Cognition, 37, 31–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Umilta, C., Priftis, K., & Zorzi, M. (2009). The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect. Experimental Brain Research, 192(3), 561–569.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Urbanski, M., & Bartolomeo, P. (2008). Line bisection in left neglect: the importance of starting right. Cortex, 44(7), 782–793.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallesi, A., McIntosh, A. R., Kovacevic, N., Chan, S. C. C., & Stuss, D. T. (2010). Age effects on the asymmetry of the motor system: evidence from cortical oscillatory activity. Biological Psychology, 85(2), 213–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijck, J. P., Gevers, W., Lafosse, C., Doricchi, F., & Fias, W. (2011). Non-spatial neglect for the mental number line. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2570–2583.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijck, J., Gevers, W., Lafosse, C., & Fias, W. (2012). The heterogeneous nature of number–space interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 182.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Vugt, P., Fransen, I., Creten, W., & Paquier, P. (2000). Line bisection performances of 650 normal children. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), 886–895.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varnava, A., & Halligan, P. W. (2007). Influence of age and sex on line bisection: a study of normal performance with implications for visuospatial neglect. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(6), 571–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varnava, A., & Halligan, P. W. (2009). Line bisection: does introspection inform cognitive strategy? Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 280–283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varnava, A., McCarthy, M., & Beaumont, J. G. (2002). Line bisection in normal adults: direction of attentional bias for near and far space. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), 1372–1378.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varnava, A., Dervinis, M., & Chambers, C. (2013). The predictive nature of pseudoneglect for visual neglect: evidence from parietal theta burst stimulation. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e65851.

    CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velanova, K., Lustig, C., Jacoby, L. L., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Evidence for frontally mediated controlled processing differences in older adults. Cerebral Cortex, 17(5), 1033–1046.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, H. K. (1923). On the estimation of the middle of lines. American Journal of Psychology, 34, 313–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, M., Gnanakumaran, V., & Goldreich, D. (2011). Tactile spatial acuity enhancement in blindness: evidence for experience-dependent mechanisms. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(19), 7028–7037.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, C., Guo, X., Jin, Z., Sun, J., Qiu, Y., Zhu, Y., et al. (2011). Influences of brain development and ageing on cortical interactive networks. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122(2), 278–283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zivotofsky, A. Z. (2004). Choosing sides: lateralization in line trisection and quadrisection as a function of reading direction and handedness. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(2), 206–211.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Ph.D. studentship from The Universita’ Suor Orsola Benincasa awarded to JB.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joanna L. Brooks.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brooks, J.L., Della Sala, S. & Darling, S. Representational Pseudoneglect: A Review. Neuropsychol Rev 24, 148–165 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-013-9245-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-013-9245-2

Keywords

Navigation