Skip to main content

The Development of the Person: An Experiential Perspective on the Ontogenesis of Psychological Complexity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Applications of Flow in Human Development and Education

Abstract

The obvious answer to the question “What is a person?” would probably focus on physical characteristics, for example, “An individual member of the human race.” Of the 14 major usages of the word listed in the Oxford Dictionary of the English Language, most refer to such natural, biological attributes.

Research reported in this chapter was largely funded by grants from the Spencer Foundation.

Reprinted with permission in W. Damon (EiC) Handbook of Child Psychology, R. Lerner (Ed.) Vol. 1. Theoretical models of Human Development, pp 635-685. New York: Wiley © 1997 Wiley

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Quotations not otherwise attributed are taken from interviews the authors and other members of the University of Chicago research team collected in the course of a project entitled Creativity in Later Life, sponsored by the Spencer Foundation (see Csikszentmihalyi 1996).

  2. 2.

    It is worth pointing out again that when the person is defined relationally, as in this chapter, it can be misleading to fall into the familiar use in the terms subject versus object, inner versus outer, and so on. This terminology tends to isolate the person from the world, which is not our intention. On the contrary, it is more consistent with our perspective to say that the “location” of the person is neither inner nor outer, or, perhaps better, is both at once.

  3. 3.

    Our focus here remains on immediate subjective experience, but it is possible to adopt other time frames and perceive the same dialectical tension. In other words, one may overcome the anxiety of an entire week, month, or year by finding a way to build new skills. As mentioned earlier, the same is true of the notion of equilibrium; that is , it can refer to immediate experience or stages that characterize larger periods of time.

  4. 4.

    Although the focus here, as in much of the chapter, is on psychological processes, creativity cannot be reduced to this level.

  5. 5.

    We will have more to say later in the chapter about this interpersonal dynamic, and about how qualities such as agency and communion in children may be nurtured in family interaction. For instance, a mother’s communion has often acted as a buffer for the father’s agency, and vice versa. This traditional, sex-typed alliance is but one ``solution” for creating a family context that spares children the fate of growing up in a home that overemphasizes one or the other quality and thus forces children into one pattern of response. We will return to this observation when considering how early experience within the family may have consequences for attaining complexity in later life. For now, we point out that parents with androgynous parenting styles have reported more enjoyment in parenting (Lamb, 1982).

  6. 6.

    Lerner(1984), in addition, contains an in-depth, multidisciplinary look at human plasticity, its foundation in evolutionary processes, and the developmental importance of flexible self regulation.

  7. 7.

    It is worth noting that this alignment of objectivity and subjectivity with masculine and feminine characteristics is best suited to instrumental domains, where it is men who have traditionally had to learn to accommodate to reality demands; this alignment would often be reversed in expressive, social activities, where women have had to assume more objective modes of self-sacrifice.

  8. 8.

    Too often the word discipline is equated with punishment. The word is a derivation of the Latin discipulus, meaning pupil. This meaning reflects the idea that discipline is about training the mind and character through experience. Insofar as punishment furthers such training or instruction, its meaning is consistent with discipline.

  9. 9.

    If reacting to a “more powerful” father is associated with learning habits of accommodation, then the increasing absence of father involvement in modern homes could help to explain the apparent decline of social integration in many communities.

  10. 10.

    The attachment literature typically describes optimal parenting in terms of a child-centered approach. This is underscored by the fact that most attachment researchers view material insensitivity as a mother's inability to take the perspective of a child (see Ainsworth 1983).

  11. 11.

    Challenges are "discovered" by children when child-centered parents structure the environment in ways that are sensitive to children's interests and thus more conducive to a discovery orientation.

  12. 12.

    Perhaps these two general patterns of socialization—one more suited for attempts to differentiate and "break" with tradition, and one more suited for integrative attempts to "build" on tradition—can help to explain the often-cited emphasis on individuality in the West and on social connection in the East. In addition, flow experience in the West is more often a private thing (e.g., in recreation or leisure), whereas flow in strongly adult-centered cultures (e.g., tribal cultures) is often a matter of public ceremony and ritual (see Turner 1979, on flow and ritual).

  13. 13.

    To the extent that both parenting approaches characterize one home context, a child presumably benefits. It is also worth noting, however, that a synthesis of sorts might take place when a child gets experience with both patterns as a result of different contexts (e.g., home and school), or perhaps as a result of experience with different age playmates—sometimes having to "follow" and accommodate, and sometimes having to "lead" and organize others' efforts.

  14. 14.

    Analogously, one can think of the scientific process as syntelic, as an oscillation between theoretical (subjective) and empirical (objective) modes of "control." emergence of basic human dualisms (e.g., mind/body, self/other, truth/falsity) and the eventual overcoming of such dualisms with full development. The legacy of play can thus be seen in the syntelic character of Baldwin's highest form of thought, aesthetic contemplation. As illustrated earlier, his descriptions of aesthetic modes are remarkably close to contemporary perspectives on post-formal thought processes, and to our remarks on flow experience: "In aesthetic experience the partial insights of intelligence and feeling are mutually conserved and supplemented" (1911, p. 279). His perspective, though, adds insight to the developmental history of such outcomes; in other words, play is germinal of the highest forms of human thought as its syntelic character is elaborated and reinstated on higher levels of organizations.

  15. 15.

    An interesting research hypothesis is that child-centered parenting enhances exploratory play, and adult-centered parenting encourages playful imitation.

  16. 16.

    In Baldwin's terminology, to "play" in adulthood means having aesthetic experiences that allow the reconciliation of the various partial truths (e.g., feeling and intellect, inner and outer).

References

  • Adlai-Gail, W. S. (1994). Exploring the autotelic personality. Doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adler, M. J. (1927). Dialectic. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1969). Some contemporary patterns in the feeding situation. In A. Ambrose (Ed.), Stimulation in early infancy. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altman, I. (1975). Environment and social behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altman, I., Vinsel, Α., & Brown, B. (1981). Dialectical conceptions in social psychology: An application to social penetration and privacy regulation. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 107–160). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, J. (1989). Life span aspects of reproduction and parental care in anthropoid primates. In J. Lancaster, J. Altmann, A. Rossi, & L. Sherrod (Eds.), Parenting across the life span: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 15–29). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, E. (1990). Streetwise: Race, class and change in an urban community. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apter, M. (1989). Reversal theory. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bachnik, J. (1992). The two “faces” of self and society in Japan. Ethos, 20, 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Brown, J. (1980). Early interactions: Consequences for social and mental development at three years. Child Development, 51, 437–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). (M. Holquist (Ed.), C. Emerson & M. Holquist Trans.) The dialogical imagination: Four essays. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1906). Thought and things: A study of the development and meaning of thought (Vol. 1). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1908). Thought and things: A study of the development and meaning of thought (Vol. 2). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1911). Thought and things: A study of the development and meaning of thought (Vols. 3–4). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (1990). Toward a psychology of wisdom and its ontogenesis. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 87–120). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 33, 344–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. (1950). Ecological psychology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, F. (1969). Creative person and creative process. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basseches, Μ. Α. (1980). Dialectical schemata: A framework for the empirical study of the development of dialectical thinking. Human Development, 23, 400–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1987). A developmental perspective on adolescent risk taking behavior in contemporary America. In C. E. Irwin (Ed.), Adolescent social behavior and health (pp. 93–125). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 349–378). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bee, H. (1992). The journey of adulthood. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171–1190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Curiosity and exploration. Science, 153, 25–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. H. (1973). Conceptions of sex-roles: Some cross-cultural and longitudinal perspectives. American Psychologist, 28, 512–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. H. (1982). Assimilation, accommodation, and the dynamics of personality development. Child Development, 53, 281–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Development of cognition, affect, and social relations. Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 13). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolk, L. (1926). Das Problem der Menschwerdung. Jena, Germany: Gustav Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortz, W. (1996). Dare to be 100. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 49–76). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brent, S. B., & Watson, D. (1980, November). Aging and wisdom: Individual and collective aspects. Paper presented at the third annual meeting of the Gerontological Society, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I. (1987). New perspectives on attachment relations: Security, communication, and internal working models. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 1061–1100). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child development (pp. 187–249). London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., & Freeman-Moir. (Eds.). (1982). The cognitive developmental psychology of James Mark Baldwin: Current theory and research in genetic epistemology. Norwood, NJ: ABLEX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1972). The nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27, 687–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1986). Value presuppositions of developmental theory. In L. Cirillo & S. Wapner (Eds.), Value presuppositions in theories of human development (pp. 19–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Jolly, Α., & Sylva, Κ. (Eds.). (1976). Play: Its role in development and evolution. New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cairns, R. B., & Hood, Κ. E. (1983). Continuity in social development: A comparative perspective on individual differences prediction. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 301–58). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chagnon, N. (1979). Mate competition favoring close kin, and village fissioning among the Yanomamo Indians. In N. A. Chagnon & W. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior (pp. 86–132). North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinen, A. B. (1984). Modal logic: A new paradigm of development and late-life potential. Human Development, 27, 42–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, G. N., & Seifer, R. (1983). Facilitating mother-infant communication: A treatment model for high-risk and developmentally delayed infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 4, 67–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, V. P., & Birren, J. E. (1980). The development of wisdom across the life span: A reexamination of an ancient topic. In P. B. Baltes & O. R. Brim (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 103–135). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Scribners.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley, C. H. (1961). The social self. In T. Parsons, E. Shils, K. Naegele, & J. Pitts (Eds.), Theories of society: Foundations of modern sociological theory (pp. 822–828). New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, C. R., Grotevant, H. D., & Condon, S. M. (1983). Individuality and connectedness in the family as a context for adolescent identity formation and role-taking skill. In H. D. Grotevant & C. R. Cooper (Eds.), Adolescent development in the family (pp. 43–59). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Still stable after all these years: Personality as a key to some issues in adulthood and old age. In P. B. Baltes & J. O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 64–103). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.). (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1990). The psychology of wisdom: An evolutionary interpretation. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 25–51). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1993). The measurement of flow in everyday life: Towards a theory of emergent motivation. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 40, pp. 57–98)., Developmental perspectives on motivation Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented teenagers: The roots of success and failure. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W. (1983). Social and personality development. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W. (1995). Greater expectations: Overcoming the culture of indulgence in America’s homes and schools. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Flaste, R. (1995). Why we do what we do: The dynamics of personal autonomy. New York: Grosset.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and effort in education. Cambridge, MA: Riverside.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Doi, T. (1986). The anatomy of self. Tokyo: Kodansha.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, M. J. (1973). Why people play. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, Ε. H. (1977). Toys and reasons. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, Ε. H. (1982). The life cycle completed. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H., Erikson, J. M., & Kivnick, H. Q. (1986). Vital involvement in old age: The experience of old age in our time. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1973). Eysenck on extroversion. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fagen, R. M. (1976). Modeling: How and why play works. In J. S. Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 96–115). New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (1977). Effects of early separation, interactive deficits, and experimental manipulations on infant-mother face-to-face interaction. Child Development, 48, 763–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (1984). Separation stress of young children transferring to new schools. Developmental Psychology, 20, 786–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (1985). Attachment as psychobiological attunement: Being on the same wave length. In M. Reite & T. Field (Eds.), Psychobiology of attachment (pp. 415–454). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (1987). Affective and interactive disturbances in infants. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 972–1005). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory: An integrative approach. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortune, R. F. (1963). Sorcerers of Dobu. New York: Dutton (Original work published in 1932).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, B. A. (1988a). Interaction as a diagnostic resource in tutoring (Tech. Rep. No. 88-3). Boulder: University of Colorado, Institute of Cognitive Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, B. A. (1988b). Cognitive and interactional aspects of correction in tutoring (Tech. Rep. No. 88-2). Boulder: University of Colorado, Institute of Cognitive Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. G. (1979). Human sociobiology. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1922). Beyond the pleasure principle. London: Hogarth Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1959). Creative writers and daydreaming. In J. S. Strackey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 9). London: Hogarth Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1995). Leading minds: An anatomy of leadership. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. (1975). Creativity: Prospects and issues. In I. Taylor & J. W. Getzels (Eds.), Perspectives in creativity (pp. 326–344). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilkey, L. (1990). Gilkey on Tillich. New York: Crossroad.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Women’s conception of self and of morality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C., Lyons, N. P., & Hanmer, T. J. (Eds.). (1990). Making connections. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday Anchor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groos, K. (1901). The play of man. New York: Appleton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadamard, T. (1954). The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardy-Brown, K., Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1981). Genetic and environmental influences on the rate of communicative development in the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 17, 704–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, L. M. (1992, December 2-6). Ritual art and the production of Hindu selves. Paper presented at the American Anthropological Association meetings, San Francisco, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspectives on the self-system. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. M. Heatherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 275–385). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, S. (1991). Adolescents and their families. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havighurst, R. J. (1953). Human development and education. New York: Longmans, Green.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heber, M. (1981). Instruction versus conversation as opportunities for learning. In W. P. Robinson (Ed.), Communications in development. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, B. B. (1984a). Parents and exploration: The effect of context on individual differences in exploratory behavior. Child Development, 55, 1237–1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, B. B. (1984b). Social support and exploration. Child Development, 55, 1246–1251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, R. (1987). Personality psychology: Back to basics. In A. J. Aronoff, A. I. Rabin, & R. A. Zucker (Eds.), The emergence of personality (pp. 79–104). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holliday, S. G., & Chandler, M. J. (1986). Wisdom: Explorations in adult competence. Basel, Switzerland: Krager.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homans, P. (1979). Jung in context: Modernity and the making of psychology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huizinga, J. (1955). Homo ludens. Boston: Beacon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, J. S. (1942). Evolution: The modern synthesis. New York: Harper and Brothers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, C. E. (Ed.). (1987). Adolescent social behavior and health. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isabella, R. Α., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 62, 373–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Johanson, D. C., & Edey, M. A. (1981). Lucy: The beginnings of humankind. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Press, Harvard University, & Jung, C. G. (1946). Psychological types. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1954). The development of personality. In H. Read, et al. (Eds.), Collected works (Vol. 17). New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1959). Aion: Researches into the phenomenology of the self. In H. Read, et al. (Eds.), Collected works (Vol. 9). New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1960). The structure and dynamics of the psyche. In H. Read, et al. (Eds.), Collected works (Vol. 8). New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1961). The theory of psychoanalysis. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahlbaugh, P. (1993). James Mark Baldwin: A bridge between social and cognitive theories of development. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 23, 79–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kakar, S. (1978). The inner world: A psychoanalytic study of childhood and society in India. New Delhi: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kojima, H. (1986). Child rearing concepts as a belief-value system of the society and the individual. In H. Stevenson, H. Azuma, & K. Hakuta (Eds.), Child development and education in Japan. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kouzes, J. M., & Posner, B. Z. (1995). The leadership challenge. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, D. A. (1983). Post-formal operations? A need for further conceptualization. Human Development, 26, 91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kris, E. (1952). Psychoanalytic explorations in art. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (1978). Mechanisms of cognitive and social development: One psychology or two? Human Development, 21, 92–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Y. (1976). Chinese dialectical thought and character. In J. F. Rychlak (Ed.), Dialectic: Humanistic rationale for behavior and development (pp. 72–86). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1980). Beyond formal operations: Uses and limits of pure logic in life span development. Human Development, 23, 141–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1982). Dynamic development and mature autonomy. Human Development, 25, 161–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1990). Wisdom as integrated thought: Historical and developmental perspectives. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 52–83). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. (Ed.). (1982). Nontraditional families: Parenting and child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., Mannell, R., & Zuzanek, J. (1986). Daily well-being of older adults with family and friends. Psychology and Aging, 1(2), 117–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., & Richards, M. H. (1994). Divergent realities: The emotional lives of mothers, fathers, and adolescents. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt, H. J., Pondy, L. R., & Boje, D. M. (1989). Readings in managerial psychology (4th ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M. (1976). Concepts and theories of human development. Reading, MS: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M. (1991). Changing organism-context relations as the basic process of development: A developmental contextual perspective. Developmental Psychology, 27, 27–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. (Eds.). (1981). Individuals as producers of their development: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M., & Lerner, J. (1987). Children in their contexts: A goodness-of-fit model. In J. Lancaster, J. Altmann, A. Rossi, & L. Sherrod (Eds.), Parenting across the life span (pp. 377–404). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Vine, R. (1980). Adulthood among the Gusii. In N. Smelser & E. Erikson (Eds.), Themes of work and love in adulthood (pp. 77–104). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, D. J. (1980). Toward a conception of the adult life course. In N. Smelser & E. Erikson (Eds.), Themes of work and love in adulthood (pp. 265–90). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, D. J. (1986). A conception of adult development. American Psychologist, 41, 3–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lévi-Strauss, C. (1967). Tristes tropiques. New York: Atheneum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Rosenblum, L. Α. (Eds.). (1974). The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. C. (1981). On constraints and adaptation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 244–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loevinger, J. (1966). The meaning and measurement of ego development. American Psychologist, 21, 195–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, Κ. (1971). Studies in animal and human behavior (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, Ε. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & Ε. M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, A. (1984). After virtue: A study in moral theory. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marriott, M. (1976). Hindu transactions: Diversity without dualism. In B. Kepferer (Ed.), Transaction and meaning: Directions in the anthropology of exchange and symbolic behavior. Philadelphia: ISHI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martini, M. (1994). Peer interaction in Polynesia: A view from the Marquesas. In J. L. Roopnarine, J. E. Johnson, & F. H. Hooper (Eds.), Children’s play in diverse cultures (pp. 73–103). Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martini, M., & Kirkpatrick, J. (1992). Parenting in Polynesia: A view from the Marquesas. In J. L. Roopnarine & D. B. Carter (Eds.), Annual advances in applied developmental psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 199–222). Norwood, NJ: ABLEX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matas, L., Arend, R. Α., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity and adaption in the second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, T. (1984). Emerging lives, enduring dispositions: Personality in adulthood. Boston: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meacham, J. A. (1983). Wisdom and the context of knowledge: Knowing that one doesn’t know. In D. Kuhn & J. A. Meacham (Eds.), On the development of developmental psychology (pp. 111–134). Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meacham, J. A. (1990). The loss of wisdom. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 181–211). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montagu, A. (1989). Growing young. Boston: Bergin & Garvey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nietzsche, F. (1968). The portable Nietzsche (W. Kaufmann, Trans.) New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nietzsche, F. (1974). The gay science (W. Kaufmann, Trans.) New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, Β. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. In R. Shweder & R. Le Vine (Eds.), Culture and its acquisition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., & Bayles, K. (1984). Mother-infant intraction and the development of individual differences in children’s cognitive competence. Developmental Psychology, 20, 166–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pandey, R. B. (1969). Hindu samskaras: A sociological study of the Hindu. New Delhi: Motilal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1987). Intuitive parenting: A dialectic counterpart to the infant’s integrative competence. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 669–720). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, T., & Bales, R. F. (1955). Family, socialization and interaction process. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1990). Wisdom: Toward organismic processes. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 244–278). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pearlin, L. I. (1982). Discontinuities in the study of aging. In T. K. Hareven & K. J. Adams (Eds.), Aging and life course perspectives: An interdisciplinary perspective. New York: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. (1996, April). Culture, gender, thinking styles, and intellectual character. Paper presented at the 1996 annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). Autobiography. In E. G. Boring, et al. (Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 4). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1966). Response to Brian Sutton-Smith. Psychological Review, 73, 111–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prigogine, I. (1980). From being to becoming: Time and complexity in the physical sciences. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rathunde, K. (1993). The experience of interest: A theoretical and empirical look at its role in adolescent talent development. In P. Pintrich & M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 8, pp. 59–98), Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rathunde, K. (1995). Wisdom and abiding interest: Interviews with three noted historians in later-life. Journal of Adult Development, 2, 159–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rathunde, K. (1996). Family context and talented adolescents’ optimal experience in productive activities. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 603–626.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rathunde, K. (in press). Support and challenge in the family: An essential combination for adolescents. In C. Bidwell, M. Csikszentmihalyi, L. Hedges, & B. Schneider (Eds.), Images and experience of work in American adolescents.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rathunde, K., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). Undivided interest and the growth of talent: A longitudinal study of adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renninger, Κ. Α., Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (Eds.). (1992). The role of interest in learning and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riegel, Κ. F. (1973). Dialectical operations: The final period of cognitive development. Human Development, 16, 46–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, C. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science: Vol. 3. Formulations of the person and the social context. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, C. (1969). Freedom to learn. Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, Β. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, Β., & Gardner, W. P. (1984). Adult guidance of cognitive development. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, Β., Mistry, J., Goncu, Α., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, (Serial No. 236).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B. (1972). Applications of social learning theory to personality. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, Κ. H., Fein, G. G., & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & Ε. M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 693–773). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (1991). Divergent thinking. Norwood, NJ: ABLEX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rychlak, J. F. (Ed.). (1976). Dialectic: Humanistic rationale for behavior and development. Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C. D. (1989). In the eye of the beholder: Views of psychological well-being among middle-aged and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 4, 195–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: 1. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., Cohen, S. E., & Forsythe, A. B. (1981). The relations of early infant mesures to later development. In S. L. Friedman & M. Sigman (Eds.), Preterm birth and psychological development. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1984). Genius, creativity, and leadership. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sinnott, J. (1984). Postformal reasoning: The relativistic stage. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armons (Eds.), Beyond formal operations (pp. 298–325). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, and antecedents. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development. American Psychologist, 34, 834–841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. M. (1974). Mother and infant at play. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. M. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (1984). Mechanisms of cognitive development. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Wisdom and its relations to intelligence and creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton-Smith, B. (1976). Current research and theory on play, games and sports. In T. Craig (Ed.), The humanistic and mental health aspects of sports, exercise and recreation. Chicago: American Medical Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, Α., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobach, E. (1981). Evolutionary aspects of the activity of the organism and its development. In R. M. Lerner & Ν. Α. Busch-Rossnagel (Eds.), Individuals as producers of their development: A life-span perspective (pp. 37–68). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tobach, Ε., & Schneiria, T. C. (1968). The biopsychology of social behavior of animals. In R. E. Cooke & S. Levin (Eds.), Biologic basis of pediatric practice (pp. 60–82). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bul-lowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginnings of human communication. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1988). Universal co-operative motives: How infants begin to know the language and culture of their parents. In G. Jahoda & I. M. Lewis (Eds.), Acquiring culture: Cross-cultural studies in child development. London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Review, 91, 185–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, V. (1979). Process, performance and pilgrimage. New Delhi: Concept.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaillant, G. (1993). The wisdom of the ego. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg, B. (1981). Play: Dormant issues and new perspectives. Human Development, 24, 357–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldrop, M. (1992). Complexity: The emerging science at the edge of order and chaos. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, M. C. (1971). Them children: A study of language learning. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A. (1988). Self-esteem and optimal experience. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 327–341). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1979). From social interaction to higher psychological functions. Human Development, 22, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1991). A sociocultural approach to mind. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 14–33). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, M. (1995). Complete guide to aging and health. San Francisco: Harmony Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. J., & Middleton, D. (1975). A study of assisted problem-solving. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K. (2014). The Development of the Person: An Experiential Perspective on the Ontogenesis of Psychological Complexity. In: Applications of Flow in Human Development and Education. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9094-9_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics